Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T20:45:13.763Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A trans-diagnostic cognitive behavioural conceptualisation of the positive and negative roles of social media use in adolescents’ mental health and wellbeing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 February 2022

Marc S. Tibber*
Affiliation:
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, UCL, London, UK
Emma Silver
Affiliation:
Tavistock & Portman NHS Foundation Trust, Tavistock Clinic, London, UK
*
*Corresponding author. Email: m.tibber@ucl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Whilst research into the association between social media and mental health is growing, clinical interest in the field has been dominated by a lack of theoretical integration and a focus on pathological patterns of use. Here we present a trans-diagnostic cognitive behavioural conceptualisation of the positive and negative roles of social media use in adolescence, with a focus on how it interacts with common mental health difficulties. Drawing on clinical experience and an integration of relevant theory/literature, the model proposes that particular patterns of social media use be judged as helpful/unhelpful to the extent that they help/hinder the adolescent from satisfying core needs, particularly those relating to acceptance and belonging. Furthermore, it introduces several key interacting processes, including purposeful/habitual modes of engagement, approach/avoidance behaviours, as well as the potential for social media to exacerbate/ameliorate cognitive biases. The purpose of the model is to act as an aide for therapists to collaboratively formulate the role of social media in young people’s lives, with a view to informing treatment, and ultimately, supporting the development of interventions to help young people use social media in the service of their needs and values.

Key learning aims

  1. (1) To gain an understanding of a trans-diagnostic conceptualisation of social media use and its interaction with common mental health difficulties in adolescence.

  2. (2) To gain an understanding of relevant research and theory underpinning the conceptualisation.

  3. (3) To gain an understanding of core processes and dimensions of social media use, and their interaction with common mental health difficulties in this age group, for the purpose of assessment and formulation.

  4. (4) To stimulate ideas about how to include adolescent service users’ online world(s) in treatment (where indicated), both with respect to potential risks to ameliorate and benefits to capitalise upon.

  5. (5) To stimulate and provide a framework for clinically relevant research in the field and the development of interventions to support young people to flourish online.

Type
Empirically Grounded Clinical Guidance Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies

Introduction

Interest in the role of social mediaFootnote 1 (SM) use in young people’s wellbeing and mental health has grown over the last decade, and a number of researchers have attempted to link the increase in mental health difficulties in this age-group with the uptake of SM, or digital screen use more generally (Twenge et al., Reference Twenge, Joiner, Rogers and Martin2018; Twenge and Campbell, Reference Twenge and Campbell2018). In the context of healthy ongoing debate (Orben and Przybylski Reference Orben and Przybylski2020a; Twenge et al., Reference Twenge, Haidt, Joiner and Campbell2020), however, a critical evaluation of the existing evidence base suggests a complex pattern of associations between SM use and wellbeing in children and adolescents, including effects that are relatively small in size, mediated/moderated by other factors, and of unclear direction of causality (Orben et al., Reference Orben, Dienlin and Przybylski2019). In addition, alongside the more commonly considered negative effects of SM, a number of positive effects of SM have also been posited (Uhls et al., Reference Uhls, Ellison and Subrahmanyam2017), with some evidence even suggesting that online opportunities and risks may be closely linked (Hollis et al., Reference Hollis, Livingstone and Sonuga-Barke2020; Livingstone and Helsper, Reference Livingstone and Helsper2010). Consequently, there is an emerging consensus that how an individual engages with SM (and digital technologies more generally) is likely to be more important than how much, i.e. frequency, intensity or duration (Blum-Ross and Livingstone, Reference Blum-Ross and Livingstone2018; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Telzer and Prinstein2020; Orben et al., Reference Orben, Weinstein and Przybylski2020b), and relatedly, that there is a need to identify digital contexts and patterns of online interactions that are differentially linked to positive and negative outcomes (Granic et al., Reference Granic, Morita and Scholten2020).

This more nuanced perspective and evaluation of SM’s relative risks and benefits has been slow to filter into mainstream clinical and educational literature, guidelines or practice. Thus, when it comes to the online world, much of the work within applied psychology has, to date, traditionally focused on extreme and highly problematic patterns of use, i.e. the field has adopted a ‘concern-centric’ approach (Orben et al., Reference Orben, Weinstein and Przybylski2020b), although this is arguably changing (Aboujaoude, Reference Aboujaoude2010; Kuss and Lopez-Fernandez, Reference Kuss and Lopez-Fernandez2016; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Ren, Long, Liu and Liu2019). Furthermore, a number of professional bodies have published guidelines on how to manage the online world of children and adolescents, and these have focused almost exclusively on the putative negative effects of SM use, despite the potential value in harnessing its benefits also (AAP Council on Communications and Media, 2016; Dubicka and Theodosiou, Reference Dubicka and Theodosiou2020; Viner et al., Reference Viner, Davie and Firth2019).

Against the backdrop of this growing interest it has been argued that the area lacks a firm theoretical foundation, despite the essential role that this plays in the development and integration of a field (Granic et al., Reference Granic, Morita and Scholten2020; Orben, 2018; Orben et al., Reference Orben, Weinstein and Przybylski2020b). In actuality, a number of theoretical models of SM use (and digital technology use more generally) do exist; however, these have typically emerged from within the fields of computer-mediated communication and media, developmental and organisational psychology, rather than clinical psychology, and as such, have tended to be non-clinical in nature, often concentrating on more general variables such as (for example) motivations for technology uptake, use or continued use, e.g. the Uses and Gratifications theory (Ruggiero, Reference Ruggiero2000), the Extended Theory of Planned Behaviour (Baker, Reference Baker2010), LaRose and Eastin’s (Reference LaRose and Eastin2004) Social Cognitive Theory of Internet Uses and Gratifications, the Technology Acceptance Model (Marangunić and Granić Reference Marangunić and Granić2015), and the Technology Integration Model (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Ellis and Ziegler2018).

A number of other models have focused on how the online world creates novel channels for communication and the implications of this for the user’s sense of self, their sense of others, as well as the nature of their social interactions, e.g. the Co-construction Model (Subrahmanyam and Šmahel, Reference Subrahmanyam and Šmahel2011e), the Self-Effects Model (Valkenburg, Reference Valkenburg2017), the Differential Susceptibility to Media Effects Model (Valkenburg and Peter, Reference Valkenburg and Peter2013) and the Social Information Processing Theory (Walther, Reference Walther1992; Walther, Reference Walther, Braithwaite and Schrodt2015). Whilst some of these have potential implications for clinical practice, and have informed our thinking as well as much of the literature that we draw upon, such links to clinical practice are typically implicit rather than explicit. Where an explicit clinical perspective has been taken in the construction of a SM use model, this has tended to focus on extreme patterns of use, such that the primary issue relates to problematic technology and/or SM use itself (Caplan, Reference Caplan2005; Davis, Reference Davis2001; Turel and Qahri-Saremi, Reference Turel and Qahri-Saremi2016; Wegmann and Brand, Reference Wegmann and Brand2019).

Thus, we would argue that whilst existing models and theoretical frameworks of SM use are highly informative, with a rich body of research underpinning them, they are of limited utility (as they stand) to the typical mental health clinician working in a general community mental health setting who wants practical guidance on how to harness the benefits and ameliorate the harms of SM use. For example, they do not lend themselves readily to the identification of targets for intervention, and because of their complexity and limitations in scope, are not suitable for sharing with service users themselves. Thus, we would argue that for a model to be of maximum clinical utility it should be intelligible and useful to the therapist and the client alike, bridging the gap between theory and practice, model and formulation, facilitating a shared understanding of the individual’s presenting difficulties in an empowering way that opens up opportunities for behavioural change (Division of Clinical Psychology, 2011). [See also Ngai et al. (Reference Ngai, Tao and Moon2015) for a review on theories, constructs and conceptual frameworks that have been drawn upon (more broadly) within the SM literature, and McFarland and Ployhart (Reference McFarland and Ployhart2015), Meier and Reinecke (Reference Meier and Reinecke2020) and Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Holden and Ariati2021) for useful frameworks to conceptualise and systematise existing research.]

In response to this gap in the literature, this paper describes a trans-diagnostic cognitive behavioural conceptualisation of the positive and negative effects of SM use on mental health and wellbeing in adolescents, with a focus on social/inter-personal processes and their interaction with common mental health difficulties, such as anxiety and depression. Drawing on our combined clinical experience of working with young people, an integration of the extant evidence base and existing theoretical frameworks of SM use and common mental health difficulties, its aim is not to pathologise everyday patterns of SM use, but instead, to achieve the following: (i) to act as a model for clinicians to integrate and make sense of relevant research from a clinical perspective, (ii) to act as an aide for clinicians to collaboratively formulate (and share) with young people the role of SM in their mental health difficulties, both in terms of risks to negotiate as well as benefits to harness, in order to (iii) direct treatment (where indicated), and (iv) in the long-term, inform the development of strategies and/or interventions to help individuals shape their online lives in a healthy way that is in line with their needs, goals and values. Consistent with these aims we recognise that the conceptualisation is an early working model to be updated iteratively as key components are either supported or refuted through empirical testing.

The paper begins with a brief overview of social processes and SM use in adolescence in order to locate and contextualise the focus of the conceptualisation, before exploring the existing literature on the links between SM use and mental health. It then presents an overview of the main theoretical foundations from which the conceptualisation explicitly draws, before presenting a précis of the conceptualisation, as well as a more in-depth consideration of each of its core components. Finally, we close with a discussion of the implications of the model for clinical practice.

Social processes and social media use in adolescence

Adolescence, defined here as the period between 10 and 24 years of age, is a broad window of development bridging childhood and adulthood, which is characterised by profound biological, psychological and social change (Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2018; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Blum and Giedd2009). It is a period of great opportunity and promise, but also vulnerability, with approximately three-quarters of all lifetime psychological disorders emerging by the end of this stage (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Amminger, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Lee and Ustün2007).

One of the primary challenges for adolescence, many have posited, is identity formation, a profoundly social process by which the young person must typically individuate from a family unit and establish a coherent identity that is embedded within a network of peer connections (Erikson, Reference Erikson1968; Granic et al., Reference Granic, Morita and Scholten2020). Whilst there is great inter-individual variation in trajectories between adolescents, during this period the individual commonly faces a multitude of external challenges that must be negotiated, from leaving the family home and learning to live independently, to earning a wage and establishing peer friendships as well as sexual and romantic partners (Sawyer et al., Reference Sawyer, Azzopardi, Wickremarathne and Patton2018). In parallel, the adolescent is also confronted with a myriad of internal/biological changes, from the development of secondary sexual characteristics under hormonal control, to a protracted process of brain maturation (Andrews et al., Reference Andrews, Ahmed and Blakemore2020).

Critically, some of the last areas of the brain to mature during development are located within the ‘social brain’, i.e. networks thought to underpin social cognitive processes such as perspective taking, emotional regulation and the management of peer influence (Andrews et al., Reference Andrews, Ahmed and Blakemore2020). Psychologically, this is reflected in a period of heightened social sensitivity, during which peer influence and vulnerability to the negative effects of social isolation and peer rejection are elevated (Orben et al., Reference Orben, Tomova and Blakemore2020a; Tomova et al., Reference Tomova, Andrews and Blakemore2021), with social interactions playing a crucial role in the development of identity (Granic et al., Reference Granic, Morita and Scholten2020; Ragelienė, Reference Ragelienė2016). Indeed, a wealth of research has linked the quality and nature of peer interactions in adolescence with a range of psychological, educational, physical health and behavioural outcomes (Almquist, Reference Almquist2009; Almquist and Östberg, Reference Almquist and Östberg2013; Menting et al., Reference Menting, Van Lier, Koot, Pardini and Loeber2015; Modin et al., Reference Modin, Östberg and Almquist2011).

Risk-taking, novelty-seeking, impulsivity, exploration and experimentation are also elevated during adolescence relative to adulthood, with social factors thought to play a crucial role (Tomova et al., Reference Tomova, Andrews and Blakemore2021). For example, adolescents are more likely than adults to take risks when in the presence of peers (Gardner and Steinberg, Reference Gardner and Steinberg2005; Shepherd et al., Reference Shepherd, Lane, Tapscott and Gentile2011), and this effect is more pronounced when the peers report a preference for risk-taking behaviour (Bingham et al., Reference Bingham, Simons-Morton, Pradhan, Li, Almani, Falk, Shope, Buckley, Ouimet and Albert2016). Two distinct but interacting psychological processes have been hypothesised as important to an understanding of adolescent risk-taking: impulsivity and sensation-seeking. Thus, according to the Life-span Wisdom Model (Romer, Reference Romer2010; Romer et al., Reference Romer, Reyna and Satterthwaite2017), there is a gradual reduction in risk-taking and impulsivity between childhood and adulthood, which is underpinned by the development of executive function skills and a maturation of the prefrontal cortex (Green et al., Reference Green, Fry and Myerson1994). This protracted development of self-regulatory skills is thought to underpin some of the more maladaptive impulsive behaviours commonly seen in childhood and early adolescence. In parallel, there is an increase in sensation-seeking that follows an inverted U-shape function and peaks around adolescence. This coincides with increased activity in limbic and prefrontal dopaminergic networks associated with reward-sensitivity (Chambers et al., Reference Chambers, Taylor and Potenza2003; Romer and Hennessy, Reference Romer and Hennessy2007). In contrast to the aforementioned process, whilst conferring additional risk, this is thought to be partially adaptive, driving an exploration of the environment that is critical for learning (Romer, Reference Romer2010; Romer et al., Reference Romer, Reyna and Satterthwaite2017) and in some contexts may be optimal (Lloyd et al., Reference Lloyd, McKay, Sebastian and Balsters2020). Thus, risk-taking and exploration/learning may be two sides of the same coin.

Against this backdrop of heightened risk-taking and peer-influence, it is not surprising that a number of authors have hypothesised the potentially transformative impact of SM (and the internet more generally) on young people’s social interactions, and by inference, identity formation (Granic et al., Reference Granic, Morita and Scholten2020; Subrahmanyam and Šmahel Reference Subrahmanyam and Šmahel2011b) and social and emotional wellbeing; see Spies Shapiro and Margolin (Reference Spies Shapiro and Margolin2014) for a review, and Crone and Konijn (Reference Crone and Konijn2018) also. Thus, the latest (pre-COVID) worldwide census data (from January to March 2010) suggest that Generation Z (aged 16 to 23 years at present) are the heaviest SM users of all, currently using SM for an average of 2.7 hours each day across an average of 8.5 SM accounts (GlobalWebIndex, 2020). Furthermore, adolescents with a diagnosed ‘mental disorder’ are more likely to use SM every day and for longer than those without a diagnosis (NHS Digital, 2017). Whilst Amy Orben has written articulately about the repeating cycle of panic that follows the emergence of any new technology (Orben, 2020b), SM undoubtedly offers novel opportunities for social experimentation, exploration and connection, but also, fresh challenges and risks, which at the very least, argue for close attention to be given to the online lives of young people presenting to mental health services.

Drawing on a contextual approach that recognises the complex interaction that occurs between the individual, their pattern of SM use and the nature of the technology with which they are engaging (as well as the broader social context in which this occurs) (Vanden Abeele, Reference Vanden Abeele2020; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Telzer and Prinstein2020), the cognitive behavioural conceptualisation we present does not assume radically different processes during adolescence (relative to other life stages), but instead, acknowledges that key social processes may be particularly pertinent and play a more central role for this age group. Thus, issues of acceptance and belonging and the balance between individual and group identity affect us all, irrespective of life stage. Likewise, the online world opens up novel ways of interacting with others for the adolescent and adult alike, with all the potential risks and benefits that these bring. Whilst a comprehensive review of research into such risks and benefits lies beyond the remit of this paper, we provide a brief overview of some of the key concepts and processes implicated below. Where available this draws on studies undertaken with adolescents; however, in view of the relative paucity of such research we also draw from a wider body of literature that includes studies undertaken with adults.

Social media use and mental health

Interpretation of existing research into the association between SM use and mental health is complicated by the fact that extant studies have explored an array of SM constructs (from self-reported time spent on any SM site, to defined behaviours undertaken on specific platforms) and mental health constructs (from specific disorders, to general risk factors for psychopathology), which are likely to exhibit distinct patterns of association. In response, Meier and Reinecke (Reference Meier and Reinecke2020) developed two organising frameworks to ‘systematize conceptual and operational approaches’ (p. 1) to computer-mediated communication (CMC) (including SM use) and mental health research.

The first framework, which focused on CMC, defined four ‘channel-centred’ levels of analysis, which included the device (e.g. smartphone), type of application (e.g. SM), application brand (e.g. Facebook), and application/brand feature (e.g. private messaging), in addition to two ‘communication-centred’ levels of analysis, which included the nature of the interaction (e.g. active vs passive engagement) as well as the message/communication itself. The second framework, which focused on mental health, distinguished between psychopathology (which they broke down further into internalising and externalising psychopathology, e.g. anxiety and aggression, respectively), and psychological wellbeing.

With respect to the existing evidence base into the association between SM use and mental health/wellbeing, a great deal has focused on the level of application or application brand (channel-centred research), e.g. time spent on SM, frequency of use, or intensity of use (Meier and Reinecke, Reference Meier and Reinecke2020). Within this research, however, the majority of studies have drawn on single-platform data, such as studies of Facebook use or cross-platform data that do not differentiate between platforms, thereby precluding identification of differential effects based on platform brand, let alone brand features (Schønning et al., Reference Schønning, Hjetland, Aarø and Skogen2020).

Systematic reviews and meta-analyses of overall levels of SM use (i.e. application level analyses) are relatively consistent, however, with evidence for a weak association between higher usage and poorer mental health (Abi-Jaoude et al., Reference Abi-Jaoude, Treurnicht Naylor and Pignatiello2020; Orben, Reference Orben2020a), including symptoms of depression, anxiety and general distress (Abi-Jaoude et al., Reference Abi-Jaoude, Treurnicht Naylor and Pignatiello2020; Keles et al., Reference Keles, McCrae and Grealish2019; McCrae et al., Reference McCrae, Gettings and Purssell2017; Orben, Reference Orben2020a). In a meta-review of meta-analyses within the field, Meier and Reinecke (Reference Meier and Reinecke2020) found that individuals who used social network sites more intensely reported more internalising psychopathology, with an effect size in the range of r ≈ 0.05–0.2. Evidence therein, however, did not support associations between overall levels of social network site use and wellbeing/life satisfaction (as opposed to psychopathology), and no meta-analyses were found to have explored SM’s link to externalising psychopathology. See Valkenburg, Meier, and Beyens (n.d.) also.

With respect to directions of causality, although relatively rare (Orben, Reference Orben2020a), where longitudinal or experimental methods have been employed, the evidence is mixed, suggesting possible bidirectional/reciprocal effects; thus, whilst high levels of SM use may impact negatively on mental health, poorer mental health may also drive increased SM use (Aalbers et al., Reference Aalbers, McNally, Heeren, de Wit and Fried2019; Frison and Eggermont, Reference Frison and Eggermont2017; Hunt et al., Reference Hunt, Marx, Lipson and Young2018; Mosquera et al., Reference Mosquera, Odunowo, McNamara, Guo and Petrie2020; Orben and Przybylski, Reference Orben and Przybylski2019).

Putative mechanisms underlying the proposed harms of SM use

A number of mechanisms underlying associations between SM use and poorer mental health have been hypothesised. One common idea, known as the social displacement hypothesis, proposes that online activity competes for time that would otherwise be spent engaged in what is presumed to be healthier or more productive activities, such as sleeping, studying or socialising, the latter with potential implications for social development. However, evidence for the social displacement hypothesis is weak (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Kearney and Xing2019b; Valkenburg and Peter Reference Valkenburg and Peter2007), and it is likely that for some, SM actually facilitates social interactions, both online and offline (Valkenburg and Peter Reference Valkenburg and Peter2007); more on this below. Further, one study concluded that SM in fact displaces neutral or unpleasant activities rather than pleasurable or rewarding ones (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Johnson and Ross2019a).

In contrast, evidence for sleep disruption and/or displacement of sleep amongst heavy SM users (Scott et al., Reference Scott, Biello and Woods2019), and heavy digital screen users more generally, is relatively robust (Orben and Przybylski, Reference Orben and Przybylski2020b), although reported effect sizes are small (Orben and Przybylski, Reference Orben and Przybylski2020b). This is of potential interest here as sleep disturbance has been linked to a number of social difficulties (Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Mendes and Prather2017), as well as a wide range of mental health disorders during adolescence (Tarokh et al., Reference Tarokh, Saletin and Carskadon2016). However, once again, experimental evidence indicates a potential bidirectional causal relationship (Bartel et al., Reference Bartel, Scheeren and Gradisar2019; Exelmans and Van den Bulck, Reference Exelmans and Van den Bulck2016; Tavernier and Willoughby, Reference Tavernier and Willoughby2014).

