Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T14:33:06.458Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

IGEMS: The Consortium on Interplay of Genes and Environment Across Multiple Studies — An Update

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 September 2019

Nancy L. Pedersen*
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Margaret Gatz
Affiliation:
Center for Economic and Social Research, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Brian K. Finch
Affiliation:
Center for Economic and Social Research, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Deborah Finkel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Indiana University Southeast, New Albany, IN, USA
David A. Butler
Affiliation:
Office of Military and Veterans Health, Health and Medicine Division, The National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, Washington, DC, USA
Anna Dahl Aslan
Affiliation:
Institute of Gerontology and Aging Research Network – Jönköping (ARN-J), School of Health and Welfare, Jönköping University, Jönköping, Sweden
Carol E. Franz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Jaakko Kaprio
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Faculty of Medicine & Institute for Molecular Medicine FIMM, HiLIFE, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Susan Lapham
Affiliation:
Research and Evaluation, American Institutes for Research, Washington, DC, USA
Matt McGue
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA Department of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Biodemography, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark
Miriam A. Mosing
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Jenae Neiderhiser
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Penn State University, University Park, PA, USA
Marianne Nygaard
Affiliation:
The Danish Twin Registry, University of Southern Denmark, Odense C, Denmark
Matthew Panizzon
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Carol A. Prescott
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Chandra A. Reynolds
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California – Riverside, Riverside, CA, USA
Perminder Sachdev
Affiliation:
Centre for Healthy Brain Ageing (CHeBA), University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Keith E. Whitfield*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Nancy L. Pedersen, Email: nancy.pedersen@ki.se

Abstract

The Interplay of Genes and Environment across Multiple Studies (IGEMS) is a consortium of 18 twin studies from 5 different countries (Sweden, Denmark, Finland, United States, and Australia) established to explore the nature of gene–environment (GE) interplay in functioning across the adult lifespan. Fifteen of the studies are longitudinal, with follow-up as long as 59 years after baseline. The combined data from over 76,000 participants aged 14–103 at intake (including over 10,000 monozygotic and over 17,000 dizygotic twin pairs) support two primary research emphases: (1) investigation of models of GE interplay of early life adversity, and social factors at micro and macro environmental levels and with diverse outcomes, including mortality, physical functioning and psychological functioning; and (2) improved understanding of risk and protective factors for dementia by incorporating unmeasured and measured genetic factors with a wide range of exposures measured in young adulthood, midlife and later life.

Type
Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2019

Although the association between social context and late-life health and functioning is well established (Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Hawkley, Crawford, Ernst, Burleson, Kowalewski and Berntson2002; Cohen, Reference Cohen2004), the mechanisms for these associations or how social context relates to the biological and genetic factors known to contribute to later life functioning has yet to be fully understood. The advantages of twin studies are the strengthening of causal inference through cotwin control methods (McGue et al., Reference McGue, Osler and Christensen2010), the use of biometric models to quantify genetic and environmental variance (Rijsdijk & Sham, Reference Rijsdijk and Sham2002), studying sex effects by leveraging data from opposite-sex pairs, determining the extent to which associations between risk and outcome are driven by the same genetic or the same environmental influences, and testing whether familial factors (genetic and rearing effects) on the outcome may change as a function of the exposure (van der Sluis et al., Reference van der Sluis, Posthuma and Dolan2012). As an international consortium with harmonized measures of risk, pathway and contextual factors, assessed longitudinally on a large number of twins, the Interplay of Genes and Environment across Multiple Studies (IGEMS) consortium is particularly well suited for investigating the contribution of gene–environment (GE) interplay to functioning in multiple domains across adulthood.

The IGEMS consortium includes more than 76,000 twins from 18 studies representing five countries (Sweden, Denmark, Finland, United States and Australia). The sample spans a wide age range (14–103 years at intake) and has sufficient power to address issues that typically elude most studies. IGEMS also includes a set of well-characterized longitudinal phenotypes, including measures of physical health, cognitive health and emotional health, and measures of multiple facets of adult socioeconomic status (SES; e.g., occupation, education, financial strain), as well as rearing SES, which are harmonized over time and across studies. The twin structure of the dataset permits using established twin methods to test key hypotheses on the nature of GE interplay, while the dense genotyping of a large subset of IGEMS participants allows us to confirm and extend these twin analyses through analyses of polygenic risk scores (PRSs) for health outcomes and for education. Importantly, IGEMS cohorts span multiple countries and historical periods, allowing us to determine whether models of GE interplay established at the micro (i.e., individual) level also apply at the macro (i.e., country and historical period) level.

There are two substantive emphases in the IGEMS consortium: First, socioeconomic conditions are a major social determinant of health (Freedman et al., Reference Freedman, Martin, Schoeni and Cornman2008; Mensah et al., Reference Mensah, Mokdad, Ford, Greenlund and Croft2005; Mirowsky & Ross, Reference Mirowsky and Ross2003; Sattler et al., Reference Sattler, Toro, Schonknecht and Schroder2012; Sharp & Gatz, Reference Sharp and Gatz2011). The oft-cited ‘gradient’ for SES represents the association between health and SES as continuous and monotonic, and not fully explained by poorer health among those who are impoverished (Adler et al., Reference Adler, Boyce, Chesney, Cohen, Folkman, Kahn and Syme1994). Most research focuses on individual-level SES — social status that accrues to occupational classification, education and income, as well as access to social, human and income capital. Others consider the macro-economic environment, such as the extent of social inequality in a country (Kawachi et al., Reference Kawachi, Levine, Miller, Lasch and Amick1994; Lynch et al., Reference Lynch, Smith, Kaplan and House2000, Reference Lynch, Smith, Harper, Hillemeier, Ross, Kaplan and Wolfson2004). IGEMS is unusual in integrating individual- and country-level contributors to health gradients. Further, although both genetic and environmental factors are known to contribute to the SES-health gradient (Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, D’Onofrio and Waldman2009; McGue et al., Reference McGue, Osler and Christensen2010; Rutter, Reference Rutter2009), the mechanisms by which these factors combine to influence health outcomes (GE interplay) are poorly understood.

Recent research has identified alternative models of GE interplay important to understanding health and disease (Boardman et al., Reference Boardman, Daw and Freese2013; Reiss et al., Reference Reiss, Leve and Neiderhiser2013; Shanahan & Boardman, Reference Shanahan, Boardman, Elder and Giele2009; Shanahan & Hofer, Reference Shanahan and Hofer2005). Although these models recognize that individuals inherit differential sensitivity to the environment, they differ in their environmental focus (disease-triggering effects of toxic environments vs. health-promoting benefits of favorable environments) and the expected genetic contribution to disease (maximized in adverse environments, in favorable environments or at both extremes). The differences between models of GE interplay have implications beyond resolving an academic dispute. Environmental improvements would be expected to reduce or eliminate genetically based health disparities under some models (e.g., diathesis-stress) but expand them under others (e.g., social distinction) or have a mixed impact (e.g., differential susceptibility) (Boardman et al., Reference Boardman, Daw and Freese2013; Reiss et al., Reference Reiss, Leve and Neiderhiser2013; Shanahan & Boardman, Reference Shanahan, Boardman, Elder and Giele2009; Shanahan & Hofer, Reference Shanahan and Hofer2005). Understanding whether socially enriched environments compensate for genetic vulnerability or whether they preferentially promote good health among genetically selected individuals, for example, is essential for both translating research into effective prevention strategies and anticipating consequences of social policies.

A second substantive emphasis of the IGEMS consortium is cognitive functioning in adulthood. In particular, Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias (ADRDs), along with mild cognitive impairment, present a major public health challenge due to the large numbers of people affected and lack of a clear path to prevention or cure (Katzman, Reference Katzman2004; Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Sommerlad, Orgeta, Costafreda, Huntley, Ames and Mukadam2017; Wang et al., Reference Wang, MacDonald, Dekhtyar and Fratiglioni2017). While it is generally recognized that Alzheimer’s disease (except for rare dominantly inherited forms) is caused by multiple genetic and environmental factors, it remains unclear how these factors contribute to the disease, whether they function independently or through interactions with each other (Gatz et al., Reference Gatz, Mortimer, Fratiglioni, Johannson, Berg, Andel and Pedersen2007), the ages at which these factors have the greatest impact (Wang et al., Reference Wang, MacDonald, Dekhtyar and Fratiglioni2017), and whether these factors affect men and women equally (Mielke et al., Reference Mielke, Vemuri and Rocca2014). Moreover, it remains to be determined whether many of these factors represent modifiable targets appropriate for intervention, or actually reflect pre-existing genetic vulnerability to ADRD and its risk factors.

IGEMS Studies

From an original consortium of 8 twin studies (Pedersen et al., Reference Pedersen, Christensen, Dahl, Finkel, Franz, Gatz and Reynolds2013), IGEMS has expanded to include 18 studies from 5 countries, representing the strongest available longitudinal twin studies of adulthood and aging in the world. The total sample size is now 76,233, including both members of 10,266 monozygotic (MZ) pairs and 17,288 dizygotic (DZ) pairs, of which 5063 pairs are opposite-sex DZ. The summary below outlines the sampling principles for each study. Numbers of pairs and age ranges at intake are provided in Table 1, as well as the number of waves and length of follow-up, where appropriate. Total Ns refer to individuals and include members of incomplete pairs. Note that updates for several of the studies are included in this issue.

Table 1. Number of twins in each study included in IGEMS, by age at intake

Note: MZ, monozygotic; DZ, dizygotic; OSDZ, opposite sex dizygotic pairs. Total N refers to individuals from both complete and incomplete pairs. Some individuals may have participated in more than one study, for example, in A50 and OATS. The totals in the bottom row count each pair or individual once.

