Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T16:19:35.985Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive functioning, behavior, and white matter microstructure in the chronic phase after pediatric mild traumatic brain injury: results from the adolescent brain cognitive development study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 March 2024

Anja K. Betz
Affiliation:
cBRAIN, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany
Suheyla Cetin-Karayumak
Affiliation:
Psychiatry Neuroimaging Laboratory, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Elena M. Bonke
Affiliation:
cBRAIN, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany Graduate School of Systemic Neurosciences, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany
Johanna Seitz-Holland
Affiliation:
Psychiatry Neuroimaging Laboratory, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Fan Zhang
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Steve Pieper
Affiliation:
Isomics, Inc., Cambridge, MA, USA
Lauren J. O'Donnell
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Yorghos Tripodis
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, Boston University School of Public Health, Boston, MA, USA
Yogesh Rathi
Affiliation:
Psychiatry Neuroimaging Laboratory, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA Department of Radiology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Martha E. Shenton
Affiliation:
Psychiatry Neuroimaging Laboratory, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA Department of Radiology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Inga K. Koerte*
Affiliation:
cBRAIN, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany Psychiatry Neuroimaging Laboratory, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA Graduate School of Systemic Neurosciences, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Inga K. Koerte; Email: ikoerte@bwh.harvard.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) is common in children. Long-term cognitive and behavioral outcomes as well as underlying structural brain alterations following pediatric mTBI have yet to be determined. In addition, the effect of age-at-injury on long-term outcomes is largely unknown.

Methods

Children with a history of mTBI (n = 406; Mage = 10 years, SDage = 0.63 years) who participated in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study were matched (1:2 ratio) with typically developing children (TDC; n = 812) and orthopedic injury (OI) controls (n = 812). Task-based executive functioning, parent-rated executive functioning and emotion-regulation, and self-reported impulsivity were assessed cross-sectionally. Regression models were used to examine the effect of mTBI on these domains. The effect of age-at-injury was assessed by comparing children with their first mTBI at either 0-3, 4-7, or 8-10 years to the respective matched TDC controls. Fractional anisotropy (FA) and mean diffusivity (MD), both MRI-based measures of white matter microstructure, were compared between children with mTBI and controls.

Results

Children with a history of mTBI displayed higher parent-rated executive dysfunction, higher impulsivity, and poorer self-regulation compared to both control groups. At closer investigation, these differences to TDC were only present in one respective age-at-injury group. No alterations were found in task-based executive functioning or white matter microstructure.

Conclusions

Findings suggest that everyday executive function, impulsivity, and emotion-regulation are affected years after pediatric mTBI. Outcomes were specific to the age at which the injury occurred, suggesting that functioning is differently affected by pediatric mTBI during vulnerable periods. Groups did not differ in white matter microstructure.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) is common and affects about 1 million children annually in the US alone (Mannix, O'Brien, & Meehan, Reference Mannix, O'Brien and Meehan2013). While most children recover from acute symptoms within weeks following mTBI, about 30% experience prolonged post-concussive symptoms months later (Babcock et al., Reference Babcock, Byczkowski, Wade, Ho, Mookerjee and Bazarian2013). Moreover, the developmental gap between children with TBI and typically developing children (TDC) may widen with time because complex skills fail to develop properly (Babikian, Merkley, Savage, Giza, & Levin, Reference Babikian, Merkley, Savage, Giza and Levin2015). To date, however, most studies of pediatric mTBI focus primarily on the first few months following injury (Goh et al., Reference Goh, Looi, Goh, Sultana, Goh, Lee and Chong2021; Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Hayward, Brown, Zwicker, Ponsford and Van Donkelaar2018). Research is thus needed to investigate long-term outcomes following pediatric mTBI and the underlying pathophysiology to guide more targeted therapeutic interventions.

Executive functioning comprises a wide range of functions that relate to cognitive control, including inhibition, shifting between tasks, working memory, planning, and decision-making (Levin & Hanten, Reference Levin and Hanten2005). Their development is often tied to the maturation and myelination of the frontal cortex white matter, both of which continue into early adulthood (Lebel & Beaulieu, Reference Lebel and Beaulieu2011). Due to the high prevalence of diffuse axonal injuries after TBI, the developing white matter microstructure and corresponding executive abilities may be particularly vulnerable to pediatric TBI (Pinto, Poretti, Meoded, Tekes, & Huisman, Reference Pinto, Poretti, Meoded, Tekes and Huisman2012). According to a recent meta-analysis, executive functions are, in fact, affected by pediatric mTBI with impairments that last for months to years after the injury (Goh et al., Reference Goh, Looi, Goh, Sultana, Goh, Lee and Chong2021). However, other studies report either no impairment in executive functioning (Maillard-Wermelinger et al., Reference Maillard-Wermelinger, Yeates, Gerry Taylor, Rusin, Bangert, Dietrich and Wright2009) or initial difficulties that resolve within months post-injury (Chadwick et al., Reference Chadwick, Roth, Minich, Taylor, Bigler, Cohen and Yeates2021). The inconsistent findings may be due to the different methods used to assess executive functioning. Task-based assessment and rating scales are often interpreted interchangeably, even though they may measure distinct functions in different contexts (Friedman & Banich, Reference Friedman and Banich2019). While task-based assessments of executive functioning are believed to isolate one cognitive process under standardized conditions, rating scales reflect executive functioning in everyday situations (Friedman & Banich, Reference Friedman and Banich2019; Lace et al., Reference Lace, Merz, Grant, Teague, Aylward, Dorflinger and Gfeller2019). If and how pediatric mTBI affects these different aspects of executive functioning in the long-term remains to be determined.

Problems with executive functioning and, subsequently, behavioral control may be observed as impulsive behavior (Nigg, Reference Nigg2017). Whilst impulsivity has been tied to sports-related concussion in adolescents and young adults (Liebel, Edwards, & Broglio, Reference Liebel, Edwards and Broglio2021), it is rarely investigated in children. Doing so has great clinical relevance because higher impulsivity is associated with an increased risk of needing psychiatric care following pediatric mTBI (Saarinen et al., Reference Saarinen, Isaksson, Himanen, Erkinjuntti, Vahlberg, Koskinen and Lähdesmäki2022).

Further, persistent emotional problems have frequently been reported following mTBI in children (Emery et al., Reference Emery, Barlow, Brooks, Max, Villavicencio-Requis, Gnanakumar and Yeates2016; Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Montroy, Clark, Holubkov, Cox and Keenan2021; Gagner, Landry-Roy, Bernier, Gravel, & Beauchamp, Reference Gagner, Landry-Roy, Bernier, Gravel and Beauchamp2018; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Starkey, Barker-Collo, Ameratunga, Theadom, Pocock and Feigin2021). In addition to simply experiencing negative emotions, emotional problems may be due to impairments in cognitive control and subsequent difficulties in the regulation of such emotions.