Perhaps the most commonly explored ‘communication-centred’ factors linked to negative outcomes are online social comparisons, and potentially relatedly, passive use. Thus, social comparisons theory (Festinger, Reference Festinger1954) proposes that our sense of self is derived, in part, from how we judge ourselves to be doing in comparison with others, and high levels of online upward social comparisons have been linked to poor self-esteem and symptoms of anxiety and depression (Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Zilanawala, Booker and Sacker2018; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhou, Yang, Kong, Niu and Fan2017b; Schmuck et al., Reference Schmuck, Karsay, Matthes and Stevic2019; Tibber et al., Reference Tibber, Zhao and Butler2020; Vogel et al., Reference Vogel, Rose, Roberts and Eckles2014; Vogel et al., Reference Vogel, Rose, Okdie, Eckles and Franz2015; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Wang, Gaskin and Hawk2017), potentially in a causal manner (Vogel et al., Reference Vogel, Rose, Roberts and Eckles2014). Furthermore, in a meta-review by Meier and Reinecke (Reference Meier and Reinecke2020), the authors reported a small association between higher social comparisons on social network sites and symptoms of depression (r = 0.23; 95% CIs = 0.12, 0.34), which was greater for upward social comparisons (r = 0.33; 95% CIs = 0.20, 0.47), i.e. comparisons made with those perceived as better off than oneself.

With respect to passive SM use (e.g. scrolling or browsing), it has been proposed that this is linked to more negative outcomes than active use (e.g. self-disclosure and online exchanges with others), with the association potentially being causal (Frison and Eggermont, Reference Frison and Eggermont2016; Frison and Eggermont, Reference Frison and Eggermont2017; Kim and Lee Reference Kim and Lee2011; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Timpano, Tran and Joormann2015; Verduyn et al., Reference Verduyn, Lee, Park, Shablack, Orvell, Bayer, Ybarra, Jonides and Kross2015; Verduyn et al., Reference Verduyn, Ybarra, Résibois, Jonides and Kross2017; Wang Reference Wang2013; Wenninger et al., Reference Wenninger, Krasnova and Buxmann2014). In fact, the negative effects of passive use and social comparisons may be linked, as passive use is itself associated with higher levels of social comparisons and associated feelings of envy (Appel et al., Reference Appel, Gerlach and Crusius2016; Krasnova et al., Reference Krasnova, Widjaja, Buxmann, Wenninger and Benbasat2015; Tandoc et al., Reference Tandoc, Ferrucci and Duffy2015; Verduyn et al., Reference Verduyn, Lee, Park, Shablack, Orvell, Bayer, Ybarra, Jonides and Kross2015; Verduyn et al., Reference Verduyn, Ybarra, Résibois, Jonides and Kross2017).

Consistent with the validity of an active/passive distinction, in their meta-review Meier and Reinecke (Reference Meier and Reinecke2020) found that whilst active ‘interactions’ on social network sites (e.g. replying, commenting and liking) were related to positive wellbeing (r = 0.14, 95% CIs = 0.08, 0.2), more passive ‘content consumption’ (e.g. browsing, searching, monitoring) was linked to negative wellbeing (r = –0.14, 95% CIs = –0.2, –0.8). However, the authors reported that the evidence for associations between interaction level factors (including active vs passive use) and psychopathology (rather than wellbeing) was ‘scarce and inconsistent’ (p. 19). Relatedly, a recent paper reviewing the literature on active/passive use failed to find evidence for its association with wellbeing or mental health, leading the authors to conclude that such a distinction may be too coarse (Valkenburg, Driel and Beyens, n.d.). Other authors have similarly critiqued a simple active/passive dichotomy (Kross et al., Reference Kross, Verduyn, Sheppes, Costello, Jonides and Ybarra2021; Meier et al., Reference Meier, Gilbert, Börner and Possler2020), suggesting that precisely what is being actively/passively engaged with in such interactions may be crucial (Valkenburg, Driel and Beyens, n.d.).

Speaking to this, in their overview of the literature, Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Holden and Ariati2021) proposed that passive viewing of one’s own profile may actually increase self-esteem, most probably because of the heavily curated nature of online profiles (including one’s own), which means that the self is portrayed in a positive light (Gonzales and Hancock, Reference Gonzales and Hancock2011). In addition, they noted that even where passive use does lead to social comparisons, the nature of these social comparisons may be critical. Thus Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Holden and Ariati2021) suggested that whilst judgemental comparisons, e.g. comparing one’s attractiveness or wealth to others’, are typically associated with negative outcomes, non-judgemental comparisons, e.g. of opinions and perspectives for informational purposes, may actually lead to mental health benefits (Park and Baek, Reference Park and Baek2018). Thus, passive use may be problematic specifically in instances where it leads to judgemental social comparisons.

Another possible mechanism of interest driving the association between SM use and poor mental health, which has received relatively little research attention to date, involves the potential for the nature of online communication to amplify or exacerbate maladaptive cognitions and/or facilitate problematic behaviours associated with pre-existing mental health difficulties and/or specific personality structures (Chohan and D’Souza, Reference Chohan and D’Souza2020). For example, online symptom-checking may fuel health anxiety (Doherty-Torstrick et al., Reference Doherty-Torstrick, Walton and Fallon2016; McMullan et al., Reference McMullan, Berle, Arnáez and Starcevic2019), links have been made between SM use (particularly appearance-related interactions and upward comparisons) and body dissatisfaction/body image disturbance (Hogue and Mills, Reference Hogue and Mills2019; Holland and Tiggemann, Reference Holland and Tiggemann2016; Meier and Reinecke, Reference Meier and Reinecke2020; Sidani et al., Reference Sidani, Shensa, Hoffman, Hanmer and Primack2016), and exposure to online self-harm material may increase suicidality (Arendt et al., Reference Arendt, Scherr and Romer2019; Memon et al., Reference Memon, Sharma, Mohite and Jain2018).

Finally, SM users may also be exposed to a number of explicit online risks, including those of a criminal nature (El Asam and Katz Reference El Asam and Katz2018), such as fraud, identity-theft, abuse and harassment, grooming, exploitation, radicalisation as well as exposure to age-inappropriate material, reputational damage, and cyber-bullying (Baccarella et al., Reference Baccarella, Wagner, Kietzmann and McCarthy2018; Department for Education, 2019; Sheldon et al., Reference Sheldon, Rauschnabel and Honeycutt2020; Subrahmanyam and Šmahel, Reference Subrahmanyam and Šmahel2011d). However, whilst the online world has broadened opportunities for such exploitation and abuse, the relative risk and frequency of such risks on- and offline should be considered. For example, a meta-analysis found that traditional (i.e. offline) bullying was twice as common as online bullying (Modecki et al., Reference Modecki, Minchin, Harbaugh, Guerra and Runions2014).

Putative mechanisms underlying the proposed benefits of SM use

Whilst the benefits of SM use have typically received much less attention to date, in research as well as the popular media, a number of health benefits have been documented. Inverting some of the proposed harms of SM use, many of these have focused on SM’s potential to facilitate social developmental processes, e.g. through opportunities for identity experimentation, self-expression and social connection, etc., rather than displacement of such opportunities (Subrahmanyam and Šmahel, Reference Subrahmanyam and Šmahel2011a; Subrahmanyam and Šmahel, Reference Subrahmanyam and Šmahel2011c).

Much research has, in particular, focused on the potential for SM use (and the use of digital technologies more broadly) to lead to an accumulation of social resources or cultivation of a sense of connectedness. For example, with respect to ‘channel-centred’ studies focusing on analyses at the level of device and application, Meier and Reinecke (Reference Meier and Reinecke2020) showed in their meta-review that individuals who used SM more intensely reported higher levels of social capital and social support. Relatedly, in a meta-analysis, Liu et al. (Reference Liu, Ainsworth and Baumeister2016) reported a positive association between general social network site use and social capital (r = 0.32, 95% CIs = 0.27, 0.37). Despite this, surprisingly few studies have, to date, extended these findings to directly test a mediating role for such SM-driven accumulation of social capital in mental health and wellbeing (Lomanowska and Guitton, Reference Lomanowska and Guitton2016). There are exceptions, however; for example, in a study of 300 Korean adults, increases in social connectedness were found to mediate the effects of SM use on subjective wellbeing (Ahn and Shin, Reference Ahn and Shin2013).

Despite the relative robustness of findings as to the potential for SM use to result in the accumulation of social capital/social support, for a number of reasons the association is unlikely to be straightforward. First, it is likely underpinned by multiple (potentially interacting) pathways, with the potential for bi-directional causality. For example, whilst feelings of disconnection may drive greater SM use, engagement with SM sites may itself cultivate feelings of connection, such that seemingly contradictory findings may emerge, even within the same dataset (Sheldon et al., Reference Sheldon, Abad and Hinsch2011).

Second, individual-level factors, i.e. attributes of the user, may moderate such associations. For example, whilst the social enhancement (or ‘Rich Get Richer’) theory proposes that individuals who are popular and/or socially resourceful offline build their popularity and social connections further through online interactions, the social compensation (or ‘Poor Get Richer’) theory suggests that individuals who are less popular and/or less socially resourceful offline compensate for this through their online interactions (Zywica and Danowski, Reference Zywica and Danowski2008). The social compensation theory may be particularly relevant to the field of mental health. Thus, there is evidence to suggest that individuals who struggle socially and/or with their mental health, including those who are low in self-esteem (Ellison et al., Reference Ellison, Steinfield and Lampe2007), socially anxious (Indian and Grieve, Reference Indian and Grieve2014), socially isolated or excluded (Andrade and Doolin, Reference Andrade and Doolin2016; Khosravi et al., Reference Khosravi, Rezvani and Wiewiora2016), lacking in social/familial support (Keresteš and Štulhofer, Reference Keresteš and Štulhofer2020), or else struggle with social communication (Mazurek, Reference Mazurek2013), may stand to benefit the most from online engagement in terms of improved wellbeing and/or the accumulation of social capital. For these individuals, SM may provide opportunities for connection and support, self-disclosure and identity experimentation that would otherwise be unattainable or else feel unsafe/unmanageable in the offline world (Bonetti et al., Reference Bonetti, Campbell and Gilmore2010).

Third, social capital is a broad, multifaceted construct; for example, as a minimum Putnam (Reference Putnam2001) and others have distinguished between bridging social capital (i.e. ‘weak ties’ between distantly connected people) and bonding social capital (i.e. ‘strong ties’ between close family or friends), and these may differ in their strength and/or patterns of association with SM use (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Ainsworth and Baumeister2016). For example, Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Holden and Ariati2021) mapped a complex pattern of associations between SM use and wellbeing that was dependent on whether engagement was with ‘strong ties’ or ‘weak ties’, and whether associated engagement was active in nature, or passive. Furthermore, they distinguished between two forms of active SM engagement: (i) active interaction/directive communication, in which the user directly interacts or communicates with others, and (ii) active broadcasting, which they define as ‘actively producing or sharing texts, photos, and videos to an unspecified audience’ (p. 5). Reviewing the evidence, Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Holden and Ariati2021) concluded that benefits to mental health are most likely when individuals use SM specifically in order to engage with close associates in an active/interactive manner (Seo et al., Reference Seo, Kim and Yang2016), a pattern that they suggested is likely to be mediated by increased social support. Thus, being active online may not suffice; instead, active and purposeful interaction may be optimal for benefits to be accrued.

Examples of active/interactive patterns of engagement that have been linked to social capital and/or wellbeing include the use of SM to meet new people or stay connected with offline contacts (Ellison et al., Reference Ellison, Steinfield and Lampe2007), self-disclosure (Gonen and Aharony, Reference Gonen and Aharony2017; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Ainsworth and Baumeister2016) and active replying to others (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Ainsworth and Baumeister2016). For example, one study had participants increase their posting behaviour on Facebook over the course of a week; relative to a control group that received no instructions, at the end of the study the experimental group reported reductions in loneliness that were driven by a greater sense of connection with friends (große Deters and Mehl, Reference große Deters and Mehl2013). Furthermore, increased perceived online support has been found to mediate effects of active Facebook use on (less) depressed mood and lower levels of loneliness (Frison and Eggermont, Reference Frison and Eggermont2016; Seo et al., Reference Seo, Kim and Yang2016). One possibility is that such active social engagement facilitates the cultivation of intimacy and building of connectedness (Lomanowska and Guitton, Reference Lomanowska and Guitton2016).

Finally, as a counter to SM’s potential to increase exposure to tangible risks and harms, SM use may also provide opportunities for access to concrete rewards and benefits, including (amongst other things) opportunities for learning (Bruguera et al., Reference Bruguera, Guitert and Romeu2019), financial revenue and career opportunities (Tang et al., Reference Tang, Gu and Whinston2012), access to news (Nielsen and Schrøder, Reference Nielsen and Schrøder2014) and entertainment, peer support and specialist knowledge (Naslund et al., Reference Naslund, Aschbrenner, Marsch and Bartels2016). Relatedly, SM may also play a supportive or curative role for some with pre-existing mental health difficulties, e.g. through mental health initiatives, online support and provision of specialist information (Luxton et al., Reference Luxton, June and Fairall2012; Moorhead et al., Reference Moorhead, Hazlett, Harrison, Carroll, Irwin and Hoving2013; Naslund et al., Reference Naslund, Aschbrenner, Marsch and Bartels2016; Subrahmanyam and Šmahel, Reference Subrahmanyam and Šmahel2011c).

Theoretical underpinnings

The primary theoretical foundations of the conceptualisation are drawn from cognitive behavioural approaches such as cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) and cognitive behavioural theory (Beck, Reference Beck1976). Thus, at the core of the conceptualisation lies a cross-sectional CBT formulation (McDonough et al., Reference McDonough, Duncan, Williams and House1997), on to which additional components are ‘bolted’ or integrated. This decision was driven by our primary aim of making the conceptualisation of maximum practical utility to clinicians working in mental health settings for young people. Thus, CBT has a strong evidence base supporting it, and has demonstrated great flexibility in its application to a wide range of mental health difficulties (Fordham et al., Reference Fordham, Sugavanam, Edwards, Stallard, Howard, Nair, Copsey, Lee, Howick, Hemming and Lamb2021), such that the approach has come to dominate mental health services, including in young people’s services in the UK, as well as further afield. It is our hope that insights and approaches to emerge from the conceptualisation can be integrated easily into existing practices by clinicians currently working face-to-face with young people.

In addition, however, because of our shift away from a causationist approach, which assumes SM to be inherently harmful or beneficial (Orben, 2020b) towards a more contextualist approach, which recognises the crucial significance of interactions between the technology, the individual and their behaviour, and further, emphasises the importance of the function or consequences of use (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Telzer and Prinstein2020), we also drew upon third-wave cognitive behavioural approaches, particularly acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) (Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Strosahl and Wilson1999), which shares such a functional contextualist perspective (Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette and Strosahl2016a). This was particularly helpful in guiding our understanding of, and focus on, the function of online behaviours (including approach and avoidance behaviours) over their topography (or form), as well as our integration of the notion of mindful engagement, which is very much in keeping with recent trends to incorporate mindfulness-based concepts and practices within mainstream cognitive behavioural therapies (Baer, Reference Baer, Hayes and Hofmann2018).

In addition to drawing on these foundational approaches to understand core psychological processes, we turned to relevant theoretical literature to inform our understanding of how the individual and the technology interact, as these are largely unexplored within the CBT tradition. Whilst we drew on a large range of theory, we will describe three theories that heavily underpin our conceptualisation, and in particular, shaped our understanding of the three core aspects of the human–technology interaction as we see them: (i) what drives an individual’s engagement with the technology (uses and gratifications theory) (Kircaburun et al., Reference Kircaburun, Alhabash, Tosuntaş and Griffiths2020), (ii) how features of the technology invite or afford different patterns of engagement (the transformation framework) (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018b) [see Karahanna et al. (Reference Karahanna, Xu, Xu and Zhang2018) also], as well as (iii) the consequences of this interaction for mental health (the interpersonal connections behaviour framework, and an extension thereof) (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Algoe and Green2018).

In selecting particular theories/frameworks to inform our conceptualisation of the human–technology interaction we drew on our understanding and synthesis of relevant literature, as well as our combined personal experience of theories, hypotheses and frameworks that have proved to be of use in understanding SM use (and the clinical implications thereof) in the young people that we work with. Thus, our decisions as to what to include or exclude were driven primarily by our over-arching aim of making the conceptualisation of maximum practical and clinical utility. Consequently, where possible we also prioritised theory that: (i) took a developmental perspective, focusing specifically on young people’s engagement, or else was highly applicable to this age group, and (ii) was of broad clinical relevance to understanding everyday patterns of use, in contrast, for example, to more specialist theories that have focused solely on pathological patterns of use.

Whilst we do not provide an introduction to CBT or third-wave cognitive behavioural approaches here, as these approaches are likely to be familiar to the common mental health practitioner as well as readers of this journal [see Beck (Reference Beck1995) and Hayes et al. (Reference Hayes, Strosahl and Wilson1999), however], we briefly introduce below the main frameworks that informed our understanding of human–computer interactions within the conceptualisation.

Uses and Gratifications theory

The Uses and Gratification theory (U&G) seeks to explain how and why people engage with media (Katz et al., Reference Katz, Blumler and Gurevitch1973), with a greater focus on the human side of the human–technology interaction. Aside from being well-established, with a substantial and wide research base to support it (Lev-On, Reference Lev-On2017), U&G is inherently empowering and of clinical utility, as it posits that users are (primarily) active agents (rather than passive recipients) of information that are motivated to seek out specific media in goal-directed ways in order to satisfy (i.e. gratify) needs and desires. [See LaRose and Eastin (Reference LaRose and Eastin2004), however, for details of how SM habits may also be integrated into a U&G-based model of technology use.]

U&G also proposes that such motivations will vary as a function of inter-individual differences in a range of social and psychological factors (Kircaburun et al., Reference Kircaburun, Alhabash, Tosuntaş and Griffiths2020), making it well suited to an idiographic – i.e. case formulation – approach that highlights personal agency and focuses on the workability of use for the individual. For example, findings from one study of college students suggested that individuals who are more anxious in face-to-face interactions and find offline socialising less rewarding may turn to SM as a ‘compensatory’ strategy, i.e. to connect with others and satisfy social needs (Papacharissi and Rubin, Reference Papacharissi and Rubin2000). In contrast, individuals who are less anxious in offline (face-to-face) interactions and find these more rewarding may be drawn to SM more for non-social purposes, e.g. for entertainment and information-seeking.

Critically, U&G also distinguishes between gratifications sought, meaning the intended purpose(s) for which the media is used (e.g. to connect with others), which we will use interchangeably with the term ‘motivations’, and gratifications obtained, meaning the actual consequences of use (e.g. increased feelings of loneliness and disconnection) (Palmgreen, Reference Palmgreen1984), which we will use interchangeably with the term ‘consequences’. For this reason, the U&G is also well suited to a functional contextual approach that emphasises the consequences of use in determining helpful versus unhelpful patterns of use (see ‘The interpersonal–connections–behaviour framework’ and ‘Types of behaviours and their maintaining role’ sections below). For example, research has shown that self-reported satisfaction with social network site use is high when gratifications obtained exceed gratifications sought, i.e. when SM use effectively satisfies the individual’s needs (Bae, Reference Bae2018).

Although not specific to adolescents or social media, U&G has been used to conceptualise young people’s SM use in a number of studies. For example, Cheung et al. (Reference Cheung, Chiu and Lee2011) showed that amongst adolescents/emerging adults, the primary motivations that predict an intention to use Facebook are social in nature, including a desire for social connectivity, social enhancement and social presence, suggesting that young people turn to SM, predominantly in order to gratify social needs. Seen through a developmental lens, such social motivations for SM use may be particularly pertinent for young people (Ciranka and van den Bos, Reference Ciranka and van den Bos2019) because they map onto key pre-occupations and developmental challenges faced, e.g. those relating to peer acceptance, social connectedness, romantic and sexual exploration, and social identity formation (Ciranka and van den Bos, Reference Ciranka and van den Bos2019; Sebastian et al., Reference Sebastian, Viding, Williams and Blakemore2010; Spies Shapiro and Margolin, Reference Spies Shapiro and Margolin2014). Furthermore, the SM environment may afford opportunities for agency and experimentation in relation to such development needs and desires (Šmahel and Subrahmanyam, 2011e) that are more difficult to access in the offline world because of constraints imposed by parents, college and other systemic factors, as well as perceived barriers relating to the self (Valkenburg and Peter, Reference Valkenburg and Peter2008). However, as noted, the SM environment also brings a number of risks and challenges, such that gratifications sought and gratifications obtained need not be perfectly aligned (Bae, Reference Bae2018).

