Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T05:36:48.525Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Peace of Mind, Academic Motivation, and Academic Achievement in Filipino High School Students

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 April 2017

Jesus Alfonso D. Datu*
Affiliation:
The University of Hong Kong (Hong Kong)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jesus Alfonso D. Datu. Division of Learning, Development, and Diversity – Faculty of Education, The University of Hong Kong (Hong Kong). E-mail: jess.datu@yahoo.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Recent literature has recognized the advantageous role of low-arousal positive affect such as feelings of peacefulness and internal harmony in collectivist cultures. However, limited research has explored the benefits of low-arousal affective states in the educational setting. The current study examined the link of peace of mind (PoM) to academic motivation (i.e., amotivation, controlled motivation, and autonomous motivation) and academic achievement among 525 Filipino high school students. Findings revealed that PoM was positively associated with academic achievement β = .16, p < .05, autonomous motivation β = .48, p < .001, and controlled motivation β = .25, p < .01. As expected, PoM was negatively related to amotivation β = –.19, p < .05, and autonomous motivation was positively associated with academic achievement β = .52, p < .01. Furthermore, the results of bias-corrected bootstrap analyses at 95% confidence interval based on 5,000 bootstrapped resamples demonstrated that peace of mind had an indirect influence on academic achievement through the mediating effects of autonomous motivation. In terms of the effect sizes, the findings showed that PoM explained about 1% to 18% of the variance in academic achievement and motivation. The theoretical and practical implications of the results are elucidated.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2017 

Positive education emphasizes the significance of social and psychological factors that facilitate key learning outcomes (Seligman, Ernst, Gillham, Reivich, & Linkins, Reference Seligman, Ernst, Gillham, Reivich and Linkins2009). Emergence of this paradigm has led to empirical investigations which focused on positive psychological constructs that serve as antecedents of adaptive academic functioning (Datu & Valdez, Reference Datu and Valdez2016; Heffner & Antaramian, Reference Heffner and Antaramian2016; Lewis, Huebner, Reschly, & Valois, Reference Lewis, Huebner, Reschly and Valois2009). One notable psychological variable that has been linked to a wide range of academic outcomes was positive affect. Positive affect pertains to individuals’ experience desirable emotional states (e.g., happiness, excitement, and elation) at various points in time (MacKinnon et al., Reference MacKinnon, Jorm, Christensen, Korten, Jacomb and Rodgers1999).

Previous literature has revealed that positive affect plays an important role in fostering student success. Supporting this viewpoint, positive affect has been linked to greater academic engagement (Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Huebner, Reschly and Valois2009), meaning in life (Datu, Reference Datu2016), and intrinsic motivation (Isen & Reeve, Reference Isen and Reeve2005). Positive affect was also weakly associated with academic achievement (Cheng & Furnham, Reference Cheng and Furnham2002; Nickerson, Diener, & Schwarz, Reference Nickerson, Diener and Schwarz2011). Yet, positive affect (i.e., cheerfulness) has not substantially associated with academic ability in undergraduate students (Fox & Spector, Reference Fox and Spector2000; Kashdan & Yuen, Reference Kashdan and Yuen2007).

Whereas these studies pointed to the salient function of positive affect in the academic context, a common limitation of these investigations involved their greater focus on medium-arousal (content and happy) to high-arousal (elated and excited) positive emotions. As past literature showed that there are marked cultural variations in cognition, emotion, and motivation (Markus & Kitayama, Reference Markus and Kitayama1991; Morling, Kitayama, & Miyamoto, Reference Morling, Kitayama and Miyamoto2002), there is a need to assess if what intensity (e.g., low-arousal and high-arousal) of positive emotions may optimize beneficial outcomes in various cultural settings. To strengthen the argument on cultural differences in emotional expression, the affect valuation theory (Tsai, Knutson, & Fung, Reference Tsai, Knutson and Fung2006) argues that medium-arousal to high-arousal affect are more relevant for individuals who are immersed in individualist contexts because unrestricted expression of dispositions, wants, and values is highly rewarded in their cultural setting. Alternatively, low-arousal positive affect (calm and relaxed) are more suitable for individuals who are embedded in collectivist settings given that constant adjustment to others’ needs and wants is important in such cultural contexts.

Recognizing these cultural variations on the expression of affective states, Lee, Lin, Huang, and Fredrickson (Reference Lee, Lin, Huang and Fredrickson2013) developed the “peace of mind” construct. Peace of mind (PoM) refers to the extent to which individuals feel internal peace, coherence, and comfort. Lee et al. (Reference Lee, Lin, Huang and Fredrickson2013) found that PoM is applicable for Chinese and European-American students. Consistent with their hypotheses, Chinese scored higher than European-American participants on PoM which implied that low-arousal positive emotions like PoM may be more generalizable to interdependent cultural contexts.