Importantly, investigating consequences of mTBI in children and adolescents also needs to consider effects of brain development (i.e., brain injury ‘hits a moving target’; Giza, Kolb, Harris, Asarnow, & Prins, Reference Giza, Kolb, Harris, Asarnow and Prins2009). A TBI may not only impact already established functions but may also influence the developmental trajectory of new functions. Children's cognitive and behavioral abilities are formed during different time-periods of development and at different paces. This means that outcomes following mTBI may depend on the age at which it occurs (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Spencer-Smith, Leventer, Coleman, Anderson, Williams and Jacobs2009; Serpa et al., Reference Serpa, Ferguson, Larson, Bailard, Cooke, Greco and Prins2021). Further, many additional factors influence brain development. To account for environmental factors (e.g., socio-economic status), children with mTBI need to be compared to closely matched controls. Additionally, research on behavioral effects of mTBI needs to consider pre-injury characteristics (e.g., higher impulsivity predisposing children for injuries) and general injury effects (e.g., emotional distress after injury). Therefore, children with a history of orthopedic injury (OI) constitute a valuable control group in addition to TDC (Babikian et al., Reference Babikian, Satz, Zaucha, Light, Lewis and Asarnow2011; Emery et al., Reference Emery, Barlow, Brooks, Max, Villavicencio-Requis, Gnanakumar and Yeates2016).

Finally, while long-term behavioral difficulties following pediatric mTBI have received increasing attention, research on their neural mechanisms remains sparse. Diffusion MR imaging (dMRI) has been proposed as a sensitive tool for detecting alterations in brain microstructure following mTBI (Königs et al., Reference Königs, Pouwels, Ernest van Heurn, Bakx, Jeroen Vermeulen, Carel Goslings and Oosterlaan2018; Shenton et al., Reference Shenton, Hamoda, Schneiderman, Bouix, Pasternak, Rathi and Zafonte2012). In fact, one study reports initial indications of a developmental stall in white matter microstructure, with fractional anisotropy (FA) increasing over time in TDC but not in children with a complicated mild or moderate TBI (Bartnik-Olson et al., Reference Bartnik-Olson, Holshouser, Ghosh, Oyoyo, Nichols, Pivonka-Jones and Ashwal2021). However, the literature on white matter microstructure in pediatric mTBI is inconsistent (Jain, Das, Agrawal, Babal, & Purohit, Reference Jain, Das, Agrawal, Babal and Purohit2021) and, to date, it is largely unknown if alterations in white matter microstructure are present years after pediatric mTBI (Lindsey, Hodges, Greer, Wilde, & Merkley, Reference Lindsey, Hodges, Greer, Wilde and Merkley2021).

In this study, children with a history of mTBI were expected to exhibit worse task-based and parent-rated executive functioning, more impulsive behavior, and worse emotion-regulation when compared to TDC and OI controls based on the large and representative Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. Further, we explore whether the age at first mTBI has an influence on the differences from controls. Finally, we investigate whether children with a history of mTBI differ in white matter microstructure compared to the two control groups. Due to the lack of imaging literature in chronic pediatric mTBI, we expected lower FA and higher mean diffusivity (MD) in the investigated tracts, as is often the case for the chronic phase of adult mTBI (Lindsey et al., Reference Lindsey, Hodges, Greer, Wilde and Merkley2021).

Methods

Study design

The ABCD study is a prospective, longitudinal study funded by the National Institute of Health (NIH) and has been conducted at 21 sites in the United States. Children at the age of 9–10 years were recruited from schools with demographically diverse backgrounds and, with the study still ongoing, will be followed for 10 years with yearly follow-ups including various demographic, cognitive, behavioral, and neuroimaging assessments (Garavan et al., Reference Garavan, Bartsch, Conway, Decastro, Goldstein, Heeringa and Zahs2018; Volkow et al., Reference Volkow, Koob, Croyle, Bianchi, Gordon, Koroshetz and Weiss2018). Written consent was obtained from the parents. The total available sample size at the time of this analysis was 11 876 for the baseline assessment, 10 414 for the 2-year, and 6251 for the 3-year follow-up. For this study, ABCD Data release 4.0 is used for all data except for diffusion MRI, where data from release 3.0 were harmonized before release 4.0 was available. Raw data are available for researchers upon request at https://nda.nih.gov/abcd/.

Study sample

Mild traumatic brain injury

Parents completed the ABCD Parent Ohio State Traumatic Brain Injury Screen-Short Modified (OTBI; Corrigan & Bogner, Reference Corrigan and Bogner2007), which asks about a series of events relevant to TBI (e.g. ‘Has your child ever been hospitalized or treated in an emergency room following an injury to his/her head or neck?’ or ‘Has your child ever injured his/her head or neck in [one of several injury mechanisms]?’). If the item is endorsed, parents report if and for how long the child suffered from (1) loss of consciousness (LOC) or (2) amnesia and/or an altered mental state. Children were categorized as having sustained a ‘possible mTBI’ (amnesia/altered mental state without LOC) or ‘mTBI’ (with LOC) by the ABCD study team. For this study, both groups were included in the mTBI group based on current criteria for the clinical diagnosis of mTBI in children and adolescents in which LOC is not required (Kay et al., Reference Kay, Harrington, Adams, Anderson, Berrol, Cicerone and Malec1993). Parents then provided the age (in years) at which the event occurred. Children with a moderate or severe TBI before (n = 2) and after baseline (n = 1) were excluded.

Control group selection

Children with a history of mTBI were compared to both TDC and OI controls. Participants in the mTBI group were matched to the two different control groups for age, sex, family income, race, and study site using the ‘optimal matching’ from the MatchIt R-package (Ho, Imai, King, & Stuart, Reference Ho, Imai, King and Stuart2011). A ratio of 1:2 (1 mTBI case matched to 2 control cases for each control group) was chosen for each control group because only ~1200 children were eligible for the OI group and differing control group sizes would have limited comparability.

Missing data

Some children had missing data on covariates of interest (e.g., parents chose not to report their income), reducing the mTBI sample size to n = 406 (n TDC = 812, n OI = 812). Additionally, cognitive and behavioral outcome measures were assessed at follow-up, so that children would have sustained their first mTBI at least 2 years before. Because data were not available for all children at this point, the sample size of mTBI and control groups varies between outcome variables. Each respective sample size can be found in Table 1 and is depicted as a flowchart in online Supplementary Fig. S1.

Table 1. Overview of dependent variables

Note. An overview of dependent variables for the examined domains with the respective type of measurement, the time-point of assessment and the available sample size. mTBI, mild traumatic brain injury; TDC, typically developing children; OI, orthopedic injury; FU, follow-up; BDEFS, Barkley Deficits in Executive Functioning Scale; NIH Flanker, NIH Toolbox Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test; UPPS-P, UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale; BIS/BAS, Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Approach System; DERS-P, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; FA, fractional anisotropy; MD, mean diffusivity; CB, cingulum bundle (left/right included); SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus (left/right included); CC, corpus callosum (connecting hemispheres).

Measures

Demographic variables

Demographic measures include biological sex (female/male), age at baseline (in months), race (American Indian and Alaska Native/Asian/Black/Multiple/Native Hawaiian and Other Pacific Islander/Other/White), and total family income. Family income in the past year was reported on an ordinal scale from 1 (‘less than $5000’) to 10 (‘$200 000 and greater’) and used as a proxy for socio-economic status (SES). Body mass index (BMI) was calculated. Additionally, handedness was assessed using the Youth Edinburgh Handedness Inventory Short Form (EHIS; Oldfield, Reference Oldfield1971).

Cognitive and behavioral variables

Executive functioning. For executive functioning, two assessment modalities were used: The first assessment was the Barkley Deficits in Executive Functioning Scale (BDEFS; Barkley, Reference Barkley2012), which is a parent-rating scale of executive functioning in the activities of daily life. The short form used for the ABCD study is comprised of 14 items that are scored from 1 (‘never or rarely’) to 4 (‘very often’) and scores were summed for the analysis. The second assessment for executive functioning was the NIH Toolbox Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test (NIH Flanker), which is a neuropsychological test of attention and inhibition (Gershon et al., Reference Gershon, Wagster, Hendrie, Fox, Cook and Nowinski2013). Here, children focus on one stimulus while inhibiting the shifting of attention to other stimuli flanking the target. For the NIH toolbox, standard scores are available and therefore, the age-corrected score was used. Regarding the other cognitive/behavioral outcomes, no normed scores were provided by the ABCD study and raw scores were used instead.