Sweden

Swedish studies are drawn from the population-based Swedish Twin Registry. The Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging (SATSA) began in 1984 (Finkel & Pedersen, Reference Finkel and Pedersen2004). The base population comprises all pairs of twins from the registry who indicated that they had been separated before the age of 11 and reared apart, and a sample of twins reared together matched on the basis of gender, date and county of birth. The OCTO-Twin Study (Origins of Variance in the Old-Old) included twin pairs who were over the age of 80 at baseline in 1991 (McClearn et al., Reference McClearn, Johansson, Berg, Pedersen, Ahern, Petrill and Plomin1997). Aging in Women and Men: A Longitudinal Study of Gender Differences in Health Behaviour and Health among Elderly (GENDER) is a study of opposite-sex twin pairs born between 1906 and 1925 (Gold et al., Reference Gold, Malmberg, McClearn, Pedersen and Berg2002). The Twin and Offspring Study in Sweden (TOSS) includes pairs of same-sex twins and their offspring (Neiderhiser & Lichtenstein, Reference Neiderhiser and Lichtenstein2008). The Study of Dementia in Swedish Twins (HARMONY) was conducted between 1998 and 2004. HARMONY screened all twins age 65 and over in the Screening Across the Lifespan of Twins (SALT) effort (Lichtenstein et al., Reference Lichtenstein, Sullivan, Cnattingius, Gatz, Johansson, Carlstrom and Pedersen2006) and clinically assessed those who screened positive or whose cotwin screened positive for cognitive impairment (Gatz et al., Reference Gatz, Fratiglioni, Johansson, Berg, Mortimer, Reynolds and Pedersen2005).

Denmark

The Longitudinal Study of Aging Danish Twins (LSADT) began in 1995 with the assessment of members of like-sex twin pairs born in Denmark prior to 1920; twins were recruited regardless of whether their cotwin was alive (Christensen et al., Reference Christensen, Holm, McGue, Corder and Vaupel1999). The study of Middle-Aged Danish Twins (MADTs) includes twins ranging in age from 46 to 68 years at the original assessment (Osler et al., Reference Osler, McGue, Lund and Christensen2008). The MId-aged Danish Twin (MIDT) study includes twins representing members of the Danish Twin Registry for the birth years 1931 through 1969 not already participating in MADT.

Finland

The older Finnish Twin Cohort (FTC) study spans four decades; it was initiated in 1975 by contacting all same-sex Finnish twin pairs born before 1958 with both cotwins alive in 1975 (Kaprio & Koskenvuo, Reference Kaprio and Koskenvuo2002). FinnTwin16 (FT16) is a cohort of younger twins born between 1975 and 1979. Waves 4 and 5, when the study participants were in their 20s and 30s, are included in IGEMS (Kaprio et al., Reference Kaprio, Pulkkinen and Rose2002).

United States

Each US study consists of an independent sample. The Minnesota Twin Study of Adult Development and Aging (MTSADA) is a population-based sample drawn from state birth records (Finkel & McGue, Reference Finkel and McGue1993; McGue et al., Reference McGue, Hirsch and Lykken1993). The Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging (VETSA) is a community-dwelling sample of male–male twin pairs, all of whom served in some branch of US military service sometime between 1965 and 1975 (Kremen et al., Reference Kremen, Franz and Lyons2013). Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) is a national telephone/mail survey originally carried out in 1995–1996 that included specific recruitment methods for twins (South & Krueger, Reference South and Krueger2012). The Carolina African-American Twin Study of Aging (CAATSA) used public records to identify all living African-American twins in the State of North Carolina born between 1920 and 1970 (Whitfield, Reference Whitfield2013). The Project Talent Twin and Sibling Study (PTTS) includes 4481 twins and triplets plus 522 of their siblings, drawn from Project Talent, a longitudinal study begun in 1960 with a nationally representative sample of US high school students born 1942–1946 (Flanagan, Reference Flanagan1962). Follow-up surveys were conducted in young adulthood (ages 19, 23 and 29), at 1, 5 and 11 years following the year of expected high school graduation, and then in 2014 (aged 68–72) and 2019 (aged 73–77). The PTTS tracked 96.4% of the original PT twins (Prescott et al., Reference Prescott, Achorn, Kaiser, Mitchell, McArdle and Lapham2013). The National Academy of Sciences-National Research Council (NAS-NRC) Twin Registry consists of white male twin pairs born in the years 1917–1927, both of whom served in the armed forces, mostly during World War II (Page, Reference Page2006).

Australia

The Australian Over 50s study (A50) is based on a questionnaire mailed between 1993 and 1995 to Australian twins aged 50–95 (Hopper, Reference Hopper2002; Hopper et al., Reference Hopper, Foley, White and Pollaers2013). The Older Australian Twins Study (OATS) incorporates in-person assessments every two years of twins age 65 and older in the three eastern states of Australia: New South Wales, Victoria and Queensland (Sachdev et al., Reference Sachdev, Lammel, Trollor, Lee, Wright, Ames and Schofield2009).

The range in study years and intake ages across the 18 IGEMS studies results in unique coverage of cohorts and historical periods. As shown in Table 2, the IGEMS sample permits sequential comparisons of sex and SES effects across six cohorts.

Table 2. Total number of individuals (% female) in each birth year range (cohort) by age at intake

IGEMS Measures

Measures used in IGEMS analyses include aging-relevant outcomes in three broad domains: physical health and functional ability (e.g., self-reported diseases, subjective health, body mass index (BMI), grip strength, motor function, activities of daily living), psychological wellbeing (e.g., depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, subjective wellbeing, loneliness) and cognitive health (i.e., scores on cognitive tests; dementia). Predictors and covariates include health behaviors (e.g., smoking, alcohol, physical activity, cognitively engaging leisure activity), social resources and indicators of SES. Table 3 presents a list of some of the primary phenotypes assessed and the number of IGEMS studies that include each variable.