With respect to associations between motivations and mental health outcomes, existing research in the area is scant (particularly in young people), and has traditionally relied on cross-sectional data that preclude causal inferences (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Holden and Ariati2021). Nonetheless, there is some evidence to suggest that interpersonal/relational motivations for SM use, such as maintaining relationships and a sense of connectedness, may be linked to benefits in terms of reduced psychopathology, including lower levels of depression (Wright et al., Reference Wright, Rosenberg, Egbert, Ploeger, Bernard and King2013) and anxiety (Reichelt, Reference Reichelt2019), as well as improved wellbeing, including higher levels of social support satisfaction (Wright et al., Reference Wright, Rosenberg, Egbert, Ploeger, Bernard and King2013), lower levels of loneliness (Teppers et al., Reference Teppers, Luyckx, Klimstra and Goossens2014), positive social adjustment (Yang and Brown, Reference Yang and Brown2013) and life satisfaction (Adnan and Mavi, Reference Adnan and Mavi2015). It is likely that this is because social benefits are more likely to be accrued when SM use is purposefully engaged with for this purpose.

Such interpersonal/relational motivations may be contrasted to non-social (or even socially avoidant) motivations, several of which have been linked to poorer outcomes. For example, in a single population sample of adults, Perugini and Solano (Reference Perugini and Solano2021) showed that whist the use of SM to maintain relationships and seek information was associated with better well-being, its use to pass the time or for exhibitionism (arguably linked to social comparisons through a heightened awareness of relative status) was associated with poorer wellbeing. Relatedly, Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Holden and Ariati2021) distinguished between two classes of SM motivations: (i) enhancement motivations, such as for maintaining existing relationships or seeking entertainment, which function to enhance positive or neutral states, and (ii) compensation motivations, such as meeting new people online, which (may) function to avoid negative affective states and compensate for perceived deficits. Whilst Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Holden and Ariati2021) proposed that such enhancement and compensation motivations may be associated with positive and negative outcomes, respectively, they noted that evidence for this is weak, and further, may reflect socially anxious individuals being driven by compensation motivations, rather than the opposite direction of causality.

The transformation framework

The transformation framework was developed as a comprehensive framework to describe the implications of the SM environment for the establishment and maintenance of peer relationships in adolescence, integrating findings from across multiple disciplines within a clinical and developmental perspective (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018b). Whilst a comprehensive review of the literature underpinning the transformation framework lies beyond the remits of this paper, it is important to acknowledge – as the authors do – that the framework draws heavily upon (and synthesises) large bodies of research from within the fields of computer-mediated communication, media psychology, developmental psychology and organisational psychology, some of which we have signposted the reader to in the introduction; see Subrahmanyam and Šmahel (Reference Subrahmanyam and Šmahel2011e), Valkenburg (Reference Valkenburg2017) and Walther (Reference Walther1992, Reference Walther, Braithwaite and Schrodt2015) for example. The transformation framework builds on these ideas, however, by relating these processes to the specific challenges of adolescent development and peer relationships, in both dyadic (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a) and group (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018b) contexts [see Karahanna et al., (Reference Karahanna, Xu, Xu and Zhang2018) also].

According to the transformation framework the SM environment represents a unique and novel social context, distinct from face-to-face interactions, with a number of consequences for the nature of cognitive, behavioural and interpersonal processes that it affords (Moreno and Uhls, Reference Moreno and Uhls2019; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018b). Thus, whilst online and offline worlds interact, the online world does not merely reflect offline processes, but has the potential to transform it, offering a distinct set of opportunities and risks (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Telzer and Prinstein2020). Specifically, the framework proposes that SM communication is characterised by a number of features, including temporal delays in social exchanges (asynchronicity), the creation of an indefinite trace or history of the communication (permanence), access and exposure to potentially large and/or open audiences (publicness), ease of access and engagement (availability), reduced availability of cues, including facial cues (cue absence), countable social metrics, such as likes and shares (quantifiable), and the potential to share photographic and video material (visualness). Different SM platforms will vary on a continuum with respect to each of these dimensions, and consequently, will afford different patterns of engagement (Karahanna et al., Reference Karahanna, Xu, Xu and Zhang2018; Moreno and Uhls, Reference Moreno and Uhls2019); see Eshraghian and Hafezieh (Reference Eshraghian and Hafezieh2017) for a review of the application of affordance theory to SM research. For example, one of the key appeals of Snapchat is that messages are only available for a defined/brief period of time (low permanence). Different tools or setting options within a given platform may also vary with respect to these feature dimensions. For example, Snapchat has now evolved to include longer-lasting and archival functionalities, including the ‘Snapchat Story’ feature, with putative impacts on the nature of user engagement (Cardell et al., Reference Cardell, Douglas, Maguire, Avieson and Giles2017).

Critically, the framework proposes that as a result of these characteristics, social interactions are transformed when they occur online, as noted, both with respect to dyadic and group communication. Specifically, they commonly: (i) change the frequency or immediacy of interactions (e.g. facilitation of high-speed messaging between individuals in a friendship group), (ii) amplify experiences and demands (e.g. intensification of an online dispute because of its public nature and the sheer volume of potential content), (iii) qualitatively change the nature of the experience (e.g. bullying may be perceived as harsher because of the anonymity of the perpetrator), (iv) facilitate compensatory behaviours, i.e. behaviours that may be more challenging in the offline world (e.g. social interactions may feel safer than offline interactions because of the asynchronicity in communication), and (v) provide opportunities for completely new behaviours and experiences (e.g. connection to specialist interest groups with people from far afield) (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018b).

The transformation framework does not imply that these effects are inherently positive or negative, but instead bring a range of potential risks and benefits. Nonetheless, and critical here with respect to impacts on mental health, Nesi and colleagues propose that, for some, a number of problematic interpersonal – or ‘depressogenic’ – processes may be amplified or increased in frequency within the context of online interactions (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018b). These include: social comparisons, co-rumination [defined as an excessive discussion of/rumination on problems and stressors with others (Rose, Reference Rose2002)], reassurance-seeking and feedback-seeking (Nesi and Prinstein, Reference Nesi and Prinstein2015). To take one example, (judgemental) social comparisons (and relatedly, negative self-image) may be facilitated online by the visualness, publicness, quantifiableness and availability of the SM environment, which essentially provides a highly detailed and easily accessible window onto others’ lives (Fardouly et al., Reference Fardouly, Pinkus and Vartanian2017; Marengo et al., Reference Marengo, Longobardi, Fabris and Settanni2018). Furthermore, the asynchronicity of the communication may also encourage heavy editing/engineering of photographs and self-representations (Kasch, Reference Kasch2013), such that the impression garnered is likely to be highly curated and idealised. Adolescents may be particularly vulnerable to such processes by virtue of their heightened sensitivity to peer influence (Ciranka and van den Bos, Reference Ciranka and van den Bos2019) and peer rejection (Sebastian et al., Reference Sebastian, Viding, Williams and Blakemore2010), as well as the fluid/developing nature of their identity (Spies Shapiro and Margolin, Reference Spies Shapiro and Margolin2014).

Conversely, several of the SM features explored may serve to afford more positive interactions and behaviours – both compensatory and novel (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018b). For example, the cue absence and asynchronicity of SM platforms may reduce the intensity of social interactions, and allow for communication to be slowed down, rehearsed and reflected upon (Bonetti et al., Reference Bonetti, Campbell and Gilmore2010; Harman et al., Reference Harman, Hansen, Cochran and Lindsey2005; Michikyan et al., Reference Michikyan, Subrahmanyam and Dennis2014; Valkenburg and Peter, Reference Valkenburg and Peter2008). This may be of particular benefit to individuals who struggle in more traditional face-to-face social settings.

As another example of how design features may facilitate positive behaviour and mental health, whilst the permanence, availability and quantifiable nature of many platforms may facilitate the spread and uptake of problematic behaviours such as self-harm (George, Reference George2019), it also has the potential to amplify prosocial interactions (Erreygers et al., Reference Erreygers, Vandebosch, Vranjes, Baillien and De Witte2019; Tsvetkova and Macy, Reference Tsvetkova and Macy2015). Indeed, there is evidence that with respect to online interactions amongst adolescents, prosocial behaviours may in fact be more common than antisocial ones (Erreygers et al., Reference Erreygers, Vandebosch, Vranjes, Baillien and De Witte2017; Lister, Reference Lister2007). This is crucial as we know that heightened sensitivity to peer influence and peer rejection during adolescence is true of prosocial as well as antisocial behaviour (Andrews et al., Reference Andrews, Ahmed and Blakemore2020). Thus, the very same features of online communication that carry risks also open up opportunities and potential benefits (Hollis et al., Reference Hollis, Livingstone and Sonuga-Barke2020; Livingstone and Helsper, Reference Livingstone and Helsper2010).

Extension of the transformation framework

In addition to the core features of SM platforms proposed by the transformation framework (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018b), which we have summarised above, we would suggest that SM platforms typically exhibit a number of additional design features, the primary function of which is to compete for users’ attention and maximise engagement (Alter, Reference Alter2017; Hill, Reference Hill2018; Lewis, Reference Lewis2017; Zuboff, Reference Zuboff2015), with potential implications for – and impacts on – the user’s ‘response state’, i.e. their cognitive, emotional and excitative state during engagement (Valkenburg and Peter, Reference Valkenburg and Peter2013), the nature of their engagement (i.e. online behaviour), and potentially, their mental health. These additional features include: (i) filtering algorithms, which selectively propagate information that is trending, (ii) intermittent status updates and notifications (Bosker, Reference Bosker2016; Lindström et al., Reference Lindström, Bellander, Schultner, Chang, Tobler and Amodio2021), and (iii) infinite scrolling and automatic play/replay content features (Noë et al., Reference Noë, Turner, Linden, Allen, Winkens and Whitaker2019).

In terms of the implications of these features for the response state of the SM user and their pattern of engagement, the nature of (i) filtering algorithms means that users are more likely to be exposed to information (e.g. posts and news stories) that they have previously accessed or else are trending within their network (Ciampaglia et al., Reference Ciampaglia, Nematzadeh, Menczer and Flammini2018), which by its very nature is likely to elicit strong emotional responses and high levels of arousal/excitation (amplifying and sensationalising). Thus, even if SM algorithms are not purposefully designed to amplify the emotional or excitative state of its users, the reality is that because emotional material increases attentional capture and user engagement (Brady et al., Reference Brady, Gantman and Van Bavel2020; Kozyreva et al., Reference Kozyreva, Lewandowsky and Hertwig2020; Tufekci, Reference Tufekci2013), there will be an algorithmic pull towards material that is controversial, extreme, polarised (Van Bavel et al., Reference Van Bavel, Rathje, Harris, Robertson and Sternisko2021), divisive and/or likely to elicit strong moral emotions such as outrage (Crockett, Reference Crockett2017; Goldenberg and Gross, Reference Goldenberg and Gross2020). For example, messages that elicit fear, disgust and surprise or include ‘moral-emotional’ words are more likely to spread (Brady et al., Reference Brady, Wills, Jost, Tucker, Van Bavel and Fiske2017; Vosoughi et al., Reference Vosoughi, Mohsenvand and Roy2017). Furthermore, immoral acts are more likely to be encountered online and elicit stronger moral outrage than similar acts learnt about in person or through other more traditional forms of media (Brady et al., Reference Brady, Wills, Jost, Tucker, Van Bavel and Fiske2017).

With respect to (ii) intermittent status updates and notifications (Bosker, Reference Bosker2016; Lindström et al., Reference Lindström, Bellander, Schultner, Chang, Tobler and Amodio2021), because of the uncertainty and unpredictability of social interactions, these will typically follow a variable-ratio reward schedule that will reinforce levels of engagement, i.e. increase usage, through basic operant conditioning processes (reinforcing). Finally, with respect to (iii) infinite scrolling, and relatedly, automatic play/replay content (Noë et al., Reference Noë, Turner, Linden, Allen, Winkens and Whitaker2019), these features maximise attentional capture and allow users to endlessly absorb material with minimal decision points and stopping cues, and hence minimal cognitive engagement with the decision process (absorbing) (LaRose, Reference LaRose2010; Noë et al., Reference Noë, Turner, Linden, Allen, Winkens and Whitaker2019).

In terms of the implications of these for mental health, the reinforcing and absorbing nature of the experience may increase the likelihood of automatic/habitual patterns of engagement (Eyal and Hoover, Reference Eyal and Hoover2014), which are potentially linked to negative mental health outcomes through a number of casual pathways. Whilst these processes (and the links between them) are explained in greater depth in the ‘Consequences and feedback loops’ section, it may suffice to note at this point, that when attention and behaviour is strongly captured by the media itself (reinforcing and absorbing), because of the limited capacity of attentional resources, attention to other cues beyond the screen (both internal and external) will be diminished, including those that may signal a potential benefit to stopping engagement, e.g. feelings of anger or fear triggered by an online interaction. Engagement thus becomes automatic or habitual, conditions under which it is easy to see how problematic patterns of use may emerge (Aarts et al., Reference Aarts, Verplanken and Knippenberg1998; Bae, Reference Bae2018; LaRose, Reference LaRose2010; LaRose et al., Reference LaRose, Lin and Eastin2003; Verplanken et al., Reference Verplanken, Aarts and Van Knippenberg1997).

With respect to the amplifying and sensationalising nature of the experience, this may feed negative emotions and exacerbate cognitive distortions/information processing biases, such as negativity biases, which lay at the core of many mental health difficulties and feed unhelpful cycles of thoughts, feelings/sensations and behaviours (Abramson et al., Reference Abramson, Metalsky and Alloy1989; Riskind, Reference Riskind1997). Again, whilst we explore these processes in greater depth in the ‘Information processing biases’ section, as a simple example, exposure to trending material that is highly emotive and triggering, e.g. graphic details of violent crime, may serve to confirm the beliefs of an anxious individual as to the dangerous nature of the world, thereby reinforcing negativity biases and increasing perceived threat (see Vignette 1 for example).

Vignette 1

Sonja was a young lady in her late teens who was highly anxious, withdrawn and avoidant [maladaptive core CBT cycle]. Over the course of therapy it became clear that some of her concerns about leaving the house related to an unrealistic appraisal of the risk of being murdered on the streets of her city, which she estimated as being 10% likely over the course of any given year [biased processing]. Further exploration of this belief and its origins indicated that it was perpetuated, in part, by her SM feed, which often featured gruesome and sensationalist stories that would elicit strong reactions and many shares within her network [biased processing, fed by amplifying and sensationalising features of the technology]. In response to this, we spent one of our sessions exploring the role of SM and her online experience in driving some of these beliefs and underlying cognitive distortions [psycho-education/SM literacy training], e.g. magnification and catastrophisation, and together identified alternative, more reliable sources of online information to help Sonja assess the genuine level of risk posed [intentional/purposeful engagement supporting relatively unbiased processing]. Further, in later sessions we explored how Sonja could meet with friends she’d initially met online in face-to-face contexts in a way that was safe, but also manageable given her current level of anxiety [social approach behaviours supported by intentional/purposeful engagement]. In the process she was able to gradually overcome her cycle of avoidance [cultivation of social approach behaviours, resulting in satisfaction of core needs relating to belonging and acceptance, and positive reinforcement], and test out some of her beliefs about how others would perceive and respond to her in person [amelioration of information processing biases].

These reinforcing, absorbing, amplifying and sensationalising processes are also likely to interact. Thus, in their Differential Susceptibility to Media Effects Model (DSMM), Valkenburg and Peter (Reference Valkenburg and Peter2013) propose that the effects of media (including impacts on behaviour and emotion, and by implication mental health) may be most pronounced and long-lasting when ‘cognitive, emotional and excitatory sliders, are high’ (p. 229). Such a state might be expected, we would argue, when the user is highly absorbed and engaged in the technology, and further, consuming and/or interacting (in a relatively uncritical way) with material that is highly emotionally evocative. Once again, there may be an algorithmic pull towards such states, although this is likely to vary across platforms and platform utilities.

The interpersonal–connections–behaviour framework

The interpersonal–connections–behaviour framework proposes a simple but powerful idea: that SM use is (in large part) beneficial or harmful to the individual to the extent that it promotes or frustrates core desires for acceptance and belonging, respectively (social gratifications obtained) (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Algoe and Green2018) [see Verduyn et al. (Reference Verduyn, Ybarra, Résibois, Jonides and Kross2017) also]. Whilst the framework was not designed specifically with adolescence in mind, we would argue that it is particularly relevant to this age group. Thus, acceptance and belonging and avoidance of rejection act as powerful motivators for behaviour during adolescence and are linked to key developmental challenges (Eisenberger et al., Reference Eisenberger, Lieberman and Williams2003; Tomova et al., Reference Tomova, Andrews and Blakemore2021). Furthermore, as noted, social motivations for SM use (gratifications sought) appear to be particularly important in driving this age group’s intention to engage with the technology (Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Chiu and Lee2011).

In their review of evidence supporting the interpersonal–connections–behaviour framework, Clark et al. (Reference Clark, Algoe and Green2018) suggested that negative outcomes are more likely when SM engagement is superficial and characterised by interaction patterns that are unlikely to cultivate genuine intimacy and long-term satisfaction of core social needs, e.g. ‘social snacking’ (p. 33), and/or patterns of use that are likely to cultivate feelings of disconnection, e.g. ‘lurking on stranger’s profiles’ (p. 33) (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Algoe and Green2018). Thus, they draw on a body of literature that highlights associations between passive patterns of use, e.g. scrolling, browsing and judgemental social comparisons, and poorer mental health/wellbeing. Conversely, Clark et al. (Reference Clark, Algoe and Green2018) highlight findings that link more active/interactive patterns of engagement, e.g. self-disclosure, online posting and direct exchanges with others, to better mental health/wellbeing, putatively through the accumulation of social capital and the cultivation of a greater sense of connectedness. We do not review these findings again here, but instead direct the reader back to the ‘Social media use and mental health’ section.

Integration of core theory

Integrating the evidence base reviewed and core theories/theoretical frameworks discussed within a developmental perspective, we propose that SM engagement may be particularly beneficial or helpful to the adolescent where use results in increased satisfaction of core needs (gratifications obtained), particularly those relating to developmentally pertinent needs of acceptance and belonging. Furthermore, we propose that such satisfaction of needs is most likely to occur when: (i) the individual’s motivations for use (gratifications sought) are primarily for social enhancement purposes, with a focus on cultivating and maintaining deep and genuine connections, (ii) the nature of the user’s online engagement is aligned with such enhancement motivations (e.g. engagement is active/interactive and non-judgemental), and relatedly, (iii) platforms used by the individual afford/support such patterns of engagement. Finally, SM may also be beneficial to mental health where it supports an amelioration of cognitive biases, e.g. negativity biases.

In contrast, SM engagement may be particularly harmful or unhelpful to the adolescent where use results in decreased satisfaction of core needs (gratifications obtained), particularly those relating to developmentally pertinent needs of acceptance and belonging, (e.g. increased feelings of loneliness or disconnection). Furthermore, we propose that this may be most likely when: (i) the individual’s motivations for use are (at least in part) social in nature (gratifications sought), but (ii) satisfaction of these needs is not supported by the individual’s actual online engagement (e.g. engagement is passive and judgemental, characterised in large part by social comparisons), and relatedly, (iii) the platforms which the individual uses do not afford helpful patterns of engagement, or else actively afford unhelpful patterns of engagement. SM engagement may also be harmful or unhelpful to the adolescent where the individual’s motivations for use (gratifications sought) directly conflict with satisfaction of core needs relating to belonging and acceptance, for example where SM use is (predominantly and persistently) motivated by compensatory motivations relating to escape or avoidance. Finally, SM may also be harmful where it feeds into existing cognitive biases that perpetuate unhelpful patterns of thoughts and feelings.

The conceptualisation

Overview

As noted, at the core of the conceptualisation lies a cross-sectional CBT cycle (McDonough et al., Reference McDonough, Duncan, Williams and House1997), which typically includes triggers, thoughts, feelings, sensations and behaviours (Fig. 1). For simplicity, feelings and sensations are collapsed, and the initial trigger/input to the model is defined as SM use. Other additions include: (i) a box to represent motivations for use, (ii) a box to represent information processing (David et al., Reference David, Miclea and Opre2004), which may be biased or relatively unbiased, and filters incoming (online and offline) information; (iii) a box to represent features and associated affordances of SM platforms (platform affordances), which may influence engagement with SM and how information is processed; (iv) a box to represent consequences or outputs of the model (i.e. gratifications obtained), with (v) feedback loops connecting consequences to motivations/SM use (as well as the user’s mode of engagement), and finally, (vi) a box to represent the mode of SM engagement, i.e. whether engagement is intentional/purposeful, or automatic/habitual.

Figure 1. The conceptualisation / model.