Impulsivity. Two self-report measures were used to assess impulsivity. The Abbreviated Youth Version of the UPPS-P (urgency, premeditation, perseverance, sensation seeking, and positive urgency) Impulsive Behavior Scale (Watts, Smith, Barch, & Sher, Reference Watts, Smith, Barch and Sher2019) consists of 20 items rated from 1 (‘not at all like me’) to 4 (‘very much like me’). The sum of all items was used in this analysis. Additionally, the Fun Seeking subscale from the Youth Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Approach System (BIS/BAS; Carver & White, Reference Carver and White1994) was used as it provides a more nuanced assessment. This subscale of the BIS/BAS contains four items scored from 0 (‘not true’) to 3 (‘very true’) and is specifically related to reward reactivity and impulsivity (Smillie, Jackson, & Dalgleish, Reference Smillie, Jackson and Dalgleish2006).

Emotion regulation. Emotion regulation was assessed using the parent-report version of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS-P; Bunford et al., Reference Bunford, Dawson, Evans, Ray, Langberg, Owens and Allan2020). Only two of the four subscales (factors) target elements of control and self-regulation and were used in the current analysis: Factor 1 reflects feelings of losing control when faced with negative emotions (Catastrophize, 11 items), and Factor 4 reflects not being able to focus one's thoughts when experiencing strong emotions (Distracted, four items). The other two factors target the experience of negative secondary emotions (Negative secondary) and the recognition of one's own emotions (Attuned) and were not included in this study. Items were scored from 0 (‘almost never’) to 5 (‘almost always’).

Diffusion MRI and white matter microstructure

MRI testing in the ABCD study is performed according to a standardized protocol. Details on image acquisition are reported elsewhere (Casey et al., Reference Casey, Cannonier, Conley, Cohen, Barch, Heitzeg and Dale2018). Data with insufficient quality as assessed by experts were excluded (Hagler et al., Reference Hagler, Hatton, Cornejo, Makowski, Fair, Dick and Dale2019). Because diffusion MRI is particularly sensitive to non-linear site- and scanner-specific effects (Mirzaalian et al., Reference Mirzaalian, Ning, Savadjiev, Pasternak, Bouix, Michailovich and Rathi2016), additional harmonization was performed across different scanners before the data were pooled and analyzed statistically. To this end, the minimally processed dMRI data (3.0 release) were harmonized with our well-validated approach based on rotation invariant spherical harmonics (RISH, https://github.com/pnlbwh/dMRIharmonization; Cetin-Karayumak et al., Reference Cetin-Karayumak, Bouix, Ning, James, Crow, Shenton and Rathi2019, Reference Cetin-Karayumak, Di Biase, Chunga, Reid, Somes, Lyall and Kubicki2020). Details on the specifics of the harmonization used in the current study are reported by Cetin-Karayumak et al. (Reference Cetin-Karayumak, Zhang, Billah, Zekelman, Makris, Pieper and Rathi2023b). We note that diffusion data acquired on MRI scanners manufactured by Philips did not pass quality control criteria during harmonization (Cetin-Karayumak et al., Reference Cetin-Karayumak, Zhang, Billah, Zekelman, Makris, Pieper and Rathi2023b), which led to the exclusion of n = 38 children with mTBI, n = 75 TDC and n = 80 OI controls from imaging analyses. The final sample size for dMRI analyses used in this study was n = 321 children with mTBI (n = 636 TDC, n = 656 OI).

After obtaining the harmonized dMRI data, whole brain tractography was performed using an advanced, multi-tensor unscented Kalman filter (UKF, https://github.com/pnlbwh/ukftractography; Malcolm, Shenton, & Rathi, Reference Malcolm, Shenton and Rathi2010) for each subject under study. Tractography data were then automatically segmented into 73 white matter tracts using an anatomically curated atlas (https://github.com/SlicerDMRI/whitematteranalysis; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Wu, Norton, Rigolo, Rathi, Makris and O'Donnell2018). For this analysis, 13 tracts were selected based on their reported association with mTBI, executive functioning, and impulsivity (Cardenas-Iniguez et al., Reference Cardenas-Iniguez, Moore, Kaczkurkin, Meyer, Satterthwaite, Fair and Lahey2022; Lindsey et al., Reference Lindsey, Hodges, Greer, Wilde and Merkley2021; Owens et al., Reference Owens, Hyatt, Gray, Miller, Lynam, Hahn and Garavan2020), including the left and right cingulum bundle (CB), two posterior sections of the superior longitudinal fasciculus (left and right SLF, II and III), and the corpus callosum divided into seven sections (CC 1–7). FA (directionality of diffusion) and MD (mean diffusivity in all directions) are the most used measures of white matter pathology associated with mTBI (Shenton et al., Reference Shenton, Hamoda, Schneiderman, Bouix, Pasternak, Rathi and Zafonte2012) and were analyzed for each of the aforementioned tracts.

Statistical analyses

For comparability between measures, cognitive/behavioral variables and white matter microstructure were z-standardized before further analysis.

Group differences

All analyses were performed using R version 4.1.2 (R Core Team, 2021); required packages are listed in the publicly available R-script (https://github.com/anjabetz/ABCD–mTBI). Linear regression models were performed for each of the outcomes with group (mTBI as the reference, TDC and OI) as the independent variable and with age at baseline, sex, race, SES, and site as covariates. To test for group differences in FA and MD, BMI and handedness were included as additional covariates. p-Values were corrected for multiple comparisons using a false-discovery-rate of 0.05 (FDR; Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995) separately for cognitive/behavioral outcomes (12 comparisons), FA (26 comparisons), and MD (26 comparisons). F2 was calculated as an effect size (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988).

Age-at-injury

To examine whether group differences between mTBI and TDC were dependent on the age at which the mTBI was sustained, the mTBI group was divided into three groups based on their retrospective age at the time they sustained the first mTBI (age-at-time of injury in years grouped in: ‘0–3’, ‘4–7’, and ‘8–10’). The matched pairs were extracted and each age-at-injury group was compared to their respective matches from the TDC group. This kept the sample ratio at 1:2 (mTBI:TDC) and allowed us to investigate non-linear effects with a simple linear model. The age groups chosen were small enough to allow for investigation of sensitive periods but large enough to be interpreted in a developmental context. The OI control group was not included in this part of the analysis, because children in this group had sustained an injury at a certain age, but that age was not assessed in the ABCD study and could neither be controlled for nor grouped together.

Results

Demographics

A total of 448 children sustained an mTBI before the ABCD baseline assessment, of which 406 had complete data on covariates (i.e. age, sex, race, SES, and site). This cohort of mTBI subjects was used for the analyses and matched to TDC and OI controls (812 children in each control group). Demographic characteristics are summarized in Table 2. The groups did not differ in any of the demographical variables due to the matching procedure; a comparison of mTBI to the total ABCD baseline sample has already been reported (Dufour, Adams, Brody, Puente, & Gray, Reference Dufour, Adams, Brody, Puente and Gray2020).

Table 2. Demographic characteristics

Note. mTBI, mild traumatic brain injury; TDC, typically developing children; OI, orthopedic injury; NHPI, Native Hawaiian and other Pacific Islander; AIAN, American Indian and Alaska Native. Handedness % in right-/left-handed/mixed. Percentages were rounded and therefore may not add up to 100.

Group differences in cognition and behavior

Standardized regression coefficients with 95% confidence intervals are depicted in Fig. 1; coefficients, test statistics, degrees of freedom, corrected p-values, and effect sizes can be found in Table 3. The control groups did not differ on any outcome variables.

Figure 1. Differences in cognition and behavior. Estimate of the standardized β-coefficients for all cognitive and behavioral variables. These represent the estimated change from the mTBI group (reference, therefore here represented by the vertical 0-line) to the respective control group. Horizontal bars reflect the estimate's 95% confidence interval. Variables with confidence intervals not including 0 are considered significant (FDR-corrected p < 0.05).

Table 3. Inferential statistics for cognition and behavior

Note. Sample size of mTBI and respective control groups varies due to data availability at follow-up. mTBI is used as the reference for regression coefficients. df, Residual degrees of freedom; TDC, typically developing controls; OI, orthopedic injury; BDEFS, Barkley Deficits in Executive Functioning Scale; NIH Flanker, NIH Toolbox Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test; UPPS-P, UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale; BIS/BAS, Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Approach System Fun Seeking Scale; DERS-P, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. *Indicates significant p-values at p < 0.05 after correction.

Executive functioning

Children with a history of mTBI displayed significantly higher parent-rated executive dysfunction on the BDEFS than either of the control groups ($\hat{\beta }$TDC = -0.24, p TDC = 0.005$; \;\;\hat{\beta }$OI = -0.23, p OI = 0.007). The NIH Flanker task, as a neuropsychological test of executive functioning, did not differ among the groups ($\hat{\beta }$TDC = 0.01, p TDC = 0.937; $\hat{\beta }$OI = -0.01, p OI = 0.937).

Impulsivity

Impulsivity as assessed with the UPPS-P was significantly higher in the mTBI group compared to both control groups ($\hat{\beta }$TDC = -0.19, p TDC = 0.005; $\hat{\beta }$OI = -0.17, p OI = 0.009), as was the score on the BIS/BAS Fun Seeking Scale ($\hat{\beta }$TDC = -0.23, p TDC = 0.002; $\hat{\beta }$OI = -0.20, p OI = 0.005).

Emotion regulation

Regarding emotional self-regulation, the first DERS-P factor (Catastrophize) was significantly higher in the mTBI group compared to both control groups ($\hat{\beta }$TDC = -0.28, p TDC = 0.002; $\hat{\beta }$OI = -0.20, p OI = 0.016), as was the fourth factor (Distracted) ($\hat{\beta }$TDC = -0.24, p TDC = 0.006; $\hat{\beta }$OI = -0.16, p OI = 0.048).

Group differences in white matter microstructure

Analyses of white matter microstructure did not reveal any significant differences in FA or MD between the groups in the CB, the SLF, or the CC (all p-values > 0.05, see online Supplementary Tables S2 and S3).

Influence of age-at-injury

For this analysis, children with a history of mTBI were split into three age-at-injury categories: 0–3 years (n = 91), 4–7 years (n = 147), and 8–10 years (n = 166). These groups were compared to their respective matched TDC controls. At baseline, children had a mean time-since-injury of 46.31 (s.d. = 32.15) months.

The difference on the BDEFS was mainly driven by the group with an mTBI between the ages of 4-7 years ($\hat{\beta }$ = 0.37, p = 0.029). The difference on the BIS/BAS Fun Seeking Scale was driven by the group with their first mTBI between 8-10 years ($\hat{\beta }$ = 0.32, p = 0.029), while the difference on the UPPS-P was driven by the age group 0-3 ($\hat{\beta }$ = 0.41, p = 0.029). Differences in the DERS Factor 1 (Catastrophize) were driven by the age group 0-3 ($\hat{\beta }$ = 0.55, p = 0.035). No other age groups differed from controls on these measures. No significant differences depending on the age-at-injury were observed on the NIH Flanker Task and on the DERS Factor 4 (see online Supplementary Table S4). Effects of age-at-injury on cognitive and behavioral variables are depicted in Fig. 2.

Figure 2. Effect of age-at-injury on cognition and behavior. Comparison of three separate groups based on age-at-injury (0–3, 4–7, 8–10 years, each represented by the vertical 0 line) to their respective matched TDC control group. Estimate of the standardized β-coefficients for all cognitive and behavioral variables. *Indicate FDR-corrected p-values < 0.05.

Regarding white matter, no differences from controls emerged when examining different ages-at-injury (see online Supplementary Tables S5 and S6).

Discussion

Children with a history of mTBI showed worse executive function and emotion regulation as well as higher impulsivity compared to controls. In addition, the age of the child at the time of injury influenced which domain was affected by mTBI. That is, children showed more emotional dysregulation if they sustained an mTBI between the age of 0 and 3 years, more executive dysfunction if they sustained an mTBI between the age of 4 and 7 years, and more impulsivity than TDC if they sustained an mTBI between the age of 0 and 3 or 8 and 10 years. This study found no differences between groups in task-based executive functioning. Finally, there were no group differences in diffusion measures of the investigated white matter tracts (i.e., CB, CC, and SLF).

Executive function, impulsivity, and emotion-regulation

Children with a history of mTBI displayed more executive dysfunction, difficulties in emotion regulation, and impulsivity based on parent- and self-report compared to TDC and OI controls. In line with our results, Jones et al. (Reference Jones, Starkey, Barker-Collo, Ameratunga, Theadom, Pocock and Feigin2021) report difficulties in executive, emotional, and behavioral domains 7 years following an mTBI. Another study reports psycho-emotional problems that persist up to 2 years following a TBI (Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Montroy, Clark, Holubkov, Cox and Keenan2021). However, still other studies report behavioral difficulties that had either subsided 1 year after mTBI (Maillard-Wermelinger et al., Reference Maillard-Wermelinger, Yeates, Gerry Taylor, Rusin, Bangert, Dietrich and Wright2009) or parent-rated executive functioning that showed an improving trajectory 2 years following TBI (Keenan, Clark, Holubkov, Cox, & Ewing-Cobbs, Reference Keenan, Clark, Holubkov, Cox and Ewing-Cobbs2021). One possible explanation for these inconsistent findings in the literature may be that there is a non-linear course of recovery. That is, after an initial alleviation of symptoms, new problems may arise with new developmental milestones and increasing demands from the child's environment. Future studies using longitudinal study designs with multiple follow-up assessments over a longer period are needed to fully address the effect of mTBI on executive function, emotion regulation, and impulsivity, i.e. studies should not focus on only individuals in the acute phase of post-injury but also over a period of time as new developmental challenges emerge.

While there is executive dysfunction in children with a history of mTBI based on parent report, this study found no group differences in executive functioning as assessed by neuropsychological testing. This is in line with a previous report on the same dataset (Dufour et al., Reference Dufour, Adams, Brody, Puente and Gray2020), which also did not find associations between mTBI and principal cognitive components derived from the NIH toolbox tests. Of note, neuropsychological test performance has previously been shown to account for less than 20% of the variance in parent-ratings after adolescent mTBI (Lace et al., Reference Lace, Merz, Grant, Teague, Aylward, Dorflinger and Gfeller2019). This does, however, not necessarily mean that they differ regarding quality of assessment. Rather, they appear to be tapping into different aspects with neuropsychological tests reflecting the ‘cold’ (i.e., cognitive, controlled, and under laboratory conditions) side of executive functioning while ratings provided by parents may also include ‘hot’ elements (e.g., emotional distress, risky behavior in everyday life) (Lace et al., Reference Lace, Merz, Grant, Teague, Aylward, Dorflinger and Gfeller2019). In the context of this study, this may explain why difficulties in emotion-regulation were found in addition to higher parent-rated executive dysfunction; both would include day-to-day abilities of self-regulation. To capture the wide concept of executive functioning, studies should ideally include multi-modal testing.