Table 3. Number of the 18 IGEMS studies with key variables

Because participating studies differed in how similar constructs were assessed, IGEMS gives emphasis to harmonization of relevant phenotypes and outcomes. Creating scores that are common across studies enables pooling data across samples, in order to increase power. Score harmonization requires overlapping item content across studies as well as across time for longitudinal hypotheses. For some measures, it was straightforward to create a common metric, for example, BMI, lung function, and blood pressure. For harmonizing education and occupation, we have recoded all studies to the International Standard Classification of Education (UNESCO, 1997) and the International Standard Classification of Occupations (Ganzeboom et al., Reference Ganzeboom, De Graaf, Treiman and De Leeuw1992) as an international standard. Where a common metric was not already available, overlapping item content and response formats were identified and item response theory or factor-analytic techniques were implemented to create harmonized scores across studies. Where there were no common items across studies, IGEMS has collected separate samples that were administered the different measures used in different IGEMS studies to measure a given construct, with those results used to establish ‘crosswalks’ between the different scales (Gatz, Reynolds et al., Reference Gatz, Reynolds, Finkel, Hahn, Zhou and Zavala2015).

Across the 18 IGEMS studies, there is genome-wide genotype information available from 22,834 subjects, including MZ cotwins of genotyped individuals (Table 4), which will be available for analysis with appropriate correction for clustering. In Sweden, there are also genotypes available from an additional 14,498 individuals who participated in SALT substudies known as TwinGene and SALTY (Scanning Across the Lifespan of Twins — Young; Magnusson et al., Reference Magnusson, Almqvist, Rahman, Ganna, Viktorin, Walum and Larsson2013), but under age 65 when screened in SALT. Thus, the total number of individuals with genome-wide genotyping available to us is 37,332. PRSs have or will be computed for cardiovascular disease, lipids, type II diabetes, Alzheimer’s disease, neuroticism, major depression and depressive symptoms, smoking and alcohol behaviors, wellbeing and educational attainment. We will compute new PRSs as new GWAS training sets become available.

Table 4. Genotype data available in participating IGEMS twin studies

Note: aIncluding imputed MZ cotwins; bFrom SATSA, GENDER, HARMONY, TwinGene > 65 years; cFrom TwinGene < 65 years and SALTY.

Approaches

IGEMS harnesses the full analytic potential of twin designs to address issues of GE interplay as well as risk and protective factors for aging-related outcomes. Methods using MZ within-pair differences allow us to test for the presence of GE without having a specific measured early environment. With a MZ within-pair approach, we established evidence of gene × environment for BMI, depressive symptoms, a physical illness index, several cognitive domains (Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Gatz, Christensen, Kaprio, Korhonen, Kremen and Pedersen2016) and longitudinal grip strength trajectories (Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Pedersen, Rantanen, Kremen, Johnson, Panizzon and Reynolds2016). Results also suggested that the apolipoprotein E gene (APOE) may act as a ‘variability gene’ for symptoms of depression and spatial reasoning, but not for other cognitive measures or BMI, with greater intrapair differences for non-ε4 carriers. For grip-strength trajectories, a buffering effect for ε2 carriers emerged, with lower sensitivity to environments and better-maintained performance.

Cotwin control methods also provide the opportunity to strengthen causal inferences and test whether associations between early life exposures and late life outcomes are due to confounding by common familial (genetic and/or shared rearing) influences. For example, Mosing et al. (Reference Mosing, Cnattingius, Gatz, Neiderhiser and Pedersen2016) found that of a number of birth characteristics, low birth weight was associated with poorer self-rated health in adulthood when evaluated with a generalized estimating equation adjusting for the twin structure. However, as these associations were attenuated in a cotwin control analyses (first in all pairs, then only in MZ pairs), there is evidence that the association is in part due to familial influences. In subsequent analyses of birth characteristics and cognitive impairment and dementia, we found evidence that low birth weight and small head circumference are risk factors for dementia. Further, head circumference was also significantly associated with age-related cognitive impairment (Mosing et al., Reference Mosing, Lundholm, Cnattingius, Gatz and Pedersen2018). Here, within-pair analyses of identical twins suggested that the observed associations between birth characteristics and cognitive decline are likely not due to underlying familial etiology.

To quantify interplay, we have applied biometric moderation models (Purcell, Reference Purcell2002; van der Sluis et al., Reference van der Sluis, Dolan, Neale and Posthuma2008). We have examined GE interactions in relation to cognitive performance (Pahlen et al., Reference Pahlen, Hamdi, Dahl Aslan, Horwitz, Panizzon, Zavala and McGue2018; Zavala et al., Reference Zavala, Beam, Finch, Gatz, Johnson, Kremen and Reynolds2018), depression (Petkus et al., Reference Petkus, Beam, Johnson, Kaprio, Korhonen, McGue and Gatz2017), subjective health (Franz et al., Reference Franz, Finkel, Panizzon, Spoon, Christensen, Gatz and Pedersen2017) and an index of physical illness (Gatz, Petkus et al., Reference Gatz, Petkus, Franz, Kaprio and Christensen2015). For most phenotypes, unique environmental variance was greater at older ages, presumably reflecting the accumulating importance of individual differences in environmental context with age. However, there was a nonuniform pattern for genetic factors over age, in combination with SES or sex moderation. In SES moderation analyses of cognition, for verbal ability and for perceptual speed (Zavala et al., Reference Zavala, Beam, Finch, Gatz, Johnson, Kremen and Reynolds2018), genetic variance was diminished in those with higher SES, perhaps reflective of a buffering effect on normative aging processes particularly for speed; whereas for short-term/working memory and spatial performance, genetic variance was amplified with higher SES, suggesting stable experiences in enriched (high SES) environments may support genetic variation.

