Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T04:21:22.183Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stimulus configuration and location in the visual field affect appetitive responses by the praying mantis, Sphodromantis lineola (Burr.)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 June 2009

Frederick R. Prete
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville, Ohio

Abstract

Adult female praying mantises, Sphodromantis lineola (Burr.), were presented with computer-generated black rectangular stimuli that moved horizontally or vertically at 82 deg/s against a homogeneous white background. Both stimulus configuration (orientation in relation to direction) and the retinal location of the stimulus image affected the rate at which mantises responded appetitively (approached or struck at the stimulus). Mantises responded most to square stimuli (12.5 deg × 12.5 deg) when they moved horizontally or vertically through, or horizontally at 24.5 deg below the center of their visual field. Mantises also responded most to vertically (vs. horizontally) oriented rectangular stimuli (12.5 deg × 47 deg) that moved through their visual-field center, irrespective of whether the stimuli moved downward or horizontally. Upward moving stimuli elicited intermediate amounts of behavior with no configuration preference. Mantises did not demonstrate a configuration preference when rectangular stimuli moved ≥24.5 deg outside of the visual-field center. Furthermore, mantises responded very little and demonstrated no configuration preferences to stimuli that moved less than ≈83 deg through their visual field even if the stimuli moved through the visual-field center.

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barrows, E.M. (1984). Perch sites and food of adult Chinese mantids (Dictyoptera: Mantidae). Proceedings of the Entomological Society of Washington 86, 898901.Google Scholar
Bartley, J.A. (1983). Prey selection and capture by the Chinese mantid (Tenodera sinensis, Saussure). Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Delaware.Google Scholar
Buchner, E. (1984). Behavioral analysis of spatial vision in insects. In Photoreception and Vision in Invertebrates, ed. Ali, M.A., pp. 561622. New York, London: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collett, T.S. (1972). Visual neurons in the anterior optic tract of the privet hawk moth. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 78, 396433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collett, T.S. (1978). Peering – a locust behavior pattern for obtaining motion parallax information. Journal of Experimental Biology 76, 237241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collett, T.S. (1988). How ladybirds approach nearby stalks: A study of visual selectivity and attention. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 163, 355363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collett, T.S. & Land, M.F. (1975). Visual control of flight behavior in the hoverfly, Syritta pipiens L. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 99, 166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collett, T.S. & Patterson, C.J. (1991). Relative motion parallax and target localisation in the locust, Schistocerca gregaria. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 169, 615621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edmunds, M. (1972). Defensive behavior in Ghanian praying mantids. Journal of Zoology of the Linnean Society 51, 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egelhaaf, M., Hausen, K., Reichardt, W. & Wehrhahn, C. (1988). Visual control in flies relies on neuronal computation of object and background motion. Trends in Neurosciences 11, 351358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, C. & Strausfeld, N.J. (1991). The functional organization of male-specific visual neurons in flies. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 169, 395411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillot, C. (1980). Entomology. New York, London: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Goulet, M., Campan, R. & Lambin, M. (1981). The visual perception of relative distances in the wood-cricket, Nemobius sylvestris. Physiological Entomology 6, 357367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Groenberg, W. & Strausfeld, N.J. (1991). Descending pathways connecting the male-specific visual system of flies to the neck and flight motor. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 169, 413426.Google Scholar
Holling, C.S. (1964). The analysis of complex population processes. Canadian Entomologist 96, 335347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horridge, G.A. & Duelli, P. (1979). Anatomy of the regional differences in the eye of the mantis, Ciulfina. Journal of Experimental Biology 80, 165190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horridge, G.A. (1986). A theory of insect vision: Velocity parallax. Proceedings of the Royal Society B (London) 229, 1327.Google Scholar
Hurd, L.E. & Eisenberg, R.M. (1984). Experimental density manipulations of the predator Tenodera sinensis (Orthoptera: Mantidae) in an old-field community. II. The influence of mantids on Arthropod community structure. Journal of Animal Ecology 53, 955967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ibbotson, M.R. (1991). Wide-field motion-sensitive neurons tuned to horizontal movement in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 168, 91102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ibbotson, M.R., Maddess, T. & Dubois, R. (1991). A system of insect neurons sensitive to horizontal and vertical image motion connects the medulla and midbrain. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 169, 355367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M.D. (1976). Concerning the feeding habits of the praying mantis, Tenodera aridifolia sinensis, Saussure. Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society 49, 164.Google Scholar
Kevan, K. McE. (1985). The mantis and the serpent. Entomologist's Monthly Magazine 121, 18.Google Scholar