Considering unhelpful patterns of SM use first, the individual has various motivations for engaging with the technology, which are underpinned by needs, including social needs, that they hope to gratify. Incoming information derived from these online interactions (social media use) is filtered by information processing biases (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Rush, Shaw and Emery1979; Wells, Reference Wells1997), which drive and perpetuate unhealthy cycles of thoughts, feelings and behaviours, and ultimately, a number of negative consequences. To take a hypothetical – although relatively typical – example, consider a young SM user (let’s call her Yma) who goes online in order to connect with her peers (motivation) and reads a friend’s online post about a recent holiday. When filtered through a negativity bias, this might elicit negative automatic thoughts such as ‘other people’s lives are better than mine’ or ‘I’m not good enough’, driving a dip in mood, and as a result, a compulsion to socially withdraw and monitor others’ posts for confirmation/disconfirmation of this belief (avoidance behaviour) (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Rush, Shaw and Emery1979; Wells, Reference Wells1997). Whilst this cycle is reminiscent of offline interactions, it may be exacerbated online by the features and associated affordances of the SM platform, e.g. the fact that the material is highly visual, constantly accessible, and linked to social metrics such as likes and shares (availability, visualness, quantifiable). Furthermore, the reinforcing and absorbing nature of the experience, which is intended to hijack attention and maximise usage, may activate an automatic/habitual mode of engagement, which reduces Yma’s capacity to effectively self-monitor and critically evaluate presented material (e.g. recognise that the friend’s profile is likely to be heavily curated). As a result, cognition takes a ‘short-cut’ or uses a heuristic, information processing is biased, and the whole cycle is perpetuated.

Conversely, healthy or non-problematic cycles of thoughts, feelings and behaviours are enabled by relatively veridical information processing. Returning to our hypothetical example, imagine that Yma reads the same holiday post when she is in a very different state of mind, and is able to process the information in a relatively unbiased, and more critical, fashion, able to recognise, for example, the heavily curated nature of her friend’s profile. Relatively free of a negativity bias, thoughts triggered in this very different context might be neutral, or perhaps even linked to a positive memory of a similar childhood holiday with a family friend, such that a feeling of warmth or gratitude is elicited. This, in turn, drives Yma to reach out to the childhood friend (motivation), searching for them online and sending them a personalised message to invite them to join their network (approach behaviour). In this context, the affordances of SM, e.g. its availableness and publicness, will have facilitated this positive cycle, making it easier for Yma to locate and connect with her childhood friend. However, this requires intentional/purposeful engagement with the technology (mode), in the absence of which the pull will be towards a passive scrolling of online material, which has itself been linked to higher levels of (judgemental) social comparisons and associated feelings of envy (Appel et al., Reference Appel, Gerlach and Crusius2016; Krasnova et al., Reference Krasnova, Widjaja, Buxmann, Wenninger and Benbasat2015; Tandoc et al., Reference Tandoc, Ferrucci and Duffy2015; Verduyn et al., Reference Verduyn, Lee, Park, Shablack, Orvell, Bayer, Ybarra, Jonides and Kross2015; Verduyn et al., Reference Verduyn, Ybarra, Résibois, Jonides and Kross2017).

As a result of this cascade of thoughts, feelings/sensations and behaviours, a set of consequences follow, the nature of which ultimately determine whether, irrespective of the motivation for engagement, the original SM behaviour engaged in proves to be helpful or unhelpful. Thus, within the conceptualisation, aside from their impact on information processing biases (and associated exacerbation/amelioration of problematic patterns of thoughts, feelings, sensations and behaviours), patterns of SM use are associated with positive and negative mental health outcomes primarily as a function of their capacity to promote or frustrate (respectively) satisfaction of core needs for acceptance and belonging.

Returning to our previous example, in the unhelpful cycle the ultimate consequences of Yma’s social withdrawal may be a greater sense of loneliness and isolation (and loss of social capital), indicating a lack of alignment between gratifications sought and gratifications obtained. This loneliness is likely to feed into, and perpetuate, a cycle of unhelpful thoughts and feelings (e.g. negative thoughts about self, others and the world), and may set up a vicious cycle whereby feelings of disconnection brought about by SM use increase Yma’s motivation to engage with the technology further in order to seek connection and alleviate such feelings; see Perloff (Reference Perloff2014). This pattern may also be facilitated by negative reinforcement, particularly where SM use provides short-term distraction from such difficult feelings.

In contrast, in the second scenario/helpful cycle, the potential gains include an increased sense of connectedness and belonging, and an accumulation of social capital, indicating a good alignment between gratifications sought and gratifications obtained. This sense of connectedness is likely to feed into – and perpetuate – a cycle of helpful thoughts and feelings (e.g. positive thoughts about self, others and the world). These consequences may also reinforce patterns of SM use, through positive and/or negative reinforcement loops. Here this is unlikely to be problematic, as engagement is linked in this instance to positive consequences; in a small minority of cases, however, there is the possibility that this may topple over into problematic patterns of use (Wartberg et al., Reference Wartberg, Kriston and Thomasius2020).

Below we now go through each of the components of the model in greater depth, explaining how they interact with one another with reference to the relevant literature.

Key components

Motivations for use

Drawing on U&G theory (Katz et al., Reference Katz, Blumler and Gurevitch1973), at the top of the conceptualisation lie motivations for use, i.e. gratifications sought, which drive initial engagement with the technology (social media use). The nature of these motivations may underlie decisions as to which platform(s) or platform utilities to engage with, as each is characterised by a particular pattern of affordances (Brandtzæg and Heim, Reference Brandtzæg and Heim2009; Karahanna et al., Reference Karahanna, Xu, Xu and Zhang2018). However, other factors are also likely to play a role, e.g. which platforms are commonly used by the individual’s peers.

Social motivations are prioritised in the conceptualisation, as noted, because of their heightened relevance to adolescents and the developmental challenges/preoccupations they face (Ciranka and van den, Bos 2019; Sebastian et al., Reference Sebastian, Viding, Williams and Blakemore2010; Spies Shapiro and Margolin, Reference Spies Shapiro and Margolin2014), because of their centrality in driving young people’s engagement with the technology (Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Chiu and Lee2011), but also, because the proposed costs and benefits of use are closely tied to social consequences of engagement (see ‘Consequences and feedback loops’ section).

An important distinction is made between enhancement and compensatory motivations (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Holden and Ariati2021), which are likely to be differentially linked to approach and avoidance behaviours online, respectively, and consequently, positive and negative outcomes (see ‘Types of behaviours and their maintaining role’ section). However, it is unlikely that there will be a perfect mapping between motivations for, and outcomes of, use (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Holden and Ariati2021). Thus, gratifications sought and gratifications obtained need not be perfectly aligned. This could be for a variety of reasons, e.g. through a social skills deficit, or chance encounter with an unsupportive or hostile audience that impedes positive outcomes despite predominant enhancement motivations and approach behaviours; in addition, there is a risk (in some cases) that habitual behaviour may come to compete with more purposeful, goal-directed behaviour as repeated behaviours are reinforced (LaRose and Eastin, Reference LaRose and Eastin2004; Palmgreen et al., Reference Palmgreen, Wenner, Rosengren, Rosengren, Wenner and Palmgreen1985; Ruggiero, Reference Ruggiero2000). In addition, in our clinical experience we have seen how initial compensation motivations may slowly be replaced by enhancement motivations, particularly where initial online interactions are found to be rewarding and confidence is gained; see Vignette 2 for an example.

Vignette 2

Phil was in his early twenties and had a diagnosis of ASD. He was socially anxious, withdrawn and depressed when I met him [maladaptive core CBT cycle]. He was also not in employment, education or training (NEET), and had very few face-to-face interactions, preferring to spend most of his time online in chat-rooms and on SM [compensatory motivation and avoidant pattern of offline interactions, likely strengthened by negative reinforcement]. I spent many weeks trying to gently encourage Phil to connect with offline social activities and interest groups in his community, with the hope that this would break a cycle of avoidance and expose him to positive reinforcers. However, none of these groups seemed to ‘stick’. In parallel, however, Phil’s online life began to flourish, and he became part of an online network of individuals from around the world who were united by a common specialist interest [transition to approach pattern of online engagement]. I encouraged Phil to pursue this further, and experiment with cultivating greater closeness with some of his new-found friends, e.g. through expressions of interest in their world [intentional/purposeful pattern of engagement and cultivation of social approach behaviours]. Several sessions later Phil informed me that the whole group intended to meet in person and attend a convention abroad, which was dedicated to their specialist interest [transfer of online to offline social connections]. Despite my own initial concerns, Phil found part-time work and saved the money necessary to cover his expenses, and made the trip towards the end of our work together. Through the process his confidence grew considerably and his mood improved, with consequent impacts on his offline relationships too [positive reinforcement of social approach behaviours and generalisation], including within his family [leading to satisfaction of core needs relating to belonging and acceptance].

Social media use

Once the individual has engaged with SM (driven by their particular set of motivations) this provides a source of information to be processed (e.g. text, images and video). Basic parameters of interest include dimensions such as frequency and duration of use, and perhaps more critically, patterns of online behaviour and types of SM platform used. A direct line connects the SM use and behaviour boxes (Fig. 1) to indicate that SM engagement represents a subset of the broader category of (on- and off-line) behaviours, and further, that SM use is recursively activated at various points in the conceptualisation, generating further information that feeds into the model. Crucial to the formulation, no single parameter of use can be assumed to be implicitly helpful or unhelpful, but must instead be conceptualised within the broader context, e.g. with consideration of the individual’s resources, vulnerabilities and life circumstances (see ‘Types of behaviours and their maintaining role’ section).

Information processing biases

Before incoming information feeds into the core CBT cycle it is processed; however, because of the ambiguity of all data and the constructive nature of perception and cognition, this processing is prone to information processing biases including cognitive distortions, and relatedly, attentional biases (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Watkins, Mansell and Shafran2004a). Central to cognitive behavioural models of mental health difficulties, cognitive distortions may include: selective abstraction, over-generalisation, magnification/minimisation, personalising, catastrophising, mind-reading and fortune-telling (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Rush, Shaw and Emery1979; Wells, Reference Wells1997). In addition, Beck wrote of the ‘primitive’ characteristics of depressive thinking styles, which he deemed to be non-dimensional/global, absolutistic and moralistic, invariant, personalised or self-referential, polarised, character-diagnosis-like and irreversible in nature (Beck, Reference Beck1976; Beck et al., Reference Beck, Rush, Shaw and Emery1979).

Such processing biases play a trans-diagnostic role in the perpetuation of many mental health symptoms (Derakhshan, Reference Derakhshan2020; Grant et al., Reference Grant, Huskey, Faunce and Friedman2020; Jopling et al., Reference Jopling, Wilson, Burke, Tracy and LeMoult2020). Negativity biases, in which ambiguous or neutral information is interpreted in an exaggeratedly negative or threatening way (Baumeister et al., Reference Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer and Vohs2001; Rozin and Royzman, Reference Rozin and Royzman2001) and negative or threatening information is preferentially attended to (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Watkins, Mansell and Shafran2004a), may be particularly important to the development and maintenance of mental health symptoms, e.g. anxiety and low mood (Abramson et al., Reference Abramson, Metalsky and Alloy1989; Riskind, Reference Riskind1997), including in adolescents (Klein et al., Reference Klein, de Voogd, Wiers and a Salemink2018). Further, where information processing is characterised by catastrophisation and over-generalisation, anticipated and perceived threat is high, driving anxiety (Sussman et al., Reference Sussman, Jin and Mohanty2020). A simple online example of this can be seen in cases of health anxiety, where online symptom checking may feed negative thoughts, negativity biases and catastrophic misinterpretation of bodily signals, sometimes referred to as ‘Cyberchondria’ (Doherty-Torstrick et al., Reference Doherty-Torstrick, Walton and Fallon2016). In addition, (judgemental) social comparisons (both online and offline) will be exacerbated, and potentially more problematic, when underpinned by egocentric biases of ‘self-reference’ or ‘personalisation’ (Beck, Reference Beck1976; p. 92), and may be elevated in adolescence (Riva et al., Reference Riva, Triscoli, Lamm, Carnaghi and Silani2016)

Crucially, however, these biases may be more or less pronounced (dichotomised for simplicity here as biased processing versus relatively unbiased processing), and vary both between individuals, as well as within the individual at different points in time. Returning to the example of Yma, if she is processing information in a highly biased way when she views the peer’s holiday photos, she will likely ignore the broader context of their life and only attend to particular – often superficial – characteristics of the information (selective abstraction), assume that this heavily curated identity is in fact largely representative of everyone else’s lives (over-generalisation, magnification), and consequently, draw unfavourable comparisons with her own life (personalising). At the extreme, for example if Yma’s (state) self-esteem is very low and/or if she is feeling particularly vulnerable at the time, she may also take this as evidence of some fundamental inadequacy in her own nature (catastrophising, magnification and character-diagnosis-like).

In contrast, if Yma is having a good day and feeling confident, she will be more likely to evaluate the information more critically within its broader context, and integrate it with other sources of information (including memories) about the friend, the SM environment, and the wider world (i.e. processing will be relatively unbiased). Thus, she may discern that the friend’s profile is heavily curated, representing an idealised self that is neither characteristic of the individual, nor others’ experiences more generally. Consequently, the negative effects of the post may be bypassed or at least ameliorated.

Whilst such biases may be similar in online and offline contexts, crucial to the model is the notion that the nature of SM communication (platform affordances) means that such biases may be exacerbated or ameliorated online (see ‘The transformation framework’ and ‘Extension of the transformation framework’ sections, and Vignette 1).

Platform features and affordances

Within the conceptualisation, design features of SM platforms, and their related affordances, interact with several aspects of the model. These are denoted by the grey dashed lines in Fig. 1. First, as noted, platform design feeds directly into SM use/SM behaviours; thus, SM platforms are designed to capture users’ attention and reinforce engagement with the technology (reinforcing, absorbing), such that users are compelled to be connected more often and for longer (Alter, Reference Alter2017). In addition, in accordance with the core transformation framework (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018b), certain maladaptive behaviours may be directly facilitated by the nature of SM communication (e.g. its asynchronicity, visualness, publicness, availability, asynchronicity, etc.), including social comparisons, co-rumination, and reassurance and feedback-seeking, which are trans-diagnostic processes that are likely to perpetuate problematic cycles of thoughts, feelings and behaviours, and feed into pre-existing mental health difficulties (Çelik and Odaci, Reference Çelik and Odaci2013).

Second, SM use may amplify information processing and attentional biases. As discussed, platform algorithms typically compete for attention, connect like-minded users, and amplify sensationalist material that is trending (reinforcing, sensationalising) (Kaylor, Reference Kaylor2019), which is by its very nature biased, e.g. catastrophising, magnifiying/minimising, overgeneralising, etc. (Allcott et al., Reference Allcott, Braghieri, Eichmeyer and Gentzkow2020). This can lead to the creation of ‘filter bubbles’ and ‘echo chambers’ (Spohr, Reference Spohr2017), which restrict the availability of contradictory perspectives, and reinforce established biases in thinking. As examples, such algorithms may increase exposure to depressogenic content and thinking for individuals who struggle with their mood (Radovic et al., Reference Radovic, Gmelin, Stein and Miller2017), anxiety-triggering material for individuals who experience anxiety (McMullan et al., Reference McMullan, Berle, Arnáez and Starcevic2019), and/or drive unrealistic, idealised expectations of beauty in those with eating disorders or body dysmorphia (Saiphoo and Vahedi, Reference Saiphoo and Vahedi2019). As noted, such algorithms may also induce ‘response states’ that are more likely to drive engagement (Valkenburg and Peter, Reference Valkenburg and Peter2013), and arguably, fuel problematic thoughts, feelings and sensations, e.g. high levels of arousal/excitability in individuals who struggle with anxiety.

It is important to note, however, that this SM-driven amplification of information processing biases is not inevitable. Thus, the exact same features of SM that have the potential to spread harmful information across networks of users with speed, e.g. its permanence, publicness and availableness, also have the potential to facilitate access to multiple, alternative perspectives that may ameliorate some of these biases (Hollis et al., Reference Hollis, Livingstone and Sonuga-Barke2020). For example, SM – when used critically and purposefully – may connect users with accurate, specialist information including mental health support and resources (Hollis et al., Reference Hollis, Livingstone and Sonuga-Barke2020; Yonker et al., Reference Yonker, Zan, Scirica, Jethwani and Kinane2015) and expert-by-experience/peer-support groups (Naslund et al., Reference Naslund, Grande, Aschbrenner and Elwyn2014; Radovic et al., Reference Radovic, Gmelin, Stein and Miller2017). Where dyadic (and group) interactions are carefully and selectively engaged in, support and care may be elicited, reducing stress and negative affect, which in turn reduce cognitive biases (Yu, Reference Yu2016). Furthermore, the asynchronicity of SM and the potential for unlimited composition time (Moreno and Uhls, Reference Moreno and Uhls2019) mean that there is the potential for users to pause before communicating, mentalise their audience and consider the consequences of their engagement.

However, all of these potential benefits require intentional/purposeful engagement, and this may be challenging for adolescents as mentalisation, self-regulation and emotion-regulation skills are continuing to develop over this period and may lag behind the development of other processes (Andrews et al., Reference Andrews, Ahmed and Blakemore2020; Moreno and Uhls, Reference Moreno and Uhls2019). The challenges posed may be greater still for young people with neurodevelopmental, social communication and/or mental health difficulties, for whom executive function and other skills may be less developed or simply atypical (Malloy-Diniz et al., Reference Malloy-Diniz, Miranda and Grassi-Oliveira2017).

The core cognitive behavioural cycle

As noted, once filtered through information processing biases, information derived from SM engagement feeds into a standard (cross-sectional) CBT cycle consisting of thoughts, feelings/sensations and behaviours. Considering maladaptive cycles first, the model draws on a relatively standard case conceptualisation of mixed anxiety and depression or low self-esteem, as these are the symptoms most often described in studies of SM use, and further, are the focus of common presentations in community mental health services. As one might expect given the social nature of the medium and the heightened sensitivity of adolescents to peer influence and social rejection (Orben et al., Reference Orben, Tomova and Blakemore2020a), particular attention is given to social/interpersonal thoughts, emotions and behaviours (Jiménez Chafey et al., Reference Jiménez Chafey, Bernal and Rosselló2009). Thus, negative automatic thoughts about self and others, e.g. ‘I’m not good enough’ and ‘other people’s lives are better than mine’, are triggered by online content or interactions, and drive feelings of low mood and envy. Feelings of anxiety are also activated, since as per Melanie Fennel’s model of self-esteem, feelings and thoughts relating to a sense of inadequacy trigger predictions of rejection (‘they won’t like me’ or ‘people will think my post is stupid’, for example) (Fennell, Reference Fennell1998), a pattern seen in day-to-day clinical practice even where the primary diagnosis is depression (Jiménez Chafey et al., Reference Jiménez Chafey, Bernal and Rosselló2009).

Predictions (and actual experiences) of rejection of this nature may be particularly pertinent in online interactions. Thus, common features of many SM platforms, e.g. the relative absence of face-to-face and other cues (cue absence) (Culnan and Markus, Reference Culnan, Markus, Jablin, Putnam, Roberts and Porter1987; Lapidot-Lefler and Barak Reference Lapidot-Lefler and Barak2012), potential exposure to large audiences across multiple contexts (publicness) (Marwick and Boyd, Reference Marwick and Boyd2011) and the amplifying/sensationalising nature of the experience, may contribute to instigate well-documented disinhibition effects and reduced moral sensitivity/empathy, such that harsh and critical judgement as well as public shaming may be common (Ge, Reference Ge2020). Furthermore, the publicness, availability and permanence of many online platforms mean that expectations from peers may be amplified online (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a; Niland et al., Reference Niland, Lyons, Goodwin and Hutton2015), and a lack of near immediate response from others may be interpreted as rejection (Katsumata et al., Reference Katsumata, Matsumoto, Kitani and Takeshima2008), which is particularly painful for young people (Eisenberger et al., Reference Eisenberger, Lieberman and Williams2003). Resulting negative thoughts and feelings/sensations may then be perpetuated further by information processing biases (see ‘Social media use’ section), as well as a range of maladaptive or unhelpful behaviours, which ultimately decrease satisfaction of core needs.

Conversely, with respect to adaptive cycles of thoughts, feelings/sensations and behaviours, positive (or simply more realistic) appraisals of self (e.g. ‘I’m okay’ or ‘not everyone has to like what I say’) drive feelings of confidence, and these, perpetuated by relatively unbiased processing, facilitate a number of helpful behaviours that increase satisfaction of core needs. The nature of these behaviours are described separately below. Once again, in this context, the very same features of SM associated with potential harms, e.g. its availability, permanence and publicness, may serve to facilitate positive engagement, providing opportunities for social connection and identity experimentation (for example) (Hollis et al., Reference Hollis, Livingstone and Sonuga-Barke2020) that may perpetuate positive beliefs about self, others and the world, and satisfy core needs relating to acceptance and belonging.