Of note, tests often isolate one cognitive ability while ratings can capture a broader scope of everyday function. This study used a neuropsychological test of inhibition, because inhibition as opposed to other forms of executive functioning has shown a sensitivity for age-at-injury effects (Resch et al., Reference Resch, Anderson, Beauchamp, Crossley, Hearps, van Heugten and Catroppa2019). However, problems with inhibition have previously been shown to subside even while other deficits persist (Keenan et al., Reference Keenan, Clark, Holubkov, Cox and Ewing-Cobbs2021). It is therefore possible that other domains of executive functioning would have captured differences when using task-based measures.

Of further note, while performance on neuropsychological testing did not differ between mTBI and control groups, the reported behavioral alterations may indirectly affect a child's academic functioning. That is, more impulsive and unregulated behavior could lead to difficulties in coordinating homework and studying, which children need to do more independently as they develop. Academic achievement in childhood and adolescence is also associated with self-reported executive functioning (van Tetering, Jolles, van der Elst, & Jolles, Reference van Tetering, Jolles, van der Elst and Jolles2022) and teacher-reported self-regulation (van Tetering, de Groot, & Jolles, Reference van Tetering, de Groot and Jolles2018). Further investigations using external criteria such as grades in addition to parent-report and neuropsychological testing are therefore warranted.

Effect of age-at-injury

In pediatric TBI, there are differing perspectives on the influence of age-at-injury. Assuming a linear relationship, a younger age is often considered a risk factor for worse outcomes following a moderate-severe TBI (Goh et al., Reference Goh, Looi, Goh, Sultana, Goh, Lee and Chong2021). However, after a mild TBI, such clear associations have not been demonstrated (Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou, & Rosenfeld, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2005). An alternative view is the model of sensitive developmental periods (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Spencer-Smith, Leventer, Coleman, Anderson, Williams and Jacobs2009; Zamani, Ryan, Wright, Caeyenberghs, & Semple, Reference Zamani, Ryan, Wright, Caeyenberghs and Semple2020), suggesting that age-at-injury does not show a ‘one size fits all’ relationship for cognitive, behavioral, and structural domains. Rather, an mTBI likely impacts children differentially based on developmental stage. Functions that are rapidly developing at time of injury are at increased risk for long-term impairments (Zamani et al., Reference Zamani, Ryan, Wright, Caeyenberghs and Semple2020). Interestingly, in this study, higher emotional dysregulation was present in children who sustained an mTBI between the ages of 0 and 3. This is in line with a review article on self-regulation and effortful control, which points out that the first few years of life are most relevant for children's emotional self-regulation. Afterwards, interindividual differences remain more stable (Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Eggum, Reference Eisenberg, Spinrad and Eggum2010). With regard to executive functioning, most studies show that associated cognitive abilities develop most rapidly during the ages of 5 to 8 (Goldstein & Naglieri, Reference Goldstein and Naglieri2014). While there is less research on behavioral executive functioning, the results of this study match this age range by showing that parent-rated executive functioning is mainly affected by children who experience an mTBI between the ages of 4 and 7. Impulsivity, on the other hand, was affected if the mTBI was sustained both between the ages of 0 and 3 or 8 and 10. In the literature, the development of impulsivity cannot be pinpointed as clearly as for other functions. A head injury before the age of 5 has previously been shown to attenuate developmental declines in impulsivity (Fullerton, Jackson, Tuvblad, Raine, & Baker, Reference Fullerton, Jackson, Tuvblad, Raine and Baker2019). On the other hand, studies showed that the later elementary school years (i.e., 8-11 years) are a transitional period for the development of impulse control (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Meng, Yu, Chen, Hung, Lin and Chi2021). A more complex explanation may be that the UPPS reflects general impulsivity, which starts to decline already in early childhood (Schwartz, Connolly, & Alsolami, Reference Schwartz, Connolly and Alsolami2022) and therefore was implicated by mTBI before 3 years. The BIS/BAS Fun Seeking scale would reflect sensation seeking specifically, which still appears to increase in late childhood (Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Connolly and Alsolami2022) and could be affected by the mTBI between 8 and 10 years. Taken together, our results emphasize the importance of taking age-at-injury into account as a complex, presumably non-linear influence.

White matter microstructure

To investigate the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the behavioral differences reported above, we examined white matter microstructure in several tracts that have previously been associated with mTBI, executive functioning, and the related behavior. To account for non-linear scanner effects, we used the newly available diffusion MRI data that were harmonized across study sites (Cetin-Karayumak et al., Reference Cetin-Karayumak, Zhang, Billah, Zekelman, Makris, Pieper and Rathi2023b). The effectiveness of this harmonization approach has been demonstrated in several neuroimaging studies (e.g., Cetin Karayumak, Kubicki, & Rathi, Reference Cetin Karayumak, Kubicki, Rathi, Frangi, Schnabel, Davatzikos, Alberola-López and Fichtinger2018, Cetin-Karayumak et al., Reference Cetin-Karayumak, Di Biase, Chunga, Reid, Somes, Lyall and Kubicki2020, Reference Cetin-Karayumak, Lyall, Di Biase, Seitz-Holland, Zhang, Kelly and Kubicki2023a; De Luca et al., Reference De Luca, Cetin-Karayumak, Leemans, Rathi, Swinnen, Gooijers and Pasternak2022; Di Biase et al., Reference Di Biase, Zalesky, Cetin-Karayumak, Rathi, Lv, Boerrigter and Cropley2021; Elad et al., Reference Elad, Cetin-Karayumak, Zhang, Cho, Lyall, Seitz-Holland and Pasternak2021; Lv et al., Reference Lv, Di Biase, Cash, Cocchi, Cropley, Klauser and Zalesky2021; Seitz et al., Reference Seitz, Cetin-Karayumak, Lyall, Pasternak, Baxi, Vangel and Kubicki2021; Ye et al., Reference Ye, Zalesky, Lv, Loi, Cetin-Karayumak, Rathi and Di Biase2021).

There were no differences detected between children with mTBI and the two control groups in FA and MD. Previous studies have reported mixed results regarding the effect of pediatric mTBI on white matter microstructure (Jain et al., Reference Jain, Das, Agrawal, Babal and Purohit2021). It is possible that mild injuries like the ones included in this study do not lead to changes in white matter microstructure (Ware et al., Reference Ware, Shukla, Goodrich-Hunsaker, Lebel, Wilde, Abildskov and Yeates2020). Alternatively, any initial changes may already have subsided by the time of scanning (Van Beek, Vanderauwera, Ghesquière, Lagae, & De Smedt, Reference Van Beek, Vanderauwera, Ghesquière, Lagae and De Smedt2015) with children having sustained their first mTBI an average of 4 years before baseline. Further, if changes in white matter microstructure are highly individual, they also may not be captured using group comparisons. In future studies, comparing individual cases to an atlas of normative data may be more sensitive for detecting white matter alterations at the level of the individual (Bouix et al., Reference Bouix, Pasternak, Rathi, Pelavin, Zafonte and Shenton2013).