Kirchner, W.H. (1989). Freely flying honeybees use image motion to estimate object distance. Naturwissenschaften 76, 281282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krombholz, P.H. (1977). Food consumption in female mantids. Ph.D. Dissertation, Tufts University, Medford, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
Land, M.F. & Eckert, H. (1985). Maps of the acute zones of fly eyes. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 156, 525538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Land, M.F. & Collett, T.S. (1974). Chasing behaviors of houseflies (Fannia canicularis). Journal of Comparative Physiology A 89, 331357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehrer, M. (1990). How bees use peripheral eye regions to localize a frontally positioned target. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 167, 173185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nickle, D.A. (1981). Predation on a mouse by the Chinese mantid Tenodera aridifolia sinensis, Saussure (Dictyoptera: Mantoidea). Proceedings of the Entomological Society of Washington 83, 802803.Google Scholar
Olberg, R.M. (1981). Object- and self-movement detectors in the ventral nerve cord of the dragonfly. Journal of Comparative Physiology 141, 327334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palka, J. (1972). Moving movement detectors. American Zoologist 12, 497505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prete, F.R. (1990). Configural prey selection in the praying mantis, Sphodromantis lineola (Burr.). Brain, Behavior, and Evolution 36, 300306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prete, F.R. (1992 a). The discrimination of visual stimuli representing prey versus non-prey by the praying mantis, Sphodromantis lineola, (Burr.). Brain, Behavior, and Evolution 39, 285288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prete, F.R. (1992 b). The effects of background pattern and contrast on prey discrimination by the praying mantis Sphodromantis lineola (Burr.). Brain, Behavior, and Evolution 40, 311320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prete, F.R. (1992 c). Predatory behavior of the praying mantids, Tenodera aridifolia sinensis (Sauss.) and Sphodromantis lineola (Burr.). Ph.D. Dissertation, The University of Chicago, Illinois.Google Scholar
Prete, F.R. & Mahaffey, R.J. (1993 a). A chamber for mass hatching and early rearing of praying mantids (Orthoptera: Mantidae). Entomological News 104, 4752.Google Scholar
Prete, F.R. & Mahaffey, R.J. (1993 b). Appetitive responses to computer-generated visual stimuli by the praying mantis, Sphodromantis lineola (Burr.). Visual Neuroscience 10, 669679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prete, F.R. & Wolfe, M.M. (1992). Religious supplicant, seductive cannibal, or reflex machine? In search of the praying mantis. Journal of the History of Biology 25, 91136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prete, F.R., Klimek, C.A. & Grossman, S.P. (1990). The predatory strike of the mantis, Tenodera aridifolia sinensis. Journal of Insect Physiology 36, 561565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prete, F.R., Placek, P.J., Wilson, M.A., Mahaffey, R.J. & Nemcek, R.R. (1992). Stimulus speed and order of presentation effect the visually released predatory behaviors of the praying mantis, Sphodromantis lineola (BUTT.). Brain, Behavior, and Evolution (in press).Google Scholar
Ridpath, M.G. (1977). Predation of frogs and small birds by Hierod-ula werneri, mantidae in tropical Australia. Journal of the Australian Entomological Society 16, 153154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robert, D. (1988). Visual steering under closed-loop conditions in flying locusts: Flexibility of optimotor response and mechanisms of correctional steering. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 158, 1524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossel, S. (1979). Regional differences in photoreceptor performance in the eye of the praying mantis. Journal of Comparative Physiology 131, 95112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossel, S. (1980). Foveal fixation and tracking in the praying mantis. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 139, 307331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowell, C.H.F. (1971). The orthopteran descending movement detector (DMD) neurons: A characterisation and review. Zeitschrift für Vergleichende Physiologie 73, 167194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwind, R. (1978). Visual system of Notonecta glauca: A neuron sensitive to movement in the binocular visual field. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 123, 315328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sobel, E.C. (1990). The locust's use of motion parallax to measure distance. Journal of Comparative Physiology 167, 579588.Google ScholarPubMed
Srinivasan, M.V., Lehrer, M. & Horridge, G.A. (1990). Visual figure – ground discrimination in the honeybee: The role of motion parallax at boundaries. Proceedings of the Royal Society B (London) 238, 331350.Google Scholar
Srinivasan, M.V., Lehrer, M., Zhang, S.W. & Horridge, G.A. (1989). How honeybees measure their distance from objects of unknown size. Journal of Comparative Physiology 165, 605613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strausfeld, N.J. (1991). Structural organization of male-specific visual neurons in calliphorid optic lobes. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 169, 379393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stavenga, D.G. (1992). Eye regionalization and spectral tuning of retinal pigments in insects. Trends in Neurosciences 15, 213218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulk, A. (1844). Habits of the mantis. Annals and Magazine of Natural History 14, 78.Google Scholar
Varjú, D. (1976). Visual edge fixation and negative phototaxis in the mealworm beetle, Tenebrio molitor. Biological Cybernetics 25, 1726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, G.K. (1959). Visual scanning in the desert locust Schistocerca gregaria, Forskal. Journal of Experimental Biology 36, 512525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wehrhahn, C., Poggio, T. & Bülthoff, H. (1982). Tracking and chasing behavior of houseflies (Musca). Biological Cybernetics 32, 239241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar