Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-tfzs5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-12T07:09:10.128Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Computerized color-vision test based upon postreceptoral channel sensitivities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2005

E. MIYAHARA
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, California State University, Fullerton
J. POKORNY
Affiliation:
Visual Science Laboratories, University of Chicago, Chicago
V.C. SMITH
Affiliation:
Visual Science Laboratories, University of Chicago, Chicago
E. SZEWCZYK
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, California State University, Fullerton
J. McCARTIN
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, California State University, Fullerton
K. CALDWELL
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, California State University, Fullerton
A. KLERER
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, California State University, Fullerton

Abstract

An automated, computerized color-vision test was designed to diagnose congenital red–green color-vision defects. The observer viewed a yellow appearing CRT screen. The principle was to measure increment thresholds for three different chromaticities, the background yellow, a red, and a green chromaticity. Spatial and temporal parameters were chosen to favor parvocellular pathway mediation of thresholds. Thresholds for the three test stimuli were estimated by four-alternative forced-choice (4AFC), randomly interleaved staircases. Four 1.5-deg, 4.2 cd/m2 square pedestals were arranged as a 2 × 2 matrix around the center of the display with 15-minute separations. A trial incremented all four squares by 1.0 cd/m2 for 133 ms. One randomly chosen square included an extra increment of a test chromaticity. The observer identified the different appearing square using the cursor. Administration time was ∼5 minutes. Normal trichromats showed clear Sloan notch as defined by log (ΔY/ΔR), whereas red–green color defectives generally showed little or no Sloan notch, indicating that their thresholds were mediated by their luminance system, not by the chromatic system. Data from 107 normal trichromats showed a mean Sloan notch of 0.654 (SD = 0.123). Among 16 color-vision defectives tested (2 protanopes, 1 protanomal, 6 deuteranopes, & 7 deuteranomals), the Sloan notch was between −0.062 and 0.353 for deutans and was <−0.10 for protans. A sufficient number of color-defective observers have not yet been tested to determine whether the test can reliably discriminate between protans and deutans. Nevertheless, the current data show that the test can work as a quick diagnostic procedure (functional trichromatism or dichromatism) of red–green color-vision defect.

Information

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2004 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Article purchase

Temporarily unavailable

References

REFERENCES

Derrington, A.M., Krauskopf, J., & Lennie, P. (1984). Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. Journal of Physiology (London) 357, 241265.Google Scholar
Foster, D.H. & Snelgar, R.S. (1983). Test and field spectral sensitivities of colour mechanisms obtained on small white backgrounds: Action of unitary opponent-colour processes? Vision Research 23, 787797.Google Scholar
Hsu, H., Pokorny, J., & Smith, V.C. (2000). A postreceptoral test of color vision. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 41, S806.Google Scholar
Miyahara, E., Pokorny, J., & Smith, V.C. (1996). Increment threshold and purity discrimination spectral sensitivities of X-chromosome-linked color defective observers. Vision Research 36, 15971613.Google Scholar
Pokorny, J. & Smith, V.C. (1997). Psychophysical signatures associated with magnocellular and parvocellular pathway contrast gain. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 14, 24772486.Google Scholar
Pokorny, J., Smith, V.C., Verriest, G., & Pinckers, A.J.L.G., eds. (1979). Congenital and Acquired Color Vision Defects. New York: Grune and Stratton.
Sharpe, L.T., Stockman, A., Jägle, H., & Nathans, J. (1999). Opsin genes, cone photopigments, color vision and colorblindness. In Color Vision: From Genes to Perception, ed. Gegenfurtner, K. & Sharpe, L.T., pp. 350. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Sloan, L.L. (1928). The effect of intensity of light, state of adaptation of the eye, and size of photometric field on the visibility curve. Psychological Monographs 38, 187.Google Scholar
Smith, V.C. & Pokorny, J. (2003). Psychophysical correlates of parvo- and magnocellular function. In Normal and Defective Colour Vision, ed. Knoblauch, K., pp. 9107. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Smith, V.C., Pokorny, J., & Sun, H. (2000). Chromatic contrast discrimination: Data and prediction for stimuli varying in L and M cone excitation. Color Research and Application 25, 105115.Google Scholar
Sperling, H.G. & Harwerth, R.S. (1971). Red–green cone interaction in the increment-threshold spectral sensitivity of primates. Science 172, 180184.Google Scholar