Types of behaviours and their maintaining role

Both online and offline behaviours must be considered in parallel, as they commonly interact in complex ways to impact on the nature of social interactions and mental wellbeing (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a), with ‘spillover effects’ being common (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Telzer and Prinstein2020). For example, social connections made in one context (online or offline) may shift to the other (Ellison et al., Reference Ellison, Steinfield and Lampe2007), although problems such as arguments and disagreements may show similar patterns of transfer (George et al., Reference George, Jensen, Russell, Gassman-Pines, Copeland, Hoyle and Odgers2020). Also, individuals may attempt to use online interactions to cope with (or escape from) difficult thoughts and feelings triggered by offline interactions, or compensate for challenges and/or shortcomings experienced in the offline world (Zywica and Danowski, Reference Zywica and Danowski2008), and vice versa.

In addition to key problematic behaviours discussed previously (e.g. social comparisons, co-rumination and reassurance seeking), a broad distinction is made within the model between social approach and social avoidance/escape behaviours, denoting movement toward and away from the world, respectively (Gable, Reference Gable2006; Gazelle and Rudolph, Reference Gazelle and Rudolph2004; Nikitin and Freund, Reference Nikitin and Freund2008). One might also consider approach and avoidance behaviours more generally, i.e. online engagement as an escape from painful thoughts and feelings, versus online engagement as a way of connecting with people and things that are valued (Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette and Strosahl2016b). Such approach and avoidance behaviours have been proposed as trans-diagnostic factors (Fernández-Rodríguez et al., Reference Fernández-Rodríguez, Paz-Caballero, González-Fernández and Pérez-Álvarez2018; Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Strosahl and Wilson1999; Sportel et al., Reference Sportel, Nauta, de Hullu, de Jong and Hartman2011), which, underpinned by largely independent behavioural activation and behavioural inhibition motivational systems (Carver, Reference Carver1996; Gable et al., Reference Gable, Reis and Elliot2000), are implicated in a broad range of mental health conditions (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Moffitt, Newman and Silva1996; Contractor et al., Reference Contractor, Elhai, Ractliffe and Forbes2013; Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette and Strosahl2016b; Kimbrel et al., Reference Kimbrel, Mitchell and Nelson-Gray2010; Lahat et al., Reference Lahat, Pérez-Edgar, Degnan, Guyer, Lejuez, Ernst, Pine and Fox2012; Serrano-Ibáñez et al., Reference Serrano-Ibáñez, Ramírez-Maestre, Esteve and López-Martínez2019).

Within the behavioural/cognitive behavioural literature, avoidance has been linked to negative outcomes through multiple pathways, including preventing disconfirmation of negative beliefs and predictions, e.g. ‘they won’t like me’ or ‘they’ll reject me’ (Salkovskis, Reference Salkovskis1991) and habituation to feared objects or outcomes, e.g. face-to-face social interactions (Ferster, Reference Ferster1973; Lewinsohn, Reference Lewinsohn1975), and reducing access to positive reinforcers, e.g. positive social interactions (Gable, Reference Gable2006; Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Strosahl and Wilson1999). Conversely, approach behaviours facilitate fearful cognitions to be tested (and potentially disproven), enable habituation to feared objects or outcomes, increase exposure to positive reinforcers, and broaden the individual’s behavioural repertoire (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Watkins, Mansell and Shafran2004b; Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette and Strosahl2016b).

Whilst withdrawal and avoidance are in no way specific to SM use (or the online world more generally), they may be exacerbated online and/or by online interactions, because of the ease with which one can access – or withdraw – to an alternative world that is immediately available at the click of a button (García-Oliva and Piqueras, Reference García-Oliva and Piqueras2016) (availableness). In parallel, this same feature, i.e. the availableness of the online world, also presents opportunities for engagement, both compensatory and novel (Hollis et al., Reference Hollis, Livingstone and Sonuga-Barke2020). Thus, whilst SM provides opportunities for increased connection and growth (approach), it can also facilitate a retreat from the social world and a pulling away from experience (avoidance). As noted previously, such social reinforcers and punishers are particularly strong drivers of adolescent behaviour (Spear, Reference Spear2011).

Critically, such behaviours can only be categorised as approach or avoidance when considered in the broader context, e.g. the individual’s present mental state and learning history, in addition to the actual function of the behaviour (Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette and Strosahl2016a). A critical factor is therefore the trajectory of the individual’s behaviour relative to their current context: i.e. does the behaviour represent a movement towards or away from the world and their goals/values? For example, for an individual who is highly socially anxious or relatively socially inexperienced, the cultivation of relatively weak online social ties (e.g. sharing of videos or photos with minimal self-disclosure), might represent the only tolerable form of social engagement, and a safe intermediary step towards the cultivation of deeper social connections (see Vignette 2 for an example). For this individual at this point in time, therefore, this would represent an approach behaviour. In contrast, for an individual who was previously gregarious and socially confident, similar online activity might represent a retreat from more intimate face-to-face interactions and painful thoughts and feelings that arise. For this individual at this point in time, therefore, this would represent an avoidance behaviour.

In support of the importance of such approach versus avoidance behaviours in determining the relative benefits and harms of SM use (and digital technology more generally), a recent longitudinal study of over 1000 participants showed that during the COVID-19 pandemic, individuals with an approach coping style reported lower levels of depression, an effect that was partially mediated by increased levels of social network site use, and an associated accumulation of social support (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Lau and Luk2020). Conversely, individuals with an avoidant coping style reported higher levels of depression, and this was mediated by more time spent on internet gaming and (in turn) the experience of cyberbullying. The authors referred to these two pathways as the social-capital-accrual and escape-from-self hypotheses. Relatedly, whilst a longitudinal study of 256 adolescents reported that individuals using Facebook in order to make new friends (approach) was associated with decreased peer-related loneliness, its use as compensation for offline challenges (avoidance) was associated with increased loneliness (Teppers et al., Reference Teppers, Luyckx, Klimstra and Goossens2014).

Consequences and feedback loops

Whilst the conceptualisation proposes that SM engagement may interact with common mental health symptoms relatively directly through an amplification or amelioration of biased information processing (with knock-on effects on thoughts, feelings/sensations and behaviours), the social consequences of engagement are of critical importance in determining impacts on wellbeing. Thus, as discussed, in accordance with the interpersonal-connection-behaviours framework (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Algoe and Green2018) a defined pattern of SM use is (primarily) deemed to be helpful or harmful to the user to the extent that it helps or hinders the user from satisfying core needs, particularly those relating to belonging and acceptance (consequences). Satisfaction or dissatisfaction of social needs are then likely, as noted, to feed back into the core CBT cycle, perpetuating positive or negative beliefs (respectively) about the self, others and the world; for example, reciprocal links have been demonstrated between positive social relationships and self-esteem over time (Harris and Orth, Reference Harris and Orth2020). Furthermore, there may also be more tangible rewards to emerge from such social connections, including emotional and practical support in times of need (Guo et al., Reference Guo, Li and Ito2014; Williams, Reference Williams2019).

Whilst other consequences are also likely to be of relevance to mental health and wellbeing, e.g. satisfaction of other needs relating to autonomy and competence (Karahanna et al., Reference Karahanna, Xu, Xu and Zhang2018; Ryan and Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2017), or satisfaction of more aspirational needs underpinned by the individual’s particular set of values (Bramwell and Richardson, Reference Bramwell and Richardson2018), the conceptualisation focuses primarily on social needs relating to acceptance and belonging. This is for several reasons: first, research highlights the crucial role of such social processes in young people’s lives and mental health (Orben et al., Reference Orben, Tomova and Blakemore2020a; Tomova et al., Reference Tomova, Andrews and Blakemore2021); second, their engagement is driven predominantly by social motivations (Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Chiu and Lee2011); and third, from our combined experience of working clinically with this age group, where SM use interacts markedly with their mental health, this is typically through such social pathways (see Vignettes 14, for example). Finally, there is also evidence to suggest that other needs and challenges of adolescence, e.g. those relating to identity formation, may (themselves) be closely related to acceptance and belonging. For example, adolescent identity development is positively associated with healthy peer attachment and quality of peer relationships, romantic relationship satisfaction, and reduced levels of loneliness, such that a need for acceptance and belonging may in fact underpin/lay at the core of identity formation (Ragelienė, Reference Ragelienė2016).

Vignette 3

Sam had been bullied (offline) at secondary school and college, and never felt like she fitted in. Now in her early twenties, she felt anxious when separated from people who were closest to her, and struggled with fears of rejection [maladaptive core CBT cycle]. Sam noticed that using SM made her feel worse because she compared herself (unfavourably) with others [likely automatic/habitual patterns of use, e.g. scrolling, and high levels of judgemental social comparisons] and felt like she was being excluded from various social events and gatherings [leading to feelings of disconnection and isolation]. Consequently, she had at one point closed all her SM accounts and stopped connecting with others online [avoidance behaviour]. However, Sam realised that since leaving college SM had been one of the main ways that she had managed to maintain contact with her peers, such that her isolation grew [loss of potential avenues for social connection and satisfaction of core needs relating to acceptance and belonging]. As part of a much larger piece of work, Sam and I thought about how she might re-engage with her online world, but in a way that was in the service of her needs and values [intentional/purposeful pattern of engagement], i.e. cultivating and maintaining friendships with others whilst trying not to get drawn into social comparisons [cultivation of social approach behaviours]. In relation to this we explored the curated nature of the online world, and thought about what parts of the self people tend to share or hide online, as well as the impact this had on Sam’s self-confidence [psycho-education/SM literacy training]. We also thought about how the affordances of different SM platforms [e.g. the visualness, publicness and quantifiable nature of platforms like Instagram] might help or hinder Sam in the changes she wanted to make in her online life [values clarification], and helped her begin to make some of these changes [cultivation of intentional/purposeful engagement].

Vignette 4

Maria was a black British girl in her late teens who had been very depressed and accessed services following a suicide attempt. Whilst Maria was highly intelligent and valued her academic ability, her low mood was beginning to impact on her performance at college, which perpetuated negative thoughts and feelings about herself [maladaptive core CBT cycle]. Talking about issues of intersectionality, including class, gender and ethnicity helped with engagement, but also fed into a co-constructed formulation that helped make sense of the origin (and impact) of some of her negative beliefs about herself, others and the world. Through Socratic Questioning and exploration of her values [values clarification], Maria became increasingly aware of the consequences of her online and offline behaviours and how they took her towards or away from her values. In particular, Maria noticed a tendency to compare herself online to ‘pretty white wealthy girls’ who seemed to be living lives that were closed to her [high levels of social comparisons], which also perpetuated negative beliefs about herself and impacted on her mood [feelings of disconnection and isolation]. Over the course of our sessions together we explored the costs and benefits of alternative online spaces [psycho-education/SM literacy training, including on platform affordances] that Maria might access in order to connect with others who she could relate to better [cultivation of social approach behaviours], for example people who ‘looked like [her]’ and had shared values. By gradually changing her patterns of SM use [cultivation of intentional/purposeful pattern of engagement] Maria began to be exposed to the stories of others, more like herself, who had succeeded in different areas of their lives. As a result, Maria started to feel a greater sense of connection [satisfaction of core needs relating to acceptance and belonging] and grew more hopeful about herself and her future.

Finally, the consequences of the model also drive a number of feedback loops that reinforce general SM use and/or particular patterns/modes of engagement. Such feedback loops operate firstly through standard operant conditioning process, e.g. positive and negative reinforcement (Nadkarni and Hofmann, Reference Nadkarni and Hofmann2012; Wegmann and Brand, Reference Wegmann and Brand2019); thus, computational modelling of SM use has shown that human behaviour on SM platforms follows basic principles of reward learning (Lindström et al., Reference Lindström, Bellander, Schultner, Chang, Tobler and Amodio2021). However, such reinforcement processes may be exacerbated online as a result of the near-constant availability of tangible feedback in the form of likes, comments and shares (availability and quantifiability) delivered on a variable-ratio reward schedule (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018b).

Reinforcement of SM engagement may be helpful, e.g. where social rewards (such as an increased feeling of connection) drive further approach behaviours, as these are likely to increase the probability that core needs will be satisfied, and hence that positive thoughts, feelings/sensations and behaviours will be perpetuated, building confidence and self-esteem. However, reinforcement of SM engagement can also be unhelpful, e.g. where it perpetuates a cycle of avoidance and withdrawal from the world, as this is likely to increase the probability that core needs will not be met, and hence that negative thoughts, feelings/sensations and behaviours will be perpetuated, and confidence/self-esteem will fall. In a minority of cases, such reinforcement may also lead to addictive or problematic patterns of use (Nadkarni and Hofmann, Reference Nadkarni and Hofmann2012; Turel and Qahri-Saremi, Reference Turel and Qahri-Saremi2016; Turel and Serenko, Reference Turel and Serenko2012); thus, even when SM behaviours are initially purposeful and goal-directed, with repetition and repeated reinforcement habits may form, such that engagement becomes relatively automatic, i.e. ‘lack[ing] effortful attention […] intentionality, awareness, and/or controllability’ (p. 197; LaRose Reference LaRose2010), with all the risks that this entails; see ‘Mode of engagement’ section and research on media habits, e.g. LaRose and Eastin (Reference LaRose and Eastin2004).

With respect to such reinforcement, it is also important to consider the short-term as well as the long-term consequences of use. Thus, it is well established that consequences that follow shortly after a behaviour acquire strong controlling functions, i.e. short-term consequences dominate in the control of behaviour (Ramnero and Torneke, Reference Ramnero and Torneke2008). For example, SM use, or particular online behaviours, may be negatively reinforced through short-term distraction from aversive feelings of loneliness, disconnection or even (potentially) withdrawal (Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Lee and Lee2013), or positively reinforced through transient or superficial interactions that trigger a momentary sense of pleasure, i.e. ‘social snacking’ (p. 33, Clark et al., Reference Clark, Algoe and Green2018; and Krämer et al., Reference Krämer, Lucas, Schmitt and Gratch2018). In both cases, however, whilst the long-term consequences of use may be a decreased satisfaction of core needs relating to acceptance and belonging, the behaviour will still be reinforced since (as noted) short-term consequences typically dominate.

Another problematic feedback loop may be established through a parallel process that operates via its effect on motivations; thus, in the Transactional Model of Social Media and Body Image Concerns model, Perloff (Reference Perloff2014) describes a process whereby the individual engages with appearance-focused SM content in order to seek reassurance and validation (gratifications sought), but as a result of online social comparative processes actually experiences an increased sense of body dissatisfaction (gratifications obtained). This leads to a greater motivation to engage with SM and seek reassurance online, such that a powerful feedback loop is set up. Relating this to our conceptualisation, where the consequences of use are a decreased sense of acceptance and belonging (or increased sense of disconnection) the user’s motivation to engage in order to seek connection may be amplified. From a behaviourist perspective this may be conceptualised as an establishing operation, altering the rewarding experience of online interactions, just as thirst alters the rewarding experience of drinking a glass of water (Ramnero and Torneke, Reference Ramnero and Torneke2008). Of course, the flip side to this is that where needs of acceptance and belonging are met, motivations to use SM may remain stable or even decrease.

Mode of engagement

An over-arching component of the model is the user’s mode of engagement; specifically, the extent to which SM is engaged with intentionally, purposefully and with awareness (i.e. mindfully), as opposed to automatically and habitually (i.e. mindlessly) (LaRose, Reference LaRose2010). Mindfulness, which is often distinguished from states of ‘automatic inattentiveness’ or ‘automatic pilot’ (Van Dam et al., Reference Van Dam, Earleywine and Borders2010) can be defined as purposeful, non-judgemental, present-centred awareness (Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Lau, Shapiro, Carlson, Anderson, Carmody, Segal, Abbey, Speca, Velting and Devins2006), and has been operationalised in both state and trait forms (Kiken et al., Reference Kiken, Garland, Bluth, Palsson and Gaylord2015). The concept (and practice) of mindfulness has become incorporated into mainstream cognitive behavioural therapies (Baer, Reference Baer, Hayes and Hofmann2018), and when separated from the concept of mindfulness meditation practice, in its more prosaic day-to-day sense mindfulness can be conceptualised as something akin to an ongoing awareness of internal and external stimuli (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Ryan and Creswell2007) and an associated ‘acting with awareness’ (Turel and Osatuyi, Reference Turel and Osatuyi2017), thus sharing some features with the concept of meta-cognition (Hussain, Reference Hussain2015).

Within the conceptualisation, intentional/purposeful SM engagement is typically associated with more positive outcomes. This is because of its potential to increase attentional control and enhance processing of internal and external signals, and thereby modulate cognitive, affective and physiological process that are central to self-regulation and emotional wellbeing (Schuman-Olivier et al., Reference Schuman-Olivier, Trombka, Lovas, Brewer, Vago, Gawande, Dunne, Lazar, Loucks and Fulwiler2020; Vago and David, Reference Vago and David2012) [‘through attentional focus, individuals learn to become more aware of habit-linked, minimally conscious affective states and bodily sensations […], thus ‘de-automating’ this largely habitual process’ (p. 201, Houlihan and Brewer, Reference Houlihan, Brewer, Shonin, Van Gordon, Griffiths and New York2016)]. Thus, as indicated in Fig. 1 by the (thicker) connecting arrows, intentional/purposeful engagement: (i) ameliorates information processing biases by enabling greater attentional control and access to internal and external signals that carry potentially relevant information (information processing) (Heppner et al., Reference Heppner, Kernis, Lakey, Campbell, Goldman, Davis and Cascio2008), allowing the user to (ii) process/weigh up the potential benefits and costs of their behaviour using this information (thoughts), and consequently, (iii) respond in a manner that is in the service of their needs and values (behaviours), the net effect of which is (typically) to: (iv) reduce negative and increase positive affective states (feelings/sensations) and drive more adaptive cycles of thoughts, feelings and sensations. In simpler terms, a more intentional/purposeful mode of engagement may increase the user’s awareness of the consequences of their online behaviour and the material to which they are exposed, facilitate a more critical appraisal of the information with which they engage, and further, enable them to be more selective in who/what they chose to connect with/attend to.

In contrast, automatic/habitual engagement may increase the individual’s susceptibility to attentional capture by the technology (reinforcing, absorbing), increasing information processing biases, and thereby perpetuate problematic cycles of thoughts, feelings/sensations and behaviours. Thus, when behaviour is automatic/habitual ‘it is guided by automated cognitive processes [i.e. biased processing], rather than being preceded by elaborate decision processes’ (p. 1355) (Aarts et al., Reference Aarts, Verplanken and Knippenberg1998; LaRose, Reference LaRose2010; Verplanken et al., Reference Verplanken, Aarts and Van Knippenberg1997). At the extreme, this may lead the user to unreflectively drift into problematic or addictive patterns of use that do not serve their needs. In contrast, mindfulness may facilitate ‘a space of awareness between craving and the problematic behaviour that would not otherwise occur’ (p. 6, Owen et al., Reference Owen, Heisterkamp, Halfmann and Vorderer2018). Again, in simpler terms, a more automatic/habitual mode of engagement will mean that the user is likely to be less aware of the consequences of their online behaviour and the material to which they are exposed, less critical of the information that they engage with, and further, less selective in who/what they connect with/attend to.

High levels of mindfulness have been linked to more veridical processing of day-to-day experience and a reduced reliance on prior beliefs and expectations, i.e. relatively unbiased information processing (Lakey et al., Reference Lakey, Kernis, Heppner and Lance2008) and through a bypassing (or amelioration) of such cognitive biases, reduced symptoms of anxiety and depression (Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Polak and Remmerswaal2019; Schmertz et al., Reference Schmertz, Masuda and Anderson2012). Furthermore, the practice of mindfulness meditation has been linked to reduced negativity biases, rumination and the need for others’ approval, potentially offering some protection against the impact of judgemental social comparison processes (Kiken and Shook, Reference Kiken and Shook2011; Ramel et al., Reference Ramel, Goldin, Carmona and McQuaid2004; Vago and Nakamura, Reference Vago and Nakamura2011) and a reduced reliance on some of the ‘depressogenic’ behaviours highlighted by the transformation framework, e.g. co-rumination, reassurance-seeking and feedback-seeking (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018b).

Perhaps more importantly, there is evidence for the role of mindfulness in supporting behaviour change (Schuman-Olivier et al., Reference Schuman-Olivier, Trombka, Lovas, Brewer, Vago, Gawande, Dunne, Lazar, Loucks and Fulwiler2020) and less compulsive/habitual patterns of behaviour (Wenk-Sormaz, Reference Wenk-Sormaz2005), including with respect to SM use and digital technologies more generally. For example, dispositional mindfulness has been shown to mediate associations between psychological maltreatment by parents, psychological symptoms, self-esteem and internet/Facebook addiction in university students (Arslan, Reference Arslan2017; Eşkisu et al., Reference Eşkisu, Çam, Gelibolu and Rasmussen2020), moderates the effects of mobile phone addiction on high levels of rumination and poor sleep quality in a protective manner (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhou, Yang, Kong, Niu and Fan2017b), and has been linked to less compulsive (mobile phone-based) SM use amongst adolescents/emerging adults via a mediating pathway that involves self-esteem and social anxiety (Apaolaza et al., Reference Apaolaza, Hartmann, D’Souza and Gilsanz2019). Furthermore, mindfulness has been shown to moderate the association between SM use and ‘burnout’ in the workplace (Charoensukmongkol, Reference Charoensukmongkol2016), and in an artificial SM environment, protects against psychological distress, negative emotions and anti-social tendencies triggered by ‘feeling left out’ when receiving few likes (Poon and Jiang, Reference Poon and Jiang2020). Thus, through a number of pathways a more mindful mode of engagement is likely to be associated with better outcomes.