Even with the seeming value of examining the role of PoM in the academic setting, very few studies have investigated the beneficial impact of PoM especially in collectivist societies. Aside from the study of Lee et al. (Reference Lee, Lin, Huang and Fredrickson2013), only Datu, Valdez, and King (Reference Datu, King and Valdez2016) have looked at the relations of PoM with an academic outcome (e.g., academic engagement). Other empirical investigations focused on the association of PoM with organizational outcomes like organizational citizenship behaviors (Ariyabuddhiphongs & Pratchawittayagorn, 2014) and task performance (Anjum, Ahmed, & Karim, Reference Anjum, Ahmed and Karim2014).

It is possible that more-culturally sensitive positive affective states like PoM may promote optimal outcomes in the educational context. The broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, Reference Fredrickson2001) posits that positive affective states (e.g., happiness, excitement, and calmness) are valuable in that they broaden range of thought-action preparations which enable individuals to acquire functional psychological resources. Individuals who experience positive affect are likely to embody approach types of motivation and wide range of adaptive outcomes. Supporting this theoretical conjecture, previous studies have shown that positive affect was linked to intrinsic motivation (Isen & Reeve, Reference Isen and Reeve2005) and academic achievement (Cheng & Furnham, Reference Cheng and Furnham2002; Nickerson et al., Reference Nickerson, Diener and Schwarz2011). Whereas PoM is regarded as one form of positive affect, Lee and colleagues (2013) note that positive affect measures in previous literature concentrated on the high-arousal feelings while limited research has been done to assess the role of low-arousal positive affective states like PoM. Furthermore, they argue that individuals in collectivist societies are more likely than those in individualist societies to experience low-arousal emotions.

It is therefore logical to assert that students’ PoM may be linked to greater academic achievement as this affective state may enable them to embody approach forms of academic motivation (i.e., autonomous motivation). However, no investigation (except for the study of Datu, Valdez, & King, Reference Datu, King and Valdez2016) has examined the role of low-arousal positive affect like PoM in the academic setting. Clearly, more studies are needed to assess the theoretical linkage of peace of mind to academic outcomes.

Therefore, the main objectives of the current research were: a.) to assess the association of PoM with academic motivation and achievement; and b.) to examine the mediating effects of academic motivational orientations (i.e., amotivation, controlled and autonomous motivation) on the link between PoM and academic achievement among Filipino high school students.

Peace of mind and psychological outcomes

Past empirical literature has shown that PoM has been associated with a number of positive academic and work-related outcomes. PoM has been positively associated with academic engagement (Datu, Valdez et al., Reference Datu, King and Valdez2016), organizational citizenship behavior (Ariyabuddhiphongs & Pratchawittayagorn, 2014), positivity ratio (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Lin, Huang and Fredrickson2013), life satisfaction (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Lin, Huang and Fredrickson2013), and task performance (Anjum et al., Reference Anjum, Ahmed and Karim2014). On the other hand, PoM has been negatively correlated with anxiety and depression (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Lin, Huang and Fredrickson2013). Noticeably, while previous studies have shown that PoM was linked to well-being outcomes, only the study of Datu, Valdez et al. (Reference Datu, King and Valdez2016) has explored the association of PoM with academic outcomes.

Academic motivation and academic outcomes

The self-determination theory (SDT) posits that the quality of motivational orientations that students embody place an important role in shaping key psychological outcomes (Ryan & Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2000). Whereas intrinsic motivation is considered the most optimal form of motivation (compared to extrinsic motivational orientations and amotivation), the framework argues that there are forms of extrinsic motivation (i.e., identified regulation and integrated regulation) that may promote adaptive behaviors.

Consistent with the taxonomy of Vansteenkiste, Zhou, Lens, and Soenens (Reference Vansteenkiste, Zhou, Lens and Soenens2005), combining identified regulation (performing an action because individuals consider them as important and valuable) and integrated regulation (doing a behavior because individuals see them as parallel to their goals and values) constitutes autonomous motivation which is a self-determined motivational orientation. Alternatively, clustering together the introjected regulation (doing a behavior because individuals feel uncomfortable or obliged to perform it) and external regulation (performing an act to get an incentive or to avoid a penalty) forms controlled motivation. Amotivation refers to absence of drive to perform an action.