Limitations

There are several limitations to the study that need to be considered. First, this analysis is cross-sectional. The trajectory of the reported difficulties could not be assessed, so it remains to be clarified whether they begin shortly after an injury and persist or whether they occur only with progressing development. Moreover, pre-injury characteristics were not available and thus could not be controlled for. It is possible that group differences were at least partly present before children sustained an mTBI. We addressed this by matching the groups carefully for age, sex, race, income, and study site, statistically controlling for these factors, and including an OI control group. This should reduce the influence of demographic, pre-injury, and general injury characteristics, but cannot completely exclude them. The analysis would have profited from an OI group that was also divided according to age-at-injury of the mTBI group. Unfortunately, this information was not provided by the ABCD medical history questionnaire. In this study, information on mTBI was based on parent-report. Therefore, the data may be subject to recall bias regarding injuries at all, their mechanism, the subsequent symptoms, or the child's age. However, this approach also offers the strength of capturing injuries that may have been on the milder spectrum and thus, not presented to a physician at time of injury. The corresponding effect sizes were very small, which is in part expected in such large population studies (Owens et al., Reference Owens, Potter, Hyatt, Albaugh, Thompson, Jernigan and Garavan2021). Given the mild injuries, the time since injury and the fact that based on the literature, only a subset of participants may experience lasting symptoms (Babcock et al., Reference Babcock, Byczkowski, Wade, Ho, Mookerjee and Bazarian2013), we still consider them clinically meaningful and important to report. Finally, children from certain racial and lower socio-economic backgrounds were under-represented in the current sample and had disproportionately high missing outcome values. While this was not to be avoided, it may limit generalizability and also prohibited the investigation of more complex interaction effects.

Conclusion

Children with a history of mTBI showed more executive dysfunction, more impulsivity, and more difficulties in emotional self-regulation compared to TDC and OI controls. Moreover, age-at-injury has an effect suggesting that there may be sensitive periods in brain development. White matter microstructure did not differ between groups. To address this, future studies should consider investigating white matter alterations at the level of the individual.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724000229

Acknowledgements

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10 000 children age 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study® is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators.

Funding statement

This project is supported by ERC Starting Grant Nr. 804326 (I. K. K. as principal investigator).

Competing interests

None.

References

Anderson, V., Catroppa, C., Morse, S., Haritou, F., & Rosenfeld, J. (2005). Functional plasticity or vulnerability after early brain injury? Pediatrics, 116(6), 13741382. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1728CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V., Spencer-Smith, M., Leventer, R., Coleman, L., Anderson, P., Williams, J., … Jacobs, R. (2009). Childhood brain insult: Can age at insult help us predict outcome? Brain, 132(1), 4556. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn293CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babcock, L., Byczkowski, T., Wade, S. L., Ho, M., Mookerjee, S., & Bazarian, J. J. (2013). Predicting postconcussion syndrome after mild traumatic brain injury in children and adolescents who present to the emergency department. JAMA Pediatrics, 167(2), 156161. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.434CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babikian, T., Merkley, T., Savage, R. C., Giza, C. C., & Levin, H. (2015). Chronic aspects of pediatric traumatic brain injury: Review of the literature. Journal of Neurotrauma, 32(23), 18491860. doi: 10.1089/neu.2015.3971CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babikian, T., Satz, P., Zaucha, K., Light, R., Lewis, R. S., & Asarnow, R. F. (2011). The UCLA longitudinal study of neurocognitive outcomes following mild pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(5), 886895. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000907CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkley, R. A. (2012). Barkley deficits in executive functioning scale – children and adolescents (BDEFS-CA). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bartnik-Olson, B., Holshouser, B., Ghosh, N., Oyoyo, U. E., Nichols, J. G., Pivonka-Jones, J., … Ashwal, S. (2021). Evolving white matter injury following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 38(1), 111121. doi: 10.1089/neu.2019.6574CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300. Abgerufen von http://www.jstor.org/stable/2346101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouix, S., Pasternak, O., Rathi, Y., Pelavin, P. E., Zafonte, R., & Shenton, M. E. (2013). Increased gray matter diffusion anisotropy in patients with persistent post-concussive symptoms following mild traumatic brain injury. PLoS ONE, 8(6), e66205. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0066205CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bunford, N., Dawson, A. E., Evans, S. W., Ray, A. R., Langberg, J. M., Owens, J. S., … Allan, D. M. (2020). The difficulties in emotion regulation scale–parent report: A psychometric investigation examining adolescents with and without ADHD. Assessment, 27(5), 921940. doi: 10.1177/1073191118792307CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardenas-Iniguez, C., Moore, T. M., Kaczkurkin, A. N., Meyer, F. A. C., Satterthwaite, T. D., Fair, D. A., … Lahey, B. B. (2022). Direct and indirect associations of widespread individual differences in brain white matter microstructure with executive functioning and general and specific dimensions of psychopathology in children. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 7(4), 362375. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.11.007Google ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 319333. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, B. J., Cannonier, T., Conley, M. I., Cohen, A. O., Barch, D. M., Heitzeg, M. M., … Dale, A. M. (2018). The adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 4354. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cetin-Karayumak, S., Bouix, S., Ning, L., James, A., Crow, T., Shenton, M., … Rathi, Y. (2019). Retrospective harmonization of multi-site diffusion MRI data acquired with different acquisition parameters. NeuroImage, 184, 180200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.073CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cetin-Karayumak, S., Di Biase, M. A., Chunga, N., Reid, B., Somes, N., Lyall, A. E., … Kubicki, M. (2020). White matter abnormalities across the lifespan of schizophrenia: A harmonized multi-site diffusion MRI study. Molecular Psychiatry, 25(12), 32083219. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0509-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cetin Karayumak, S., Kubicki, M., & Rathi, Y. (2018). Harmonizing diffusion MRI data across magnetic field strengths. In Frangi, A. F., Schnabel, J. A., Davatzikos, C., Alberola-López, C. & Fichtinger, G. (Hrsg.), Medical image computing and computer assisted intervention – MICCAI 2018 (S. 116124). Cham: Springer International Publishing. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-00931-1_14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cetin-Karayumak, S., Lyall, A. E., Di Biase, M. A., Seitz-Holland, J., Zhang, F., Kelly, S., … Kubicki, M. (2023a). Characterization of the extracellular free water signal in schizophrenia using multi-site diffusion MRI harmonization. Molecular Psychiatry, 28(5), 20302038. doi: 10.1038/s41380-023-02068-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cetin-Karayumak, S., Zhang, F., Billah, T., Zekelman, L., Makris, N., Pieper, S., … Rathi, Y. (2023b). Harmonized diffusion MRI data and white matter measures from the baseline adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study [bioRxiv Preprint]. Neuroscience. doi: 10.1101/2023.04.04.535587Google Scholar
Chadwick, L., Roth, E., Minich, N. M., Taylor, H. G., Bigler, E. D., Cohen, D. M., … Yeates, K. O. (2021). Cognitive outcomes in children with mild traumatic brain injury: An examination using the National Institutes of Health toolbox cognition battery. Journal of Neurotrauma, 38(18), 25902599. doi: 10.1089/neu.2020.7513CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, H. Y., Meng, L. F., Yu, Y., Chen, C. C., Hung, L. Y., Lin, S. C., … Chi, H. J. (2021). Developmental traits of impulse control behavior in school children under controlled attention, motor function, and perception. Children, 8(10), 922. doi: 10.3390/children8100922CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. London: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203771587Google Scholar
Corrigan, J. D., & Bogner, J. (2007). Initial reliability and validity of the Ohio State University TBI identification method. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 22(6), 318329. doi: 10.1097/01.HTR.0000300227.67748.77CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Luca, A., Cetin-Karayumak, S., Leemans, A., Rathi, Y., Swinnen, S., Gooijers, J., … Pasternak, O. (2022). Cross-site harmonization of multi-shell diffusion MRI measures based on rotational invariant spherical harmonics (RISH). NeuroImage, 259, 119439. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119439Google Scholar
Di Biase, M. A., Zalesky, A., Cetin-Karayumak, S., Rathi, Y., Lv, J., Boerrigter, D., … Cropley, V. L. (2021). Large-scale evidence for an association between peripheral inflammation and white matter free water in schizophrenia and healthy individuals. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 47(2), 542551. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa134CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dufour, S. C., Adams, R. S., Brody, D. L., Puente, A. N., & Gray, J. C. (2020). Prevalence and correlates of concussion in children: Data from the adolescent brain cognitive development study. Cortex, 131, 237250. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.07.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Eggum, N. D. (2010). Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children's maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 495525. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elad, D., Cetin-Karayumak, S., Zhang, F., Cho, K. I. K., Lyall, A. E., Seitz-Holland, J., … Pasternak, O. (2021). Improving the predictive potential of diffusion MRI in schizophrenia using normative models – towards subject-level classification. Human Brain Mapping, 42(14), 46584670. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25574CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emery, C. A., Barlow, K. M., Brooks, B. L., Max, J. E., Villavicencio-Requis, A., Gnanakumar, V., … Yeates, K. O. (2016). A systematic review of psychiatric, psychological, and behavioural outcomes following mild traumatic brain injury in children and adolescents. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 61(5), 259269. doi: 10.1177/0706743716643741CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ewing-Cobbs, L., Montroy, J. J., Clark, A. E., Holubkov, R., Cox, C. S., & Keenan, H. T. (2021). As time goes by: Understanding child and family factors shaping behavioral outcomes after traumatic brain injury. Frontiers in Neurology, 12, 687740. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.687740CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, N. P., & Banich, M. T. (2019). Questionnaires and task-based measures assess different aspects of self-regulation: Both are needed. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(49), 2439624397. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1915315116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fullerton, A. F., Jackson, N. J., Tuvblad, C., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2019). Early childhood head injury attenuates declines in impulsivity and aggression across adolescent development in twins. Neuropsychology, 33(8), 10351044. doi: 10.1037/neu0000570CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gagner, C., Landry-Roy, C., Bernier, A., Gravel, J., & Beauchamp, M. H. (2018). Behavioral consequences of mild traumatic brain injury in preschoolers. Psychological Medicine, 48(9), 15511559. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717003221CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garavan, H., Bartsch, H., Conway, K., Decastro, A., Goldstein, R. Z., Heeringa, S., … Zahs, D. (2018). Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 1622. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershon, R. C., Wagster, M. V., Hendrie, H. C., Fox, N. A., Cook, K. F., & Nowinski, C. J. (2013). NIH toolbox for assessment of neurological and behavioral function. Neurology, 80(11, S3), S2S6. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872e5fCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giza, C. C., Kolb, B., Harris, N. G., Asarnow, R. F., & Prins, M. L. (2009). Hitting a moving target: Basic mechanisms of recovery from acquired developmental brain injury. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 12(5), 255268. doi: 10.1080/17518420903087558CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goh, M. S. L., Looi, D. S. H., Goh, J. L., Sultana, R., Goh, S. S. M., Lee, J. H., & Chong, S. L. (2021). The impact of traumatic brain injury on neurocognitive outcomes in children: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 92(8), 847853. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2020-325066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, S., & Naglieri, J. A. (Hrsg.). (2014). Handbook of executive functioning. New York, NY: Springer New York. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagler, D. J., Hatton, S. N., Cornejo, M. D., Makowski, C., Fair, D. A., Dick, A. S., … Dale, A. M. (2019). Image processing and analysis methods for the adolescent brain cognitive development study. NeuroImage, 202, 116091. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116091Google Scholar
Ho, D. E., Imai, K., King, G., & Stuart, E. A. (2011). MatchIt: Nonparametric preprocessing for parametric causal inference. Journal of Statistical Software, 42(8), 128. doi: 10.18637/jss.v042.i08CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jain, B., Das, A. K., Agrawal, M., Babal, R., & Purohit, D. K. (2021). Implications of DTI in mild traumatic brain injury for detecting neurological recovery and predicting long-term behavioural outcome in paediatric and young population – A systematic review. Child's Nervous System, 37(8), 24752486. doi: 10.1007/s00381-021-05240-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, K. M., Starkey, N., Barker-Collo, S., Ameratunga, S., Theadom, A., Pocock, K., … Feigin, V. L. (2021). Parent and teacher-reported child outcomes seven years after mild traumatic brain injury: A nested case control study. Frontiers in Neurology, 12, 683661. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.683661Google Scholar