Clinical implications

Assessment and formulation

Given the relative ubiquity of SM use, we think that, as a minimum, an exploration of service users’ online worlds should constitute an integral part of the assessment process, particularly for young people, for whom their online life may be as rich and vivid as their offline world. Whilst this should incorporate exploration of explicit online risks (particularly where impulsive or risky behaviour is indicated more generally, or where the individual is deemed vulnerable in some way), we hope that the conceptualisation will encourage clinicians to go further than this, and explore service users’ patterns of use, online relationships and interactions more generally, and think about how these might interact with existing difficulties to both exacerbate and/or ameliorate symptoms (see Table 1).

Table 1. Ideas for assessment and intervention based on a trans-diagnostic cognitive behavioural conceptualisation of the positive and negative roles of social media use in adolescents’ mental health and wellbeing

This includes (potential) areas for assessment, factors to consider within each of these areas, example questions that can be used to elicit relevant information within each of these areas/factors for consideration, in addition to ideas for possible interventions depending on information gathered. The interventions listed are drawn (in main part) from standard behavioural and cognitbehavive behavioural approaches and are informed by our own clinical practice. Note: the examples presented represent ideas for potential exploration and should be used flexibly depending on context, and in response to material emerging from sessions, rather than used prescriptively as an assessment pro forma or treatment protocol.

Where SM use is seen as playing a significant role in the individual’s life (e.g. as a precipitating, perpetuating or protective factor), these should be collaboratively integrated into an individualised conceptualisation, as per any CBT treatment. Thus, we are not proposing that the conceptualisation be used to inform a ‘CBT for SM use’ protocol, but that instead, the conceptualisation (or components of it where deemed relevant) be integrated into standard CBT formulations or case conceptualisations. At the core of the conceptualisation (Fig. 1) lies a standard cross-sectional CBT cycle, on to which all (or a subset of) the suggested additional components can essentially be ‘bolted on’ (motivations for use, information processing, consequences, platform affordances and feedback loops) to see if these increase the explanatory power/utility of the model. For example, building in an understanding of how SM platforms (platform affordances) can exacerbate cognitive biases and fuel problematic behaviour, e.g. social comparisons in depression, or online symptom checking in health anxiety and panic, may aid the individual’s understanding of their difficulties and what is maintaining them, and critically, suggest targets for intervention (see Vignettes for examples). Conversely, noting where SM represents an under-used resource for cultivation of social connection and identity exploration may highlight new directions for engagement.

Intervention

The primary function of a clinical model or formulation is to inform a treatment plan. Whilst, as we have noted, an individualised case conceptualisation should be constructed in collaboration with the young person, and the purpose of the proposed model is not to ‘treat’ (or pathologise) SM use, the model does suggest a number of potential likely foci for intervention that are associated with core SM-related precipitating, perpetuating and protective factors. These should be addressed at all stages of the treatment process, i.e. integrated into assessment, formulation, psychoeducation and intervention.

With respect to psychoeducation, using the conceptualisation that we have presented, standard cognitive behavioural psychoeducation (e.g. exploration of the role of various behaviours in the perpetuation of core maladaptive cycles), may be integrated with SM literacy training (Livingstone and Helsper, Reference Livingstone and Helsper2010; Torrent, Reference Torrent, Culver and Kerr2014) in order to help the young person understand how the technology interacts with their personal presenting difficulties (for better and for worse), and further, to help them develop ‘the technical and cognitive competencies users need to use social media in an effective and efficient way for social interaction and communication on the web’ (p. 4, Daneels and Vanwynsberghe, Reference Daneels and Vanwynsberghe2017).

According to Schreurs and Vandenbosch’s (2021) Social Media Literacy model (SMILE), such SM literacy serves as both a moderator in the association between SM use and mental health/wellbeing, as well as a predictor; see Valkenburg and Peter (Reference Valkenburg and Peter2013) also. With respect to its role as a moderator, SMILE proposes that the literate user is more aware of the potential negative impacts of their use, as well as certain online material, and as such, is better equipped to regulate their emotional and behavioural responses to it. SMILE also proposes that the literate user will be better equipped to ‘focus on the scarce positive aspects’ of otherwise negative online experiences (p. 324, Schreurs and Vandenbosch, Reference Schreurs and Vandenbosch2021), presumably thereby driving a more adaptive cycle of thoughts, feelings, sensations and behaviours. With respect to the role of SM literacy as a predictor of mental health/wellbeing, SMILE proposes that literate users are more likely to be exposed to more positive/less harmful online material through a more critical and intentional evaluation of who and what to engage with, as well as selective attention towards more positive material. Considered within our conceptualisation, such cultivation of SM/psychological literacy might be seen as equipping and incentivising the user to engage with the technology in a more critical and purposeful way (intentional/purposeful mode of engagement), with all the benefits that this entails, e.g. greater weighing of contextual information, greater control over attention and behaviour, more critical evaluation of material/amelioration of processing biases, etc.

Potential topics for exploration within such psychoeducation/SM literacy training include the heavily curated nature of the online world, i.e. the ‘highlights reel’ (Madden et al., Reference Madden, Lenhart, Cortesi, Gasser, Duggan and Smith2013) or ‘positivity bias’ as a phenomenon (Schreurs and Vandenbosch, Reference Schreurs and Vandenbosch2021), characteristics of SM platforms and their implications for the nature of online interactions (Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018a; Nesi et al., Reference Nesi, Choukas-Bradley and Prinstein2018b), e.g. the high prevalence of low quality information/misinformation online (Agarwal et al., Reference Agarwal, Gupta and Kraut2008), and the nature (and implications) of well-documented online psychological phenomena such as online disinhibition (Suler, Reference Suler2004).

In terms of possible foci for active intervention, as with any CBT treatment protocol a good place to start is raising awareness of core processes identified in the conceptualisation, as we cannot begin to change that which we are not aware of. This might involve noticing and identifying typical patterns of SM use and tracking their impact, with a view (once again) to encouraging more mindful and critical engagement, e.g. journalling, thought recording, activity scheduling and behavioural experiments.

With respect to behaviours, values-based work might be introduced to explore where the individual’s SM use (and behaviour more generally) is in line with their needs, values and aspirations, and where discrepancies are identified, strategies incorporated to effect behavioural change (Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Strosahl and Wilson2003). For example, the use of activity scheduling and graded exposure may be particularly useful to help the individual overcome cycles of avoidance and withdrawal, e.g. encouraging individuals to take (meaningful, values-congruent) risks in social interactions and cultivate social connections (see Vignette 2). The SM environment may be a particularly helpful starting place to initiate this work as its publicness and availableness means that small steps towards social engagement can be easily implemented. In addition, the relative asynchronicity of the online world (or much of it) allows social interactions to be slowed down, reflected upon, and brought to therapy sessions to discuss in a way that is not possible in real-time, face-to-face social interactions. Also, the young person may have interests, skills and resources that are specific to (or enhanced by) the online world, and which can be drawn upon in the service of their values.

With respect to specific platform affordances, where these are deemed to be interacting problematically with presenting issues, alternatives may be explored. As an example, for someone with low self-esteem that is triggered or perpetuated by judgemental social comparisons an alternative SM platform that is less public, visual, permanent, asynchronous and quantifiable, might be explored as a way of maintaining social connections, whilst minimising the potential risks of such interactions (see Vignettes 3 and 4 for examples).

With respect to consequences and reinforcement loops included in the conceptualisation, these may be helpfully explored through a functional analysis of the individual’s SM use. In this regard, drawing out the long-term consequences of usage patterns may highlight activity that is reinforced by ‘short-term gain’, at the cost of ‘long-term pain’ (Rizvi, Reference Rizvi2019). Furthermore, understanding what is driving/maintaining problematic engagement with SM (including motivations and reinforcement loops) may inform appropriate interventions. For example, if engagement is driven by a fear of missing out (Przybylski et al., Reference Przybylski, Murayama, Dehaan and Gladwell2013), cutting down on SM use might be helpful as it removes a major trigger, whereas if a need for connection is driving engagement, reducing use is unlikely to be helpful unless alternative means of socialising are devised.

Finally, given the proposed over-arching role of mindful/purposeful (versus mindless/habitual) modes of engagement within the conceptualisation, some introduction to mindfulness skills training might also be indicated. This would enable the individual to become more aware of relevant internal and external signals, including thoughts, feelings, sensations and behavioural intentions related to their SM use, track the consequences of use, and encourage more intentional/purposeful engagement.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this paper described a trans-diagnostic cognitive behavioural conceptualisation of the positive and negative roles of SM use in adolescent mental health and wellbeing, with a focus on how SM interacts with common mental health difficulties seen in everyday practice in community mental health settings. Furthermore, it provided ideas for how this conceptualisation might be drawn upon in clinical practice, with case vignettes and suggestions for integration into cognitive behavioural assessment and intervention. Whilst some of the components of the conceptualisation are firmly rooted in the extant literature, others are more speculative; nonetheless, the conceptualisation is presented as a foundation for future exploration, including a testing of key components and predictions to emerge from the conceptualisation, as well an assessment of its practical utility in day-to-day clinical practice.

Like it or not, SM (and digital technology more generally) is here to stay, and has come to transform most, if not all, aspects of our lives, as well as the lives of the people we see clinically. We hope therefore that this paper will – at the very least – convince the reader that keen attention to the online worlds of the young people we see clinically is essential if we are to gain a thorough understanding of the full range of difficulties and challenges, rewards and opportunities that they face and can draw upon in their lives.

Key practice points

  1. (1) When working with adolescents always consider their online life at the assessment and formulation stage, and where indicated, during treatment.

  2. (2) When exploring service users’ online life, consider the nature and function of the behaviours they engage in, with particular attention to social approach and social avoidance behaviours.

  3. (3) When exploring service users’ online life, consider the nature and design of platforms that they use, as each will have a different set of affordances, with potential implications for formulation and treatment.

  4. (4) When integrating service users’ online life into treatment, consider potential benefits to capitalise upon as well as risks to navigate.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Dr Jack Hollingdale and Dr Samantha Bottrill for their useful comments on the manuscript.

Author contributions

Marc Tibber: Conceptualization (lead), Writing – original draft (lead), Writing – review & editing (lead); Emma Silver: Writing – review & editing (supporting).

Financial support

None.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare none.

Ethics statement

This paper abided by the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct as set out by the BABCP and BPS.

Data availability statement

Data availability is not applicable to this article as no new data were created or analysed in this study.

Footnotes

1 For the purpose of this paper we define social media relatively broadly to include applications, platforms and other technologies that have been classified by Kaplan and Haenlein (Reference Kaplan and Haenlein2010) as medium or high with respect to ‘social presence’ and ‘media richness’. This definition includes social network sites such as Facebook and MySpace, content communities such as YouTube and Instagram, virtual social and game worlds such as Second Life and World of Warcraft, but does not include blogs/micro-blogs or collaborative projects such as Wikipedia.

References

Further reading

Orben, A. (2020a). Teenagers, screens and social media: a narrative review of reviews and key studies. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 55, 407414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesi, J., Choukas-Bradley, S., & Prinstein, M. J. (2018a). Transformation of adolescent peer relations in the social media context: Part 1 – a theoretical framework and application to dyadic peer relationships. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 21, 267294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesi, J., Choukas-Bradley, S., & Prinstein, M. J. (2018b). Transformation of adolescent peer relations in the social media context: Part 2 – application to peer group processes and future directions for research. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 21, 295319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Aalbers, G., McNally, R. J., Heeren, A., de Wit, S., & Fried, E. I. (2019). Social media and depression symptoms: a network perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148, 14541462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
AAP Council on Communications and Media (2016). Media and young minds. Pediatrics, 138, e20162591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aarts, H., Verplanken, B., & Knippenberg, A. (1998). Predicting behavior from actions in the past: repeated decision making or a matter of habit? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 13551374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abi-Jaoude, E., Treurnicht Naylor, K., & Pignatiello, A. (2020). Smartphones, social media use and youth mental health. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 192, E136141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aboujaoude, E. (2010). Problematic internet use: an overview. World Psychiatry, 9, 8590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: a theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96, 358–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adnan, H. M., & Mavi, S. R. (2015). Facebook satisfaction, life satisfaction: Malaysian undergraduate experience. Jurnal Komunikasi: Malaysian Journal of Communication, 31, 649–71.Google Scholar
Agarwal, R., Gupta, A. K., & Kraut, R. (2008). The interplay between digital and social networks. Information Systems Research, 19, 243–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahn, D., & Shin, D. H. (2013). Is the social use of media for seeking connectedness or for avoiding social isolation? Mechanisms underlying media use and subjective well-being. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 2453–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allcott, H., Braghieri, L., Eichmeyer, S., & Gentzkow, M. (2020). The welfare effects of social media. American Economic Review, 110, 629676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Almquist, Y. (2009). Peer status in school and adult disease risk: a 30-year follow-up study of disease-specific morbidity in a Stockholm cohort. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 63, 1028–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Almquist, Y. B., & Östberg, V. (2013). Social relationships and subsequent health-related behaviours: linkages between adolescent peer status and levels of adult smoking in a Stockholm cohort. Addiction, 108, 629–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alter, A. (2017). Irresistible – The Rise of Addictive Technology and the Business of Keeping Us Hooked. New York, NY, USA: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Andrade, A. D., & Doolin, B. (2016). Information and communication technology and the social inclusion of refugees. MIS Quarterly: Management Information Systems, 40, 405–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, J. L., Ahmed, S., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2020). Navigating the social environment in adolescence: the role of social brain development. Biological Psychiatry, 89, 109118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apaolaza, V., Hartmann, P., D’Souza, C., & Gilsanz, A. (2019). Mindfulness, compulsive mobile social media use, and derived stress: the mediating roles of self-esteem and social anxiety. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 22, 388396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Appel, H., Gerlach, A. L., & Crusius, J. (2016). The interplay between Facebook use, social comparison, envy, and depression. Current Opinion in Psychology, 9, 4449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arendt, F., Scherr, S., & Romer, D. (2019). Effects of exposure to self-harm on social media: evidence from a two-wave panel study among young adults. New Media & Society, 21, 24222442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arslan, G. (2017). Psychological maltreatment, forgiveness, mindfulness, and internet addiction among young adults: a study of mediation effect. Computers in Human Behavior, 72, 5766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baccarella, C. V., Wagner, T. F., Kietzmann, J. H., & McCarthy, I. P. (2018). Social media? It’s serious! Understanding the dark side of social media. European Management Journal, 36, 431–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bae, M. (2018). Understanding the effect of the discrepancy between sought and obtained gratification on social networking site users’ satisfaction and continuance intention. Computers in Human Behavior, 79, 137–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baer, R. (2018). Mindfulness practice. In Process Based CBT – The Science and Core Clinical Competencies of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy, pp. 389402, edited by Hayes, S.C., & Hofmann, S. G. Oakland, CA, USA: Context Press.Google Scholar
Baker, R. K. (2010). Predicting adolescents’ use of social networking sites from an extended theory of planned behaviour perspective – Google Search. Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 1591–1597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartel, K., Scheeren, R. & Gradisar, M. (2019.) Altering adolescents’ pre-bedtime phone use to achieve better sleep health. Health Communication, 34, 456462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C. & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, A. (1976). Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. Michigan, USA: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive Therapy of Depression (1st edn). New York, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Beck, J. (1995). Cognitive Therapy, Basics and Beyond. New York, USA: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bingham, C. R., Simons-Morton, B. G., Pradhan, A. K., Li, K., Almani, F., Falk, E. B., Shope, J. T., Buckley, L., Ouimet, M. C., & Albert, P. S. (2016). Peer passenger norms and pressure: experimental effects on simulated driving among teenage males. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 41, 124–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., Segal, Z. V., Abbey, S., Speca, M., Velting, D., & Devins, G. (2006). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230–41.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J. (2018). Inventing Ourselves: The Secret Life of the Teenage Brain. Doubleday.Google Scholar
Blum-Ross, A., & Livingstone, S. (2018). The trouble with ‘screen time’ rules. In Digital Parenting – The Challenges for Families in the Digital Age, pp. 179–188, edited by G. Mascheroni, G., Ponte, C., & Jorge, A. Borås: The International Clearinghouse on Children, Youth and Media.Google Scholar
Bonetti, L., Campbell, M. A., & Gilmore, L. (2010). The relationship of loneliness and social anxiety with children’s and adolescents’ online communication. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 13, 279–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosker, B. (2016). The Binge Breaker. The Atlantic, November 2016.Google Scholar
Brady, W. J., Gantman, A. P., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2020). Attentional capture helps explain why moral and emotional content go viral. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149, 746756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brady, W. J., Wills, J. A., Jost, J. T., Tucker, J. A., Van Bavel, J. J., & Fiske, S. T. (2017). Emotion shapes the diffusion of moralized content in social networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114, 73137318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bramwell, K., & Richardson, T. (2018). Improvements in depression and mental health after acceptance and commitment therapy are related to changes in defusion and values-based action. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 48, 914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandtzæg, P. B., & Heim, J. (2009). Why people use social networking sites. In Lecture Notes in Computer Science (including subseries Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence and Lecture Notes in Bioinformatics), vol. 5621, pp. 143152. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg.Google Scholar
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18, 211237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruguera, C., Guitert, M., & Romeu, T. (2019). Social media and professional development: a systematic review. Research in Learning Technology, 27.Google Scholar
Caplan, S. E. (2005). A social skill account of problematic internet use. Journal of Communication, 55, 721736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cardell, K., Douglas, K., & Maguire, E. (2017). ‘Stories’: social media and ephemeral narratives as memoir. In Mediating Memory: Tracing the Limits of Memoir. Routledge Interdisciplinary Perspectives on Literature, pp. 157172, edited by Avieson, B., & Giles, F. J. S. New York, USA: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S. (1996). Emergent integration in contemporary personality psychology. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 319334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Newman, D. L., & Silva, P. A. (1996). Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders: longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 1033–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Çelik, Ç. B., & Odaci, H. (2013). The Relationship between problematic internet use and interpersonal cognitive distortions and life satisfaction in university students. Children and Youth Services Review, 35, 505508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, R. A., Taylor, J. R., & Potenza, M. N. (2003). Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 10411052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charoensukmongkol, P. (2016). Mindful Facebooking: the moderating role of mindfulness on the relationship between social media use intensity at work and burnout. Journal of Health Psychology, 21, 19661980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, C., Lau, Y. C., & Luk, J. W. (2020). Social capital-accrual, escape-from-self, and time-displacement effects of internet use during the COVID-19 stay-at-home period: prospective, quantitative survey study. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 22.Google ScholarPubMed
Cheung, C. M. K., Chiu, P. Y., & Lee, M. K. O. (2011). Online social networks: why do students use Facebook? Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 13371343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, C. M. K., Lee, Z. W. Y., & Lee, M. K. O. (2013). Understanding compulsive use of Facebook through the reinforcement processes. In Proceedings of the 21st European Conference on Information Systems, pp. 1–12.Google Scholar
Chohan, U. W., & D’Souza, A. (2020). The joys and ills of social media: a review. SSRN Electronic Journal.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ciampaglia, G. L., Nematzadeh, A., Menczer, F., & Flammini, A. (2018). How algorithmic popularity bias hinders or promotes quality. Scientific Reports, 8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ciranka, S., & van den Bos, W. (2019). Social influence in adolescent decision-making: a formal framework. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, J. L., Algoe, S. B., & Green, M. C. (2018). Social network sites and well-being: the role of social connection. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 3237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Contractor, A. A., Elhai, J. D., Ractliffe, K. C., & Forbes, D. (2013). PTSD’s underlying symptom dimensions and relations with behavioral inhibition and activation. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27, 645651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockett, M. J. (2017). Moral outrage in the digital age. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 769771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crone, E. A., & Konijn, E. A. (2018). Media use and brain development during adolescence. Nature Communications, 9, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Culnan, M. J., & Markus, M. (1987). Information technologies. In Handbook of Organizational Communication: An Interdisciplinary Perspective, pp. 420443, edited by Jablin, F. M., Putnam, L. L., Roberts, K. H., & Porter, L. W. Thousand Oaks, CA, USA: Sage.Google Scholar
Daneels, R., & Vanwynsberghe, H. (2017). Mediating social media use: connecting parents’ mediation strategies and social media literacy. Cyberpsychology, 11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
David, D., Miclea, M. & Opre, A. (2004). The information-processing approach to the human mind: basics and beyond. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 60, 353368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, R. A. (2001). Cognitive-behavioral model of pathological internet use. Computers in Human Behavior, 17, 187195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Department for Education (2019). Teaching Online Safety in School: Guidance Supporting Schools to Teach Their Pupils How to Stay Safe Online, Within New and Existing School Subjects.Google Scholar
Derakhshan, N. (2020). Attentional control and cognitive biases as determinants of vulnerability and resilience in anxiety and depression. In Cognitive Biases in Health and Psychiatric Disorders, pp. 261274. Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Division of Clinical Psychology (2011). Good Practice Guidelines on the Use of Psychological Formulation. London, UK.Google Scholar
Doherty-Torstrick, E. R., Walton, K. E., & Fallon, B. A. (2016). Cyberchondria: parsing health anxiety from online behavior. Psychosomatics, 57, 390400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubicka, B., & Theodosiou, L. (2020). Technology Use and the Mental Health of Children and Young People.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
El Asam, A., & Katz, A. (2018). Vulnerable young people and their experience of online risks. Human–Computer Interaction, 33, 281304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook ‘friends:’ social capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12, 11431168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and Crisis. New York, USA: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Erreygers, S., Vandebosch, H., Vranjes, I., Baillien, E., & De Witte, H. (2017). Nice or naughty? The role of emotions and digital media use in explaining adolescents’ online prosocial and antisocial behavior. Media Psychology, 20, 374400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erreygers, S., Vandebosch, H., Vranjes, I., Baillien, B., & De Witte, H. (2019). Feel good, do good online? Spillover and crossover effects of happiness on adolescents’ online prosocial behavior. Journal of Happiness Studies, 20, 12411258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eshraghian, F., & Hafezieh, N. (2017). Affordance theory in social media research: systematic review and synthesis of the literature. In 25th European Conference on Information Systems (ECIS), pp. 1–12.Google Scholar
Eşkisu, M., Çam, Z., Gelibolu, S., & Rasmussen, K. R. (2020). Trait mindfulness as a protective factor in connections between psychological issues and Facebook addiction among Turkish university students. Studia Psychologica, 62, 213–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Exelmans, L., & Van den Bulck, J. (2016). The use of media as a sleep aid in adults. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 14, 121133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eyal, N., & Hoover, R. (2014). Hooked: How to Build Habit-Forming Products. New York, USA: Penguin.Google Scholar
Fardouly, J., Pinkus, R. T., & Vartanian, L. R. (2017). The impact of appearance comparisons made through social media, traditional media, and in person in women’s everyday lives. Body Image, 20, 3139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennell, M. J. V. (1998). Cognitive therapy in the treatment of low self-esteem. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 4, 296304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández-Rodríguez, C., Paz-Caballero, D., González-Fernández, S., & Pérez-Álvarez, M. (2018). Activation vs. experiential avoidance as a transdiagnostic condition of emotional distress: an empirical study. Frontiers in Psychology, 9.Google ScholarPubMed
Ferster, C. B. (1973). A functional analysis of depression. The American Psychologist, 28, 857–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fordham, B., Sugavanam, T., Edwards, K., Stallard, P., Howard, R., Nair, R. D., Copsey, B., Lee, H., Howick, J., Hemming, K., & Lamb, S. E. (2021). The evidence for cognitive behavioural therapy in any condition, population or context: a meta-review of systematic reviews and panoramic meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 51, 2129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2016). Exploring the relationships between different types of Facebook use, perceived online social support, and adolescents’ depressed mood. Social Science Computer Review, 34, 153–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frison, E., and Eggermont, S. (2017). Browsing, posting, and liking on Instagram: the reciprocal relationships between different types of Instagram use and adolescents’ depressed mood. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 20, 603609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gable, S. L. (2006). Approach and avoidance social motives and goals. Journal of Personality, 74, 175222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Behavioral activation and inhibition in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1135–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García-Oliva, C., & Piqueras, J. A. (2016). Experiential avoidance and technological addictions in adolescents. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5, 293303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, M., & Steinberg, L. (2005). Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 41, 625635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gazelle, H., & Rudolph, K. D. (2004). Moving toward and away from the world: social approach and avoidance trajectories in anxious solitary youth. Child Development, 75, 829849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ge, X. (2020). Social media reduce users’ moral sensitivity: online shaming as a possible consequence. Aggressive Behavior, ab.21904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, M. (2019). The importance of social media content for teens’ risks for self-harm. Journal of Adolescent Health, 65, 910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
George, M. J., Jensen, M. R., Russell, M. A., Gassman-Pines, A., Copeland, W. E., Hoyle, R. H., & Odgers, C. L. (2020). Young adolescents’ digital technology use, perceived impairments, and well-being in a representative sample. Journal of Pediatrics, 219, 180–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
GlobalWebIndex (2020). Social – GlobalWebIndex’s Flagship Report on the Latest Trends in Social Media.Google Scholar
Goldenberg, A., & Gross, J. J. (2020). Digital emotion contagion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24, 316328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonen, S., & Aharony, N. (2017). Relations between privacy behaviors and social capital on Facebook. Libri, 67, 103–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzales, A. L., & Hancock, J. T. (2011). Mirror, mirror on my Facebook wall: effects of exposure to Facebook on self-esteem. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 7983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, A. M., Mendes, W. B., & Prather, A. A. (2017). The social side of sleep: elucidating the links between sleep and social processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 470475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granic, I., Morita, H., & Scholten, H. (2020). Beyond screen time: identity development in the digital age. Psychological Inquiry, 31, 195223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, S. S., Huskey, A. M., Faunce, J. A., & Friedman, B. H. (2020). Negative memory biases in health and psychiatric disorders. In Cognitive Biases in Health and Psychiatric Disorders, pp. 173191. Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, L., Fry, A. F., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: a life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 3336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
große Deters, F., & Mehl, M. R. (2013). Does posting Facebook status updates increase or decrease loneliness? An online social networking experiment. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 579586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guo, Y., Li, Y., & Ito, N. (2014). Exploring the predicted effect of social networking site use on perceived social capital and psychological well-being of Chinese international students in Japan. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17, 5258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, J. A., Johnson, R. M., & Ross, E. M. (2019a). Where does the time go? An experimental test of what social media displaces and displaced activities’ associations with affective well-being and quality of day. New Media & Society, 21, 674692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, J. A., Kearney, M. W., & Xing, C. (2019b). Two tests of social displacement through social media use. Information, Communication & Society, 22, 13961413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harman, J. P., Hansen, C. E., Cochran, M. E., & Lindsey, C. R. (2005). Liar, liar: internet faking but not frequency of use affects social skills, self-esteem, social anxiety, and aggression. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 8, 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, M. A., & Orth, U. (2020). The link between self-esteem and social relationships: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 119, 1459–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004a). Attention. In Cognitive Behavioural Processes Across Psychological Disorders: A Transdiagnostic Approach to Research and Treatment, pp. 2572. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004b). Behaviour. In Cognitive Behavioural Processes Across Psychological Disorders: A Transdiagnostic Approach to Research and Treatment, pp. 239269. New York, USA: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An Experiential Approach to Behavior Change. New York, USA: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2003). Valuing. In Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An Eperiential Approach to Behaviour Change, pp. 204234. New York, USA: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. (2016a). Analytic goals and the varieties of scientific contextualism. In The Act in Context – The Canonical Papers of Steven C. Hayes, pp. 100116. New York, USA: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. (2016b). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: a functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. In The Act in Context – The Canonical Papers of Steven C. Hayes, pp. 161171. in THE ACT IN CONTEXT - The Canonical Papers of Steven C. Hayes. New York, USA: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Heppner, W. L., Kernis, M. H., Lakey, C. E., Campbell, W. K., Goldman, B. M., Davis, P. J., & Cascio, E. V. (2008). Mindfulness as a means of reducing aggressive behavior: dispositional and situational evidence. Aggressive Behavior, 34, 486496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, M. (2018). Panorama – Smartphones: The Dark Side.Google Scholar
Hogue, J. V., & Mills, J. S. (2019). The effects of active social media engagement with peers on body image in young women. Body Image, 28, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holland, G., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). A systematic review of the impact of the use of social networking sites on body image and disordered eating outcomes. Body Image, 17, 100110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollis, C., Livingstone, S., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2020). Editorial: The role of digital technology in children and young people’s mental health – a triple-edged sword? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61, 837–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houlihan, S. D., & Brewer, J. A. (2016). The emerging science of mindfulness as a treatment for addiction. In Mindfulness and Buddhist-Derived Approaches in Mental Health and Addiction, pp. 191210, edited by Shonin, E., Van Gordon, W., & Griffiths, M. D. New York, USA: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, M. G., Marx, R., Lipson, C., & Young, J. (2018). No more FOMO: limiting social media decreases loneliness and depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 37, 751768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hussain, D. (2015). Meta-cognition in mindfulness: a conceptual analysis. Psychological Thought, 8, 132–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Indian, M., & Grieve, R. (2014). When Facebook is easier than face-to-face: social support derived from Facebook in socially anxious individuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 59, 102106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiménez Chafey, M. I., Bernal, G., & Rosselló, J. (2009). Clinical case study: CBT for depression in a Puerto Rican adolescent: challenges and variability in treatment response. Depression and Anxiety, 26, 98103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S. B., Blum, R. W., & Giedd, J. N. (2009). Adolescent maturity and the brain: the promise and pitfalls of neuroscience research in adolescent health policy. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45, 216–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jopling, E., Wilson, J., Burke, M., Tracy, A., & LeMoult, J. (2020). Positive interpretation bias across the psychiatric disorders. In Cognitive Biases in Health and Psychiatric Disorders, pp. 99117. Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, A. M., & Haenlein, M. (2010). Users of the world, unite! The challenges and opportunities of social media. Business Horizons, 53, 5968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karahanna, E., Xu, S. X., Xu, Y., & Zhang, N. (2018). The needs-affordances-features perspective for the use of social media. MIS Quarterly: Management Information Systems, 42, 737756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasch, D. (2013). Social Media Selves: College Students’ Curation of Self and Others Through Facebook. UCLA Electronic Theses and Dissertations.Google Scholar
Katsumata, Y., Matsumoto, T., Kitani, M., & Takeshima, T. (2008). Electronic media use and suicidal ideation in Japanese adolescents. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 62, 744746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M. (1973). Uses and gratifications research. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 37, 509–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaylor, B. (2019). Likes, retweets, and polarization. Review & Expositor, 116, 183192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keles, B., McCrae, N., & Grealish, A. (2019). A systematic review: the influence of social media on depression, anxiety and psychological distress in adolescents. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 25, 7993.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, Y., Zilanawala, A., Booker, C., & Sacker, A. (2018). Social media use and adolescent mental health: findings from the UK Millennium Cohort Study. EClinicalMedicine, 6, 5968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keresteš, G., & Štulhofer, A. (2020). Adolescents’ online social network use and life satisfaction: a latent growth curve modeling approach. Computers in Human Behavior, 104, 106187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Amminger, G. P., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Lee, S., & Ustün, T. B. (2007). Age of onset of mental disorders: a review of recent literature. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20, 359364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khosravi, P., Rezvani, A., & Wiewiora, A. (2016). The impact of technology on older adults’ social isolation. Computers in Human Behavior, 63, 594603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiken, L. G., Garland, E. L., Bluth, K., Palsson, O. S., & Gaylord, S. A. (2015). From a state to a trait: trajectories of state mindfulness in meditation during intervention predict changes in trait mindfulness. Personality and Individual Differences, 81, 4146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiken, L. G., & Shook, N. J. (2011). Looking up: mindfulness increases positive judgments and reduces negativity bias. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 425–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, J., & Lee, J. E. R. (2011). The Facebook paths to happiness: effects of the number of Facebook friends and self-presentation on subjective well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 359–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimbrel, N. A., Mitchell, J. T., & Nelson-Gray, R. O. (2010). An examination of the relationship between behavioral approach system (BAS) sensitivity and social interaction anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 372378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kircaburun, K., Alhabash, S., Tosuntaş, S. B., & Griffiths, M. D. (2020). Uses and gratifications of problematic social media use among university students: a simultaneous examination of the big five of personality traits, social media platforms, and social media use motives. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 18, 525–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, A. M., de Voogd, L., Wiers, R. W., & a Salemink, E. (2018). Biases in attention and interpretation in adolescents with varying levels of anxiety and depression. Cognition and Emotion, 32, 14781486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kozyreva, A., Lewandowsky, S., & Hertwig, R. (2020). Citizens versus the internet: confronting digital challenges with cognitive tools. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 21, 103156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krämer, N. C., Lucas, G., Schmitt, L., & Gratch, J. (2018). Social snacking with a virtual agent – on the interrelation of need to belong and effects of social responsiveness when interacting with artificial entities. International Journal of Human Computer Studies, 109, 112121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krasnova, H., Widjaja, T., Buxmann, P., Wenninger, H., & Benbasat, I. (2015). Why following friends can hurt you: an exploratory investigation of the effects of envy on social networking sites among college-age users. Information Systems Research, 26, 585605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Sheppes, G., Costello, C. K., Jonides, J., & Ybarra, O. (2021). Social media and well-being: pitfalls, progress, and next steps. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25, 5566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuss, D. J., & Lopez-Fernandez, O. (2016). Internet addiction and problematic internet use: a systematic review of clinical research. World Journal of Psychiatry, 6, 143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahat, A., Pérez-Edgar, K., Degnan, K. A., Guyer, A. E., Lejuez, C. W., Ernst, M., Pine, D. S., & Fox, N. A. (2012). Early childhood temperament predicts substance use in young adults. Translational Psychiatry, 2, e157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakey, C. E., Kernis, M. H., Heppner, W. L., & Lance, C. E. (2008). Individual differences in authenticity and mindfulness as predictors of verbal defensiveness. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 230238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lapidot-Lefler, N., & Barak, A. (2012). Effects of anonymity, invisibility, and lack of eye-contact on toxic online disinhibition. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 434443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaRose, R. (2010). The problem of media habits. Communication Theory, 20, 194222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaRose, R., & Eastin, M. S. (2004). A social cognitive theory of internet uses and gratifications: toward a new model of media attendance. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 48, 358–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaRose, R., Lin, C. A., & Eastin, M. S. (2003). Unregulated internet usage: addiction, habit, or deficient self-regulation? Media Psychology, 5, 225253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lev-On, A. (2017). Uses and gratifications: evidence for various media. In The International Encyclopedia of Media Effects, pp. 19. Wiley Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lewinsohn, P. M. (1975). Engagement in pleasant activities and depression level. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84, 729731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, P. (2017). ‘Our minds can be hijacked’: the tech insiders who fear a smartphone dystopia. Guardian – Weekend Magazine Technology Special (October).Google Scholar
Lindström, B., Bellander, M., Schultner, D. T., Chang, A., Tobler, P. N., & Amodio, D. M. (2021). A computational reward learning account of social media engagement. Nature Communications, 12.Google ScholarPubMed
Lister, K. M. (2007). Aggression and prosocial behavior in adolescents’ internet and face-to-face interactions. Bowling Green State University.Google Scholar
Liu, D., Ainsworth, S. E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2016). A meta-analysis of social networking online and social capital. Review of General Psychology, 20, 369391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Q.-Q., Zhou, Z.-K., Yang, X.-J., Niu, G.-F., Tian, Y., & Fan, C.-Y. (2017a). Upward social comparison on social network sites and depressive symptoms: a moderated mediation model of self-esteem and optimism. Personality and Individual Differences, 113, 223228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Q. Q., Zhou, Z. K., Yang, X. J., Kong, F. C., Niu, G. F., & Fan, C. Y. (2017b). Mobile phone addiction and sleep quality among Chinese adolescents: a moderated mediation model. Computers in Human Behavior, 72, 108114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livingstone, S., & Helsper, E. (2010). Balancing opportunities and risks in teenagers’ use of the internet: the role of online skills and internet self-efficacy. New Media and Society, 12, 309329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd, A., McKay, R., Sebastian, C., & Balsters, J. (2020). Are adolescents more optimal decision-makers in novel environments? Examining exploration and learning in a patch foraging paradigm. Developmental Science, 24, e13075.Google Scholar
Lomanowska, A. M., & Guitton, M. J. (2016). Online intimacy and well-being in the digital age. Internet Interventions, 4, 138–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luxton, D. D., June, J. D., & Fairall, J. M. (2012). Social media and suicide: a public health perspective. American Journal of Public Health, 102 (suppl 2), S195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madden, M., Lenhart, A., Cortesi, S., Gasser, U., Duggan, M., & Smith, A. (2013). Teens, Social Media, and Privacy. Washington, DC, USA.Google Scholar
Malloy-Diniz, L. F., Miranda, D. M., & Grassi-Oliveira, R. (2017). Editorial: Executive functions in psychiatric disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marangunić, N., & Granić, A. (2015). Technology acceptance model: a literature review from 1986 to 2013. Universal Access in the Information Society, 14, 8195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marengo, D., Longobardi, C., Fabris, M. A., & Settanni, M. (2018). Highly-visual social media and internalizing symptoms in adolescence: the mediating role of body image concerns. Computers in Human Behavior, 82, 6369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marwick, A. E., & Boyd, D. (2011). I tweet honestly, I tweet passionately: twitter users, context collapse, and the imagined audience. New Media & Society, 13, 114133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, B., Polak, M. G., & Remmerswaal, D. (2019). Mindfulness, interpretation bias, and levels of anxiety and depression: two mediation studies. Mindfulness, 10, 5565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazurek, M. O. (2013). Social media use among adults with autism spectrum disorders. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 17091714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, N., Gettings, S., & Purssell, E. (2017). Social media and depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review. Adolescent Research Review, 2, 315–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonough, P., Duncan, G. J., Williams, D., & House, J. (1997). Income dynamics and adult mortality in the United States, 1972 through 1989. American Journal of Public Health, 87, 14761483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McFarland, L. A., & Ployhart, R. E. (2015). Social media: a contextual framework to guide research and practice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 16531677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMullan, R. D., Berle, D., Arnáez, S., & Starcevic, V. (2019). The relationships between health anxiety, online health information seeking, and cyberchondria: systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 245, 270–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meier, A., Gilbert, A., Börner, S., & Possler, D. (2020). Instagram inspiration: how upward comparison on social network sites can contribute to well-being. Journal of Communication, 70, 721743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, A., & Reinecke, L. (2020). Computer-mediated communication, social media, and mental health: a conceptual and empirical meta-review. Communication Research, 009365022095822.Google Scholar
Memon, A. M., Sharma, S. G., Mohite, S. S., & Jain, S. (2018). The role of online social networking on deliberate self-harm and suicidality in adolescents: a systematized review of literature. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 60, 384392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menting, B., Van Lier, P. A. C., Koot, H. M., Pardini, D., & Loeber, R. (2015). Cognitive impulsivity and the development of delinquency from late childhood to early adulthood: moderating effects of parenting behavior and peer relationships. Development and Psychopathology, 28, 167183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michikyan, M., Subrahmanyam, K., & Dennis, J. (2014). Can you tell who I am? Neuroticism, extraversion, and online self-presentation among young adults. Computers in Human Behavior, 33, 179–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Modecki, K. L., Minchin, J., Harbaugh, A. G., Guerra, N. G., & Runions, K. C. (2014). Bullying prevalence across contexts: a meta-analysis measuring cyber and traditional bullying. Journal of Adolescent Health, 55, 602611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Modin, B., Östberg, V., & Almquist, Y. (2011). Childhood peer status and adult susceptibility to anxiety and depression. A 30-year hospital follow-up. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 187199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moorhead, S. A., Hazlett, D. E., Harrison, L., Carroll, J. K., Irwin, A., & Hoving, C. (2013). A new dimension of health care: systematic review of the uses, benefits, and limitations of social media for health communication. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 15, e85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreno, M. A., & Uhls, Y. T. (2019). Applying an affordances approach and a developmental lens to approach adolescent social media use. Digital Health, 5, 205520761982667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mosquera, R., Odunowo, M., McNamara, T., Guo, X., & Petrie, R. (2020). The economic effects of Facebook. Experimental Economics, 23, 575602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nadkarni, A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2012). Why do people use Facebook? Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 243249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naslund, J. A., Aschbrenner, K. A., Marsch, L. A., & Bartels, S. J. (2016). The future of mental health care: peer-to-peer support and social media. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 25, 113122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naslund, J. A., Grande, S. W., Aschbrenner, K. A., & Elwyn, G. (2014). Naturally occurring peer support through social media: the experiences of individuals with severe mental illness using YouTube edited by L. De Haan. PLoS One, 9, e110171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nesi, J., Choukas-Bradley, S., & Prinstein, M. J. (2018a). Transformation of adolescent peer relations in the social media context: Part 1 – a theoretical framework and application to dyadic peer relationships. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 21, 267294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesi, J., Choukas-Bradley, S., & Prinstein, M. J. (2018b). Transformation of adolescent peer relations in the social media context: Part 2 – application to peer group processes and future directions for research. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 21, 295319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesi, J., & Prinstein, M. J. (2015). Using social media for social comparison and feedback-seeking: gender and popularity moderate associations with depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 14271438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesi, J., Telzer, E. H., & Prinstein, M. J. (2020). Adolescent development in the digital media context. Psychological Inquiry, 31, 229234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ngai, E. W. T., Tao, S. S. C., & Moon, K. K. L. (2015). Social media research: theories, constructs, and conceptual frameworks. International Journal of Information Management, 35, 3344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
NHS Digital (2017. Mental Health of Children and Young People in England.Google Scholar
Nielsen, R. K., & Schrøder, K. C. (2014). The relative importance of social media for accessing, finding, and engaging with news. Digital Journalism, 2, 472489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nikitin, J., & Freund, A. M. (2008). The role of social approach and avoidance motives for subjective well-being and the successful transition to adulthood. Applied Psychology, 57 (suppl 1), 90111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niland, P., Lyons, A. C., Goodwin, I., & Hutton, F. (2015). Friendship work on Facebook: young adults’ understandings and practices of friendship. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 25, 123–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noë, B., Turner, L. D., Linden, D. E. J., Allen, S. M., Winkens, B., & Whitaker, R. M. (2019). Identifying indicators of smartphone addiction through user-app interaction. Computers in Human Behavior, 99, 5665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orben, A. (2018). Cyberpsychology: a field lacking theoretical foundations. PsyPAG The Quarterly, 105 (open access version).Google Scholar
Orben, A. (2020a). Teenagers, screens and social media: a narrative review of reviews and key studies. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 55, 407414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orben, A. (2020b). The Sisyphean cycle of technology panics. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 174569162091937.Google Scholar
Orben, A., Dienlin, T., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). Social media’s enduring effect on adolescent life satisfaction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116, 1022610228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orben, A., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). Screens, teens, and psychological well-being: evidence from three time-use-diary studies. Psychological Science, 30, 682696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orben, A., & Przybylski, A. K. (2020a). Reply to: Underestimating digital media harm. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 349351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orben, A., & Przybylski, A. K. (2020b). Teenage sleep and technology engagement across the week. PeerJ, 8, e8427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orben, A., Tomova, L., & Blakemore, S. J. (2020a). The effects of social deprivation on adolescent development and mental health. The Lancet Child and Adolescent Health, 4, 634640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orben, A., Weinstein, N., & Przybylski, A. K. (2020b). Only holistic and iterative change will fix digital technology research. Psychological Inquiry, 31, 235241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, B., Heisterkamp, B., Halfmann, A., & Vorderer, P. (2018). Trait mindfulness and problematic smartphone use. In Empirical Studies of Contemplative Practices, pp. 181205.Google Scholar
Palmgreen, P. (1984). Uses and gratifications: a theoretical perspective. Annals of the International Communication Association, 8, 2055.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmgreen, P., Wenner, L., & Rosengren, K. (1985). Uses and gratifications research: the past ten years. In Media Gratification Research, pp. 1137, edited by Rosengren, K., Wenner, L., & Palmgreen, P. Beverly Hills, CA, USA: SAGE Publications Ltd.Google Scholar
Papacharissi, Z., & Rubin, A. M. (2000). Predictors of internet use. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 44, 175196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, S. Y., & Baek, Y. M. (2018). Two faces of social comparison on Facebook: the interplay between social comparison orientation, emotions, and psychological well-being. Computers in Human Behavior, 79, 8393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perloff, R. M. (2014). Social media effects on young women’s body image concerns: theoretical perspectives and an agenda for research. Sex Roles, 71, 363–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perugini, M. L. L., & Solano, A. C. (2021). Normal and maladaptive personality traits as predictors of motives for social media use and its effects on well-being. Psychological Reports, 124, 10701092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poon, K. T., & Jiang, Y. (2020). Getting less likes on social media: mindfulness ameliorates the detrimental effects of feeling left out online. Mindfulness, 11, 10381048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., Dehaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013). Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 18411848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, R. D. (2001). Bowling Alone: The Collapse and Revival of American Community. Simon & Schuster Ltd.Google Scholar
Radovic, A., Gmelin, T., Stein, B. D., & Miller, E. (2017). Depressed adolescents’ positive and negative use of social media. Journal of Adolescence, 55, 515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ragelienė, T. (2016). Links of adolescent identity development and relationship with peers: a systematic literature review. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 25, 97105.Google ScholarPubMed
Ramel, W., Goldin, P. R., Carmona, P. E., & McQuaid, J. R. (2004). The effects of mindfulness meditation on cognitive processes and affect in patients with past depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28, 433–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramnero, J., & Torneke, N. (2008). Knowing your ABCs. In The ABCs of Human Behavior – Behavioral Principles for the Practicing Clinician, pp. 4759. Oakland, CA, USA: New Harbinger.Google Scholar
Reichelt, A. (2019). Social media and mental health: uses and gratifications of social networking in relation to depression, anxiety and offline relationships among young adults. University of Twente.Google Scholar
Riskind, J. H. (1997). Looming vulnerability to threat: a cognitive paradigm for anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 685702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riva, F., Triscoli, C., Lamm, C., Carnaghi, A., & Silani, G. (2016). Emotional egocentricity bias across the life-span. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8, 74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizvi, S. L. (2019). The basics of the chain analysis. In Chain Analysis in Dialectical Behavior Therapy, pp. 122. New York, USA: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Romer, D. (2010). Adolescent risk taking, impulsivity, and brain development: implications for prevention. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 263276.Google ScholarPubMed
Romer, D., & Hennessy, M. (2007). A biosocial-affect model of adolescent sensation seeking: the role of affect evaluation and peer-group influence in adolescent drug use. Prevention Science, 8, 89101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romer, D., Reyna, V. F., & Satterthwaite, T. D. (2017). Beyond stereotypes of adolescent risk taking: placing the adolescent brain in developmental context. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 1934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, A. J. (2002). Co-rumination in the friendships of girls and boys. Child Development, 73, 18301843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 296320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruggiero, T. E. (2000). Uses and gratifications theory in the 21st century. Mass Communication and Society, 3, 337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Basic psychological needs theory: satisfaction and frustration of autonomy, competence, and relatedness in relation to psychological wellness and full functioning. In Self Determination Theory – Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation, Development, and Wellness, pp. 239–71. Guilford Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiphoo, A. N., & Vahedi, Z. (2019). A meta-analytic review of the relationship between social media use and body image disturbance. Computers in Human Behavior, 101, 259275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salkovskis, P. M. (1991). The importance of behaviour in the maintenance of anxiety and panic: a cognitive account. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 19, 619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sawyer, S. M., Azzopardi, P. S., Wickremarathne, D., & Patton, G. C. (2018). The age of adolescence. The Lancet Child and Adolescent Health, 2, 223228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmertz, S. K., Masuda, A., & Anderson, P. L. (2012). Cognitive processes mediate the relation between mindfulness and social anxiety within a clinical sample. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68, 362–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmuck, D., Karsay, K., Matthes, J., & Stevic, A. (2019). ‘Looking up and feeling down’. The influence of mobile social networking site use on upward social comparison, self-esteem, and well-being of adult smartphone users. Telematics and Informatics, 42, 101240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schønning, V., Hjetland, G. J., Aarø, L. E., & Skogen, J. C. (2020). Social media use and mental health and well-being among adolescents – a scoping review. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schreurs, L., & Vandenbosch, L. (2021). Introducing the Social Media Literacy (SMILE) model with the case of the positivity bias on social media. Journal of Children and Media, 15, 320–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuman-Olivier, Z., Trombka, M., Lovas, D. A., Brewer, J. A., Vago, D. R., Gawande, R., Dunne, J. P., Lazar, S. W., Loucks, E. B., & Fulwiler, C. (2020). Mindfulness and Behavior Change. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 28, 371394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, H., Biello, S. M., & Woods, H. C. (2019). Social media use and adolescent sleep patterns: cross-sectional findings from the UK Millennium Cohort Study. BMJ Open, 9, e031161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebastian, C., Viding, E., Williams, K. D., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence. Brain and Cognition, 72, 134145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seo, M., Kim, J., & Yang, H. (2016). Frequent interaction and fast feedback predict perceived social support: using crawled and self-reported data of Facebook users. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 21, 282297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serrano-Ibáñez, E. R., Ramírez-Maestre, C., Esteve, R., & López-Martínez, A. E. (2019). The behavioural inhibition system, behavioural activation system and experiential avoidance as explanatory variables of comorbid chronic pain and posttraumatic stress symptoms. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 10.Google ScholarPubMed
Shaw, A. M., Timpano, K. R., Tran, T. B., & Joormann, J. (2015). Correlates of Facebook usage patterns: the relationship between passive Facebook use, social anxiety symptoms, and brooding. Computers in Human Behavior, 48, 575–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, H., Ellis, D. A., & Ziegler, F. V. (2018). The Technology Integration Model (TIM). predicting the continued use of technology. Computers in Human Behavior, 83, 204214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheldon, K. M., Abad, N., & Hinsch, C. (2011). A two-process view of Facebook use and relatedness need-satisfaction: disconnection drives use, and connection rewards it. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1, 215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheldon, P., Rauschnabel, P. A., Honeycutt, J. M. (2020). Section 1: The dark side of personal social media use. In The Dark Side of Social Media Use – Psychological, Managerial, and Societal Perspectives, pp. 385. London, UK: Academic Press, Elsevier.Google Scholar
Shepherd, J. L., Lane, D. J., Tapscott, R. L., & Gentile, D. A. (2011). Susceptible to social influence: risky ‘driving’ in response to peer pressure. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 41, 773797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sidani, J. E., Shensa, A., Hoffman, B., Hanmer, J., & Primack, B. A. (2016). The association between social media use and eating concerns among US young adults. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 116, 14651472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Šmahel, K., & Subrahmanyam, D. (2011). Connecting online behavior to adolescent development: a theoretical framework. In Digital Youth – The Role of Media in Development, pp. 2740. New York, USA: Springer.Google Scholar
Spear, L. P. (2011). Rewards, aversions and affect in adolescence: emerging convergences across laboratory animal and human data. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 390403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spies Shapiro, L. A., & Margolin, G. (2014). Growing up wired: social networking sites and adolescent psychosocial development. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 17, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spohr, D. (2017). Fake news and ideological polarization. Business Information Review, 34, 150–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sportel, B. E., Nauta, M. H., de Hullu, E., de Jong, P. J., & Hartman, C. A. (2011). Behavioral inhibition and attentional control in adolescents: robust relationships with anxiety and depression. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 20, 149156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Subrahmanyam, K., & Šmahel, D. (2011a). Constructing identity online: identity exploration and self-presentation. In Digital Youth – The Role of Media in Development, pp. 5980. New York, USA: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Subrahmanyam, K., & Šmahel, D. (2011b). Intimacy and the internet: relationships with friends, romantic partners, and family members. In Digital Youth – The Role of Media in Development, pp. 81102. New York, USA: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Subrahmanyam, K., & Šmahel, D. (2011c). Technology and health: using the internet for wellness and illness. In Digital Youth – The Role of Media in Development, pp. 143155. New York, USA: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Subrahmanyam, K., & Šmahel, D. (2011d). The darker sides of the internet: violence, cyber bullying, and victimization. In Digital Youth – The Role of Media in Development, pp. 179200. New York, USA: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Subrahmanyam, K., & Šmahel, D. (2011e). Connecting online behavior to adolescent development: a theoretical framework. In Digital Youth – The Role of Media in Development, pp. 2740. New York, USA: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suler, J. (2004). The online disinhibition effect. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 7, 321–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sussman, T. J., Jin, J., & Mohanty, A. (2020). The impact of top-down factors on threat perception biases in health and anxiety. In Cognitive Biases in Health and Psychiatric Disorders, pp. 215241. Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tandoc, E. C., Ferrucci, P. & Duffy, M. (2015). Facebook use, envy, and depression among college students: is Facebooking depressing? Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 139–46.Google Scholar
Tang, Q., Gu, B., & Whinston, A. (2012). Content contribution for revenue sharing and reputation in social media: a dynamic structural model. Journal of Management Information Systems, 29, 4176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tarokh, L., Saletin, J., M., & Carskadon, M. A. (2016). Sleep in adolescence: physiology, cognition and mental health. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 70, 182188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tavernier, R., & Willoughby, T. (2014). Sleep problems: predictor or outcome of media use among emerging adults at university? Journal of Sleep Research, 23, 389396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teppers, E., Luyckx, K., Klimstra, T. A. & Goossens, L. (2014). Loneliness and Facebook motives in adolescence: a longitudinal inquiry into directionality of effect. Journal of Adolescence, 37, 691699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tibber, M. S., Zhao, J., & Butler, S. (2020). The association between self-esteem and dimensions and classes of cross-platform social media use in a sample of emerging adults – evidence from regression and latent class analyses. Computers in Human Behavior, 109, 106371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomova, L., Andrews, J. L., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2021. The importance of belonging and the avoidance of social risk taking in adolescence. Developmental Review, 61, 100981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrent, J. (2014). MIL and the Web 3.0. In Global Citizenship in a Digital World, edited by Culver, S. H., & Kerr, P. A., pp. 2730. International Clearinghouse on Children, Youth and Media at NORDICOM.Google Scholar
Tsvetkova, M., & Macy, M. (2015). The contagion of prosocial behavior and the emergence of voluntary-contribution communities. In Social Phenomena: From Data Analysis to Models, pp. 117134. Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Tufekci, Z. (2013). Not this one. American Behavioral Scientist, 57, 848870.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turel, O., & Osatuyi, B. (2017). A peer-influence perspective on compulsive social networking site use: trait mindfulness as a double-edged wword. Computers in Human Behavior, 77, 4753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turel, O., & Qahri-Saremi, H. (2016). Problematic use of social networking sites: antecedents and consequence from a dual-system theory perspective. Journal of Management Information Systems, 33, 10871116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turel, O., & Serenko, A. (2012). The benefits and dangers of enjoyment with social networking websites. European Journal of Information Systems, 21, 512–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2018). Associations between screen time and lower psychological well-being among children and adolescents: evidence from a population-based study. Preventive Medicine Reports, 12, 271283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twenge, J. M., Haidt, J., Joiner, T. E., & Campbell, W. K. (2020). Underestimating digital media harm. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 346348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twenge, J. M., Joiner, T. E., Rogers, M. L., & Martin, G. N. (2018). Increases in depressive symptoms, suicide-related outcomes, and suicide rates among U.S. adolescents after 2010 and links to increased new media screen time. Clinical Psychological Science, 6, 317.Google Scholar
Uhls, Y. T., Ellison, N. B., & Subrahmanyam, K. (2017). Benefits and costs of social media in adolescence. Pediatrics, 140 (suppl 2), S6770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vago, D. R., & David, S. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6.Google ScholarPubMed
Vago, D. R., & Nakamura, Y. (2011). Selective attentional bias towards pain-related threat in fibromyalgia: preliminary evidence for effects of mindfulness meditation training. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 581594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valkenburg, P. M. (2017). Understanding self-effects in social media. Human Communication Research, 43, 477490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valkenburg, P. M., van Driel, I. I., & Beyens, I. (n.d.). The associations of active and passive social media use with well-being: a critical scoping review. PsyArXiv Preprints, 137.Google Scholar
Valkenburg, P. M., Meier, A., & Beyens, I. (n.d.). Social media use and its impact on adolescent mental health: an umbrella review of the evidence. PsyArXiv Preprints.Google Scholar
Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2013). The differential susceptibility to media effects model. Journal of Communication, 63, 221243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2007). Online communication and adolescent well-being: testing the stimulation versus the displacement hypothesis. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12, 11691182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2008). Adolescents’ identity experiments on the internet. Communication Research, 35, 208–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Bavel, J. J., Rathje, S., Harris, E., Robertson, C., & Sternisko, A. (2021). How social media shapes polarization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences.Google Scholar
Van Dam, N. T., Earleywine, M., & Borders, A. (2010). Measuring mindfulness? An item response theory analysis of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 805–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanden Abeele, M. M. P. (2020). Digital wellbeing as a dynamic construct. Communication Theory.Google Scholar
Verduyn, P., Lee, D. S., Park, J., Shablack, H., Orvell, A., Bayer, J., Ybarra, O., Jonides, J., & Kross, E. (2015). Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: experimental and longitudinal evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 480488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verduyn, P., Ybarra, O., Résibois, M., Jonides, J., & Kross, E. (2017). Do social network sites enhance or undermine subjective well-being? A critical review. Social Issues and Policy Review, 11, 274302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verplanken, B., Aarts, H., & Van Knippenberg, A. (1997). Habit, information acquisition, and the process of making travel mode choices. European Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 539–60.3.0.CO;2-A>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viner, R. M., Davie, M., & Firth, A. (2019). The Health Impacts of Screen Time: A Guide for Clinicians and Parents.Google Scholar
Vogel, E. A., Rose, J. P., Okdie, B. M., Eckles, K., & Franz, F. (2015). Who compares and despairs? The effect of social comparison orientation on social media use and its outcomes. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 249256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vogel, E. A., Rose, J. P., Roberts, L. R., Eckles, K. (2014). Social comparison, social media, and self-esteem. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 3, 206222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vosoughi, S., Mohsenvand, M. N., & Roy, D. (2017). Rumor gauge: predicting the veracity of rumors on Twitter. ACM Transactions on Knowledge Discovery from Data, 11, 136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction. Communication Research, 19, 5290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walther, J. B. (2015). Social information processing theory: impressions and relationship development online. In Engaging Theories in Interpersonal Communication – Multiple Perspectives, pp. 573588, edited by Braithwaite, D. O., & Schrodt, P. Los Angeles, CA, USA: SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Wang, J.-L., Wang, H.-Z., Gaskin, J., & Hawk, S. (2017). The mediating roles of upward social comparison and self-esteem and the moderating role of social comparison orientation in the association between social networking site usage and subjective well-being. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Q., Ren, H., Long, J., Liu, Y., & Liu, T. (2019). Research progress and debates on gaming disorder. General Psychiatry, 32.Google ScholarPubMed
Wang, S. S. (2013). ‘I share, therefore I am’: personality traits, life satisfaction, and Facebook check-ins. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16, 870877.Google ScholarPubMed
Wartberg, L., Kriston, L., & Thomasius, R. (2020). Internet gaming disorder and problematic social media use in a representative sample of German adolescents: prevalence estimates, comorbid depressive symptoms and related psychosocial aspects. Computers in Human Behavior, 103, 3136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wegmann, E., & Brand, M. (2019). A narrative overview about psychosocial characteristics as risk factors of a problematic social networks use. Current Addiction Reports, 6, 402409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, A. (1997). Cognitive Therapy of Anxiety Disorders: A Practical Manual and Conceptual Guide. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Wenk-Sormaz, H. (2005). Meditation can reduce habitual responding. Advances in Mind-Body Medicine, 21, 3349.Google ScholarPubMed
Wenninger, H., Krasnova, H., & Buxmann, P. (2014). Activity matters: investigating the influence of Facebook on life satisfaction of teenage users. ECIS 2014 Proceedings.Google Scholar
Williams, J. R. (2019). The use of online social networking sites to nurture and cultivate bonding social capital: a systematic review of the literature from 1997 to 2018. New Media & Society, 21, 27102729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, K. B., Rosenberg, J., Egbert, N., Ploeger, N. A., Bernard, D. R., & King, S. (2013). Communication competence, social support, and depression among college students: a model of Facebook and face-to-face support network influence. Journal of Health Communication, 18, 4157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, C.-C., Holden, S. M., & Ariati, J. (2021). Social media and psychological well-being among youth: the multidimensional model of social media use. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 120.Google ScholarPubMed
Yang, C. C., & Brown, B. B. (2013). Motives for using Facebook, patterns of Facebook activities, and late adolescents’ social adjustment to college. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42, 403416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yonker, L. M., Zan, S., Scirica, C. V., Jethwani, K., & Kinane, T. B. (2015). ‘Friending’ teens: systematic review of social media in adolescent and young adult health care. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 17, e4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, R. (2016). Stress potentiates decision biases: a stress induced deliberation-to-intuition (SIDI) model. Neurobiology of Stress, 3, 8395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuboff, S. (2015). Big other: surveillance capitalism and the prospects of an information civilization. Journal of Information Technology, 30, 7589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zywica, J., & Danowski, J. (2008). The faces of Facebookers: investigating social enhancement and social compensation hypotheses; predicting FacebookTM and offline popularity from sociability and self-esteem, and mapping the meanings of popularity with semantic networks. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 14, 134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. The conceptualisation / model.

Figure 1

Table 1. Ideas for assessment and intervention based on a trans-diagnostic cognitive behavioural conceptualisation of the positive and negative roles of social media use in adolescents’ mental health and wellbeing

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.