Previous studies have demonstrated that these motivational orientations (amotivation, controlled and autonomous motivation) shaped key academic outcomes. Supporting this conjecture, autonomous motivation was positively associated with academic performance (e.g., Guay, Ratelle, Roy, & Litalien, Reference Guay, Ratelle, Roy and Litalien2010; Kusurkar, Ten Cate, Vos, Westers, & Croiset, Reference Kusurkar, Ten Cate, Vos, Westers and Croiset2013), academic adjustment (Ratelle, Guay, Vallerand, Larose, & Senécal, Reference Ratelle, Guay, Vallerand, Larose and Senécal2007), and study strategies (Kusurkar et al., Reference Kusurkar, Ten Cate, Vos, Westers and Croiset2013).

Theoretical perspective

The broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, Reference Fredrickson2001) posits that positive emotions are very adaptive because they expand individuals’ thought-action repertoire which is optimal for building key psychological resources. In the academic setting, scholars have shown that positive emotions optimize academic engagement (Datu et al., Reference Datu, King and Valdez2017; Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Huebner, Reschly and Valois2009), intrinsic motivational orientation (Isen & Reeve, Reference Isen and Reeve2005), and academic achievement (Nickerson et al., Reference Nickerson, Diener and Schwarz2011). However, it seems that past research has mainly concentrated on assessing beneficial impact of high-arousal (e.g., elation) and medium-arousal (e.g., joy) positive emotions on educational outcomes.

Solely focusing on evaluating the link of medium to high-arousal positive emotions on academic functioning in a collectivist society may be problematic for at least two reasons. First, researchers have demonstrated that medium to high-arousal positive emotions operate as more adaptive emotional states in Western societies than in collectivist settings (Morling et al., Reference Morling, Kitayama and Miyamoto2002; Tsai et al., Reference Tsai, Knutson and Fung2006). Second, individuals in collectivist contexts are more likely to realize the value of expressing low-arousal positive emotions (Tsai et al., Reference Tsai, Knutson and Fung2006). These findings indicate that more research is desired to explore the relationship of low-arousal positive emotions to academic outcomes.

With reference to the existing literature about the educational benefits of positive emotions, it seems that having PoM may be linked to higher autonomous motivation and academic achievement. This is because high levels of PoM can potentially broaden students’ mind set and expand behavioral strategies that are beneficial for the attainment of successful learning outcomes (e.g., Guay et al., Reference Guay, Ratelle, Roy and Litalien2010; Kusurkar et al., Reference Kusurkar, Ten Cate, Vos, Westers and Croiset2013). This conjecture was consistent with the findings from previous literature regarding the association of positive emotions with intrinsic motivation (Isen & Reeve, Reference Isen and Reeve2005) and academic achievement (Nickerson et al., Reference Nickerson, Diener and Schwarz2011).

Furthermore, it is also possible that PoM can promote externally-oriented reasons for studying (controlled motivation). It is likely that having high degree of PoM may broaden students’ mind sets which will enable them to realize the importance of studying to gain external rewards and avoid feeling guilty about not studying (controlled motivation). In other words, controlled motivation may serve as an academic resource that can potentially develop through experiencing PoM. The conjecture on controlled motivation as an academic resource corroborated with the previous literature which demonstrates that socially-oriented motivation may be beneficial for students who are embedded in collectivist contexts (Cheng & Lam, Reference Cheng and Lam2013; King & McInerney, Reference King and McInerney2014).

The study posited that low-arousal positive emotions like PoM may be linked to higher academic achievement as these emotional states can promote adaptive motivational orientations (e.g., autonomous and controlled motivations), which in turn, may be associated with greater achievement. Put simply, autonomous motivation, controlled motivation, and amotivation can serve as specific mechanisms as to why PoM may be related to higher academic performance.

The philippine setting

The current research examined the association of PoM with the academic motivation and achievement of Filipino high school students in the Philippine context. Examining the relations of PoM to academic outcomes is an important direction because previous research indicates that individuals in the Philippine society are more likely to espouse an interdependent than an independent self-view (Datu, Reference Datu2015; Hofstede, Reference Hofstede2001). Existing studies show that Filipino students may significantly benefit from experiencing high levels of socially-oriented happiness as this construct has been linked to greater positive affect and life satisfaction (Datu et al., Reference Datu, King and Valdez2017). However, it seems that very limited evidence has been generated regarding the nomological network of PoM in the Philippine setting because to date, only the investigation of Datu, Valdez et al. (Reference Datu, King and Valdez2016) have assessed the association of PoM with relevant academic outcomes like student engagement.

The primary aim of the current study was to examine the influence of PoM on academic motivation (i.e., amotivation, controlled motivation, and autonomous motivation) and academic achievement in Filipino high school students. It also assessed the mediating role of academic motivation on the relations between PoM and academic motivation.

The present research addressed notable