Because IGEMS has genome-wide genetic data, we are able to create PRS scores and incorporate these into our models of GE interplay. In this case, the PRS scores will be entered as a moderator, together with other indicators of the environment, such as SES. The interaction between PRS and SES in regression models predicting health outcomes of interest will inform whether those at high genetic risk for a health outcome are more or less susceptible to, for example, health-promoting benefits of favorable environments.

Country-level SES indicators are available from various online sources. These data provide historical measures of social and economic conditions from the mid-1800s to the early 2000s for each of the five IGEMS countries. Variables include average years of education, educational inequality, gross domestic product per capita (GDP), Gini coefficient of income inequality, public social spending and top 1% income share. As a demonstration of the use of country-level SES indicators, we examined harmonized depressive symptom scores across five countries and a wide range of birth cohorts from 1890 through 1970. We used Top 1% (share of wealth held by top 1% of residents) to index country-level inequality when participants were aged 10 (World Inequality Database, 2017). Controlling for age when the depressive symptom measure was completed, gender, and country-level GDP, adult depressive symptom scores were higher among those exposed to greater inequality as youths. Using a modified twin correlation model, we found greater genetic effects on depressive symptoms with exposure to greater inequality (Gatz et al., Reference Gatz, Finch, Beam and Thomas2018).

Summary

The IGEMS consortium harnesses a combination of twin designs and multiple studies representing different cohorts and contexts. The accomplishments of the consortium demonstrate the feasibility of this type of collaboration in addressing GE interplay with respect to important age-related outcomes.

Acknowledgments

Members of the IGEMS Consortium include Karolinska Institutet: Nancy Pedersen, Miriam Mosing, Malin Ericsson; Jönköping University: Anna Dahl Aslan, Ida Karlsson; University of Southern California: Margaret Gatz, Brian Finch, Kyla Thomas, Christopher Beam, Susan Luczak, Carol Prescott, Em Arpawong, Catalina Zavala, Andrew Petkus; University of Southern Denmark: Kaare Christensen, Marianne Nygaard, Mette Wod; University of Minnesota: Matt McGue, Robert Krueger; University of California, Riverside: Chandra Reynolds, Elizabeth Munoz, Shandell Pahlen, Dianna Phillips; Indiana University Southeast: Deborah Finkel; University of California, San Diego: William Kremen, Carol Franz, Matthew Panizzon, Jeremy Elman, Daniel Gustavson; Edinburgh University: Wendy Johnson; The Pennsylvania State University: Jenae Neiderhiser; Boston University: Michael Lyons; Wayne State University: Keith Whitfield; University of Helsinki: Jaakko Kaprio, Elina Sillanpää, Eero Vuoksimaa; University of New South Wales: Perminder Sachdev, Vibeke Catts, Marie Kondo, Teresa Lee, Karen Mather, Anbu Thalamuthu, Simone Reppermund; QIMR Berghofer: Nicholas G. Martin; American Institutes for Research: Susan Lapham, Kelly Peters; National Academies of Sciences, Engineering and Medicine: David Butler; Duke University: Brenda Plassman. We thank Patricia St. Clair and Ellen Walters for their work on data management.

Financial support

IGEMS is supported by the National Institutes of Health Grants No. R01 AG037985, R56 AG037985, R01 AG059329, R01 AG060470, RF1 AG058068. SATSA was supported by grants R01 AG04563, R01 AG10175, the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Aging, the Swedish Council For Working Life and Social Research (FAS) (97:0147:1B, 2009-0795) and Swedish Research Council (825-2007-7460, 825-2009-6141). OCTO-Twin was supported by grant R01 AG08861. Gender was supported by the MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Aging, The Axel and Margaret Ax:son Johnson’s Foundation, The Swedish Council for Social Research and the Swedish Foundation for Health Care Sciences and Allergy Research. TOSS was supported by grant R01 MH54610 from the National Institutes of Health. The Danish Twin Registry is supported by grants from The National Program for Research Infrastructure 2007 from the Danish Agency for Science and Innovation, the Velux Foundation and the US National Institute of Health (P01 AG08761). The Minnesota Twin Study of Adult Development and Aging was supported by NIA grant R01 AG06886. VETSA was supported by National Institute of Health grants NIA R01 AG018384, R01 AG018386, R01 AG022381 and R01 AG022982, and, in part, with resources of the VA San Diego Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental Health. The Cooperative Studies Program of the Office of Research & Development of the United States Department of Veterans Affairs has provided financial support for the development and maintenance of the Vietnam Era Twin (VET) Registry. Data collection and analyses in the Finnish Twin Cohort and Finntwin16 have been supported by ENGAGE — European Network for Genetic and Genomic Epidemiology, FP7-HEALTH-F4-2007, grant agreement number 201413, National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (grants AA-12502, AA-00145 and AA-09203), the Academy of Finland Center of Excellence in Complex Disease Genetics (grant numbers: 213506, 129680) and the Academy of Finland (grants 100499, 205585, 118555, 141054, 265240, 263278, 264146, 308248 and 312073). This MIDUS study was supported by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Midlife Development and by National Institute on Aging Grant AG20166. Funding for the Australian Over-50’s twin study was supported by Mr. George Landers of Chania, Crete. We acknowledge the contribution of the OATS research team (https://cheba.unsw.edu.au/project/older-australian-twins-study) to this study. The OATS study has been funded by a National Health & Medical Research Council (NHMRC) and Australian Research Council (ARC) Strategic Award Grant of the Ageing Well, Ageing Productively Program (ID No. 401162) and NHMRC Project Grants (ID 1045325 and 1085606). OATS participant recruitment was facilitated through Twins Research Australia, a national resource in part supported by a Centre for Research Excellence Grant (ID: 1079102), from the National Health and Medical Research Council. We thank the participants for their time and generosity in contributing to this research. The Carolina African American Twin Study of Aging (CAATSA) was funded by NIA grant R01 AG13662. The Project Talent Twin Study has been supported by National Institute of Health grants R01 AG043656 and R01 AG056163, and development funds from American Institutes of Research. Funding for archiving the NAS-NRC Twin Registry data was provided by NIH Grant No. R21 AG039572. The content of this manuscript is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIA/NIH, or the VA.

Author note

IGEMS data are not publicly available given the variety of data agreements and regulations governing the different studies and countries. However, many of the individual studies participating in IGEMS do have ways to access their data, and many of the datasets may be accessed through National Archive of Computerized Data on Aging (NACDA).

Footnotes

*

This article is written on behalf of the IGEMS consortium. Members are listed in Acknowledgements.

References

Adler, N. E., Boyce, W. T., Chesney, M., Cohen, S., Folkman, S., Kahn, R., & Syme, S. L. (1994). Socioeconomic status and health: The challenge of the gradient. American Psychologist, 49, 1524.10.1037/0003-066X.49.1.15CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boardman, J. D., Daw, J., & Freese, J. (2013). Defining the environment in gene-environment research: Lessons from social epidemiology. American Journal of Public Health, 103, S64S72.10.2105/AJPH.2013.301355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Crawford, L. E., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M. H., Kowalewski, R. B., … Berntson, G. G. (2002). Loneliness and health: Potential mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 407417.10.1097/00006842-200205000-00005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, K., Holm, N. V., McGue, M., Corder, L., & Vaupel, J. W. (1999). A Danish population-based twin study on general health in the elderly. Journal of Aging and Health, 11, 4964.10.1177/089826439901100103CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S. (2004). Social relationships and health. American Psychologist, 59, 676684.10.1037/0003-066X.59.8.676CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkel, D., & McGue, M. (1993). The origins of individual differences in memory among the elderly: a behavior genetic analysis. Psychology and Aging, 8, 527537.10.1037/0882-7974.8.4.527CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkel, D., & Pedersen, N. L. (2004). Processing speed and longitudinal trajectories of change for cognitive abilities: The Swedish adoption/Twin study of aging. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 11, 325345.10.1080/13825580490511152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flanagan, J. C. (1962). Project Talent. Applied Psychology, 11, 314.10.1111/j.1464-0597.1962.tb00441.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franz, C., Finkel, D., Panizzon, M., Spoon, K., Christensen, K., Gatz, M., … Pedersen, N. (2017). Facets of subjective health from early adulthood to old age. Journal of Aging and Health, 29, 149171.10.1177/0898264315625488CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freedman, V. A., Martin, L. G., Schoeni, R. F., & Cornman, J. C. (2008). Declines in late-life disability: The role of early- and mid-life factors. Social Science & Medicine, 66, 15881602.10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ganzeboom, H. B. G., De Graaf, P., Treiman, D. J., & De Leeuw, J. (1992). A standard international socio-economic index of occupational status. Social Science Research, 21, 156.10.1016/0049-089X(92)90017-BCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gatz, M., Finch, B., Beam, C., & Thomas, K. (2018). Interplay of a country’s income inequality in childhood and adult depressive symptoms. Behavior Genetics, 48, 472.Google Scholar
Gatz, M., Fratiglioni, L., Johansson, B., Berg, S., Mortimer, J. A., Reynolds, C. A., … Pedersen, N. L. (2005). Complete ascertainment of dementia in the Swedish Twin Registry: The harmony study. Neurobiology of Aging, 26, 439447.10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.04.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gatz, M., Mortimer, J. A., Fratiglioni, L., Johannson, B., Berg, S., Andel, R., … Pedersen, N. L. (2007). Accounting for the relationship between low education and dementia: A twin study. Physiology & Behavior, 92, 232237.10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gatz, M., Petkus, A. J., Franz, C., Kaprio, J., & Christensen, K. (2015). Age moderation of individual differences in chronic medical illness burden. Behavior Genetics, 45, 657.Google Scholar
Gatz, M., Reynolds, C. A., Finkel, D., Hahn, C., Zhou, Y., & Zavala, C. (2015). Data harmonization in aging research: Not so fast. Experimental Aging Research, 41, 475495.10.1080/0361073X.2015.1085748CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gold, C. H., Malmberg, B., McClearn, G. E., Pedersen, N. L., & Berg, S. (2002). Gender and health: A study of older unlike-sex twins. Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57B, S168S176.10.1093/geronb/57.3.S168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, J. L. (2002). The Australian Twin Registry. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 5, 329336.10.1375/136905202320906048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopper, J. L., Foley, D. L., White, P. A., & Pollaers, V. (2013). Australian Twin Registry: 30 years of progress. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 3442.10.1017/thg.2012.121CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaprio, J., & Koskenvuo, M. (2002). Genetic and environmental factors in complex diseases: The older Finnish Twin Cohort. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 5, 358365.10.1375/136905202320906093CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaprio, J., Pulkkinen, L., & Rose, R. J. (2002). Genetic and environmental factors in health-related behaviors: Studies on Finnish twins and twin families. Twin Research, 5, 366371.10.1375/136905202320906101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katzman, R. (2004). A neurologist’s view of Alzheimer’s disease and dementia. International Psychogeriatrics, 16, 259273.10.1017/S1041610204000456CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kawachi, I., Levine, S., Miller, S. M., Lasch, K., & Amick, B. (1994). Income Inequality and Life Expectancy: Theory, Research, and Policy (Society and Health Working Paper Series, 94–2). Boston, MA: Harvard School of Public Health.Google Scholar