Kay, T., Harrington, D., Adams, R., Anderson, T., Berrol, S., Cicerone, K., … Malec, J. (1993). Definition of mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 8(3), 8687. doi: 10.1097/00001199-199309000-00010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, H. T., Clark, A. E., Holubkov, R., Cox, C. S., & Ewing-Cobbs, L. (2021). Trajectories of children's executive function after traumatic brain injury. JAMA Network Open, 4(3), e212624. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.2624CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Königs, M., Pouwels, P. J., Ernest van Heurn, L., Bakx, R., Jeroen Vermeulen, R., Carel Goslings, J., … Oosterlaan, J. (2018). Relevance of neuroimaging for neurocognitive and behavioral outcome after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 12(1), 2943. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9673-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lace, J. W., Merz, Z. C., Grant, A. F., Teague, C. L., Aylward, S., Dorflinger, J., & Gfeller, J. D. (2019). Relationships between the BRIEF/BRIEF-SR and performance-based neuropsychological tests in adolescents with mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Pediatric Neuropsychology, 5(4), 163176. doi: 10.1007/s40817-019-00074-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lebel, C., & Beaulieu, C. (2011). Longitudinal development of human brain wiring continues from childhood into adulthood. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(30), 1093710947. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5302-10.2011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, H. S., & Hanten, G. (2005). Executive functions after traumatic brain injury in children. Pediatric Neurology, 33(2), 7993. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2005.02.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebel, S. W., Edwards, K. A. M., & Broglio, S. P. (2021). Sensation-seeking and impulsivity in athletes with sport-related concussion. Current Psychiatry Reports, 23(4), 15. doi: 10.1007/s11920-021-01232-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindsey, H. M., Hodges, C. B., Greer, K. M., Wilde, E. A., & Merkley, T. L. (2021). Diffusion-weighted imaging in mild traumatic brain injury: A systematic review of the literature. Neuropsychology Review, 33(1), 42121. doi: 10.1007/s11065-021-09485-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lv, J., Di Biase, M., Cash, R. F. H., Cocchi, L., Cropley, V. L., Klauser, P., … Zalesky, A. (2021). Individual deviations from normative models of brain structure in a large cross-sectional schizophrenia cohort. Molecular Psychiatry, 26(7), 35123523. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-00882-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maillard-Wermelinger, A., Yeates, K. O., Gerry Taylor, H., Rusin, J., Bangert, B., Dietrich, A., … Wright, M. (2009). Mild traumatic brain injury and executive functions in school-aged children. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 12(5), 330341. doi: 10.3109/17518420903087251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malcolm, J. G., Shenton, M. E., & Rathi, Y. (2010). Filtered multitensor tractography. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 29(9), 16641675. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2010.2048121CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mannix, R., O'Brien, M. J., & Meehan, W. P. (2013). The epidemiology of outpatient visits for minor head injury: 2005 to 2009. Neurosurgery, 73(1), 129134. doi: 10.1227/01.neu.0000429846.14579.41CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mirzaalian, H., Ning, L., Savadjiev, P., Pasternak, O., Bouix, S., Michailovich, O., … Rathi, Y. (2016). Inter-site and inter-scanner diffusion MRI data harmonization. NeuroImage, 135, 311323. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual research review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(4), 361383. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, M. M., Hyatt, C. S., Gray, J. C., Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Hahn, S., … Garavan, H. (2020). Neuroanatomical correlates of impulsive traits in children aged 9 to 10. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(8), 831844. doi: 10.1037/abn0000627CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, M. M., Potter, A., Hyatt, C. S., Albaugh, M., Thompson, W. K., Jernigan, T., … Garavan, H. (2021). Recalibrating expectations about effect size: A multi-method survey of effect sizes in the ABCD study. PLoS ONE, 16(9), e0257535. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0257535CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinto, P. S., Poretti, A., Meoded, A., Tekes, A., & Huisman, T. A. G. M. (2012). The unique features of traumatic brain injury in children. Review of the characteristics of the pediatric skull and brain, mechanisms of trauma, patterns of injury, complications and their imaging findings – part 1. Journal of Neuroimaging, 22(2), e1e17. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2011.00688.xGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Resch, C., Anderson, V. A., Beauchamp, M. H., Crossley, L., Hearps, S. J. C., van Heugten, C. M., … Catroppa, C. (2019). Age-dependent differences in the impact of paediatric traumatic brain injury on executive functions: A prospective study using susceptibility-weighted imaging. Neuropsychologia, 124, 236245. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.12.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saarinen, M., Isaksson, N., Himanen, L., Erkinjuntti, N., Vahlberg, T., Koskinen, S., … Lähdesmäki, T. (2022). Cognitive functions and symptoms predicting later use of psychiatric services following mild traumatic brain injury in school-age. Brain Injury, 37(5), 388396. doi: 10.1080/02699052.2022.2145365CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, J., Hayward, K. S., Brown, K. E., Zwicker, J. G., Ponsford, J., Van Donkelaar, P.. (2018). Imaging in pediatric concussion: A systematic review. Pediatrics, 141(5), e20173406. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-3406CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, J. A., Connolly, E. J., & Alsolami, A. (2022). Within-individual changes in impulsivity and sensation seeking from childhood to early adulthood and educational attainment. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 51(11), 21902204. doi: 10.1007/s10964-022-01663-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seitz, J., Cetin-Karayumak, S., Lyall, A., Pasternak, O., Baxi, M., Vangel, M., … Kubicki, M. (2021). Investigating sexual dimorphism of human white matter in a harmonized, multisite diffusion magnetic resonance imaging study. Cerebral Cortex, 31(1), 201212. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serpa, R. O., Ferguson, L., Larson, C., Bailard, J., Cooke, S., Greco, T., … Prins, M. L. (2021). Pathophysiology of pediatric traumatic brain injury. Frontiers in Neurology, 12, 696510. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.696510Google Scholar
Shenton, M. E., Hamoda, H. M., Schneiderman, J. S., Bouix, S., Pasternak, O., Rathi, Y., … Zafonte, R. (2012). A review of magnetic resonance imaging and diffusion tensor imaging findings in mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 6(2), 137192. doi: 10.1007/s11682-012-9156-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smillie, L. D., Jackson, C. J., & Dalgleish, L. I. (2006). Conceptual distinctions among Carver and White's (1994) BAS scales: A reward-reactivity versus trait impulsivity perspective. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(5), 10391050. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Beek, L., Vanderauwera, J., Ghesquière, P., Lagae, L., & De Smedt, B. (2015). Longitudinal changes in mathematical abilities and white matter following paediatric mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 29(13–14), 17011710. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2015.1075172CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Tetering, M. A. J., de Groot, R. H. M., & Jolles, J. (2018). Teacher-evaluated self-regulation is related to school achievement and influenced by parental education in schoolchildren aged 8–12: A case–control study. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 438. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00438CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Tetering, M. A. J., Jolles, J., van der Elst, W., & Jolles, D. D. (2022). School achievement in early adolescence is associated with students’ self-perceived executive functions. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 734576. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.734576CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volkow, N. D., Koob, G. F., Croyle, R. T., Bianchi, D. W., Gordon, J. A., Koroshetz, W. J., … Weiss, S. R. B. (2018). The conception of the ABCD study: From substance use to a broad NIH collaboration. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 47. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ware, A. L., Shukla, A., Goodrich-Hunsaker, N. J., Lebel, C., Wilde, E. A., Abildskov, T. J., … Yeates, K. O. (2020). Post-acute white matter microstructure predicts post-acute and chronic post-concussive symptom severity following mild traumatic brain injury in children. NeuroImage: Clinical, 25, 102106. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.102106Google Scholar
Watts, A. L., Smith, G. T., Barch, D. M., & Sher, K. J. (2019). Factor structure, measurement and structural invariance, and external validity of an abbreviated youth version of the UPPS-P impulsive behavior scale. Psychological Assessment, 32(4), 336347. doi: 10.1037/pas0000791CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ye, H., Zalesky, A., Lv, J., Loi, S. M., Cetin-Karayumak, S., Rathi, Y., … Di Biase, M. A. (2021). Network analysis of symptom comorbidity in schizophrenia: Relationship to illness course and brain white matter microstructure. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 47(4), 11561167. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbab015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zamani, A., Ryan, N. P., Wright, D. K., Caeyenberghs, K., & Semple, B. D. (2020). The impact of traumatic injury to the immature human brain: A scoping review with insights from advanced structural neuroimaging. Journal of Neurotrauma, 37(5), 724738. doi: 10.1089/neu.2019.6895CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, F., Wu, Y., Norton, I., Rigolo, L., Rathi, Y., Makris, N., & O'Donnell, L. J. (2018). An anatomically curated fiber clustering white matter atlas for consistent white matter tract parcellation across the lifespan. NeuroImage, 179, 429447. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.06.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Overview of dependent variables

Figure 1

Table 2. Demographic characteristics

Figure 2

Figure 1. Differences in cognition and behavior. Estimate of the standardized β-coefficients for all cognitive and behavioral variables. These represent the estimated change from the mTBI group (reference, therefore here represented by the vertical 0-line) to the respective control group. Horizontal bars reflect the estimate's 95% confidence interval. Variables with confidence intervals not including 0 are considered significant (FDR-corrected p < 0.05).

Figure 3

Table 3. Inferential statistics for cognition and behavior

Figure 4

Figure 2. Effect of age-at-injury on cognition and behavior. Comparison of three separate groups based on age-at-injury (0–3, 4–7, 8–10 years, each represented by the vertical 0 line) to their respective matched TDC control group. Estimate of the standardized β-coefficients for all cognitive and behavioral variables. *Indicate FDR-corrected p-values < 0.05.

Supplementary material: File

Betz et al. supplementary material

Betz et al. supplementary material
Download Betz et al. supplementary material(File)
File 184 KB