Kremen, W. S., Franz, C. E., & Lyons, M. J. (2013). VETSA: The Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 399402.10.1017/thg.2012.86CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, B. B., D’Onofrio, B. M., & Waldman, I. D. (2009). Using epidemiologic methods to test hypotheses regarding causal influences on child and adolescent mental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 5362.10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01980.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtenstein, P., Sullivan, P. F., Cnattingius, S., Gatz, M., Johansson, S., Carlstrom, E., … Pedersen, N. L. (2006). The Swedish Twin Registry in the third millennium: An update. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 875882.10.1375/twin.9.6.875CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, G., Sommerlad, A., Orgeta, V., Costafreda, S. G., Huntley, J., Ames, D., … Mukadam, N. (2017). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. The Lancet, 390, 26732734.10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynch, J., Smith, G. D., Harper, S. A., Hillemeier, M., Ross, N., Kaplan, G. A., & Wolfson, M. (2004). Is income inequality a determinant of population health? Part 1. A systematic review. Millbank Quarterly, 82, 599.10.1111/j.0887-378X.2004.00302.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynch, J., Smith, G. D., Kaplan, G. A., & House, J. S. (2000). Income inequality and mortality: importance to health of individual income, psychosocial environment, or material conditions. British Medical Journal, 320, 1200.10.1136/bmj.320.7243.1200CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnusson, P. K., Almqvist, C., Rahman, I., Ganna, A., Viktorin, A., Walum, H., & Larsson, H. (2013). The Swedish Twin Registry: establishment of a biobank and other recent developments. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 317329.10.1017/thg.2012.104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClearn, G. E., Johansson, B., Berg, S., Pedersen, N. L., Ahern, F., Petrill, S. A., & Plomin, R. (1997). Substantial genetic influence on cognitive abilities in twins 80 or more years old. Science, 276, 15601563.10.1126/science.276.5318.1560CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGue, M., Hirsch, B., & Lykken, D. T. (1993). Age and the self-perception of ability: A twin study analysis. Psychology and Aging, 8, 7280.10.1037/0882-7974.8.1.72CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGue, M., Osler, M., & Christensen, K. (2010). Causal inference and observational aging research: The utility of twins. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 546556.10.1177/1745691610383511CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mensah, G. A., Mokdad, A. H., Ford, E. S., Greenlund, K. J., & Croft, J. B. (2005). State of disparities in cardiovascular health in the United States. Circulation, 111, 12331241.10.1161/01.CIR.0000158136.76824.04CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mielke, M. M., Vemuri, P., & Rocca, W. A. (2014). Clinical epidemiology of Alzheimer’s disease: assessing sex and gender differences. Clinical Epidemiology, 6, 3748.10.2147/CLEP.S37929CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (2003). Education, Social Class, and Health. New York, NY: Aldine deGruyter.Google Scholar
Mosing, M. A., Cnattingius, S., Gatz, M., Neiderhiser, J. M., & Pedersen, N. L. (2016). Associations Between Fetal Growth and Self-Perceived Health Throughout Adulthood: A Co-twin Control Study. Behavior Genetics, 46, 457466.10.1007/s10519-015-9776-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mosing, M. A., Lundholm, C., Cnattingius, S., Gatz, M., & Pedersen, N. L. (2018). Associations between birth characteristics and age-related cognitive impairment and dementia: A registry-based cohort study. PLOS Medicine, 15, e1002609. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002609 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neiderhiser, J. M., & Lichtenstein, P. (2008). The Twin and Offspring Study in Sweden: Advancing our understanding of genotype-environment interplay by studying twins and their families. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 40, 11161123.Google Scholar
Osler, M., McGue, M., Lund, R., & Christensen, K. (2008). Marital status and twins’ health and behavior: an analysis of middle-aged Danish twins. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 482487.10.1097/PSY.0b013e31816f857bCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page, W. F. (2006). Update on the NAS-NRC Twin Registry. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 985987.10.1375/twin.9.6.985CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pahlen, S., Hamdi, H. R., Dahl Aslan, A. K., Horwitz, B. N., Panizzon, I., Zavala, C., … McGue, M. (2018). Age-moderation of genetic and environmental contributions to cognitive functioning in mid- and late-life for specific cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 68, 7081.10.1016/j.intell.2017.12.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pedersen, N. L., Christensen, K., Dahl, A., Finkel, D., Franz, C. E., Gatz, M., … Reynolds, C. A. (2013). IGEMS: The Consortium on Interplay of Genes and Environment across Multiple Studies. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 481489.10.1017/thg.2012.110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, I., Pedersen, N. L., Rantanen, T., Kremen, W. S., Johnson, W., Panizzon, M. S., … Reynolds, C. A. (2016). GxE interaction influences trajectories of hand grip strength. Behavior Genetics, 46, 2030.10.1007/s10519-015-9736-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petkus, A. J., Beam, C. R., Johnson, W., Kaprio, J., Korhonen, T., McGue, M., … Gatz, M. (2017). Gene–environment interplay in depressive symptoms: Moderation by age, sex, and physical illness. Psychological Medicine, 47, 18361847.10.1017/S0033291717000290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prescott, C. A., Achorn, D. L., Kaiser, A., Mitchell, L., McArdle, J. J., & Lapham, S. J. (2013). The Project TALENT Twin and Sibling Study. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 18, 437448.10.1017/thg.2012.71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purcell, S. (2002). Variance components models for gene-environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research, 5, 554571.10.1375/136905202762342026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reiss, D., Leve, L. D., & Neiderhiser, J. (2013). How genes and the social environment moderate each other. American Journal of Public Health, 103, S111S121.10.2105/AJPH.2013.301408CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, C. A., Gatz, M., Christensen, K., Kaprio, J., Korhonen, T., Kremen, W. S., … Pedersen, N. L. (2016). Gene-Environment interplay in physical, psychological, and cognitive domains in mid to late adulthood: Is APOE a variability gene? Behavior Genetics, 46, 419.10.1007/s10519-015-9761-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rijsdijk, F. V., & Sham, P. C. (2002). Analytic approaches to twin data using structural equation models. Briefings in Bioinformatics, 3, 119133.10.1093/bib/3.2.119CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (2009). Understanding and testing risk mechanisms for mental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 4452.10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01976.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sachdev, P. S., Lammel, A., Trollor, J. N., Lee, T., Wright, M. J., Ames, D., … Schofield, P. R. (2009). A comprehensive neuropsychiatric study of elderly twins: The Older Australian Twins Study. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 12, 573582.10.1375/twin.12.6.573CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sattler, C., Toro, P., Schonknecht, P., & Schroder, J. (2012). Cognitive activity, education and socioeconomic status as preventive factors for mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Research, 196, 9095.10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shanahan, M. J., & Boardman, J. D. (2009). Genetics and behavior in the life course: A promising frontier. In Elder, G. H. & Giele, J. Z. (Ed.), The Craft of Life Course Research (pp. 215235). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Shanahan, M. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2005). Social context in gene–environment interactions: Retrospect and prospect. Journals of Gerontology B, 60B, 6576.10.1093/geronb/60.Special_Issue_1.65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharp, E. S., & Gatz, M. (2011). Relationship between education and dementia: An updated systematic review. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 25, 289304.10.1097/WAD.0b013e318211c83cCrossRefGoogle Scholar
South, S. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2012). Genetic strategies for probing conscientiousness and its relationship to aging. Developmental Psychology, 50, 13621376.10.1037/a0030725CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
UNESCO. (1997). ISCED1997: International Standard Classification of Education. Montreal, Quebec: UNESCO Institute for Statistics.Google Scholar
van der Sluis, S., Dolan, C. V., Neale, M. C., & Posthuma, D. (2008). A general test for gene-environment interaction in sib pair-based association analysis of quantitative traits. Behavior Genetics, 38, 372389.10.1007/s10519-008-9201-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Sluis, S., Posthuma, D., & Dolan, C. V. (2012). A note on false positives and power in G x E modelling of twin data. Behavior Genetics, 42, 170186.10.1007/s10519-011-9480-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, H.-X., MacDonald, S. W. S., Dekhtyar, S., & Fratiglioni, L. (2017). Association of lifelong exposure to cognitive reserve-enhancing factors with dementia risk: A community-based cohort study. PLOS Medicine, 14, e1002251.10.1371/journal.pmed.1002251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitfield, K. E. (2013). A registry of adult African American Twins: The Carolina African American Twin Study of Aging. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 476480.10.1017/thg.2012.79CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zavala, C., Beam, C. R., Finch, B. K., Gatz, M., Johnson, W., Kremen, W. S., … Reynolds, C. A. (2018). Attained SES as a moderator of adult cognitive performance: Testing gene-environment interaction in various cognitive domains. Developmental Psychology, 54, 23562370.10.1037/dev0000576CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Number of twins in each study included in IGEMS, by age at intake

Figure 1

Table 2. Total number of individuals (% female) in each birth year range (cohort) by age at intake

Figure 2

Table 3. Number of the 18 IGEMS studies with key variables

Figure 3

Table 4. Genotype data available in participating IGEMS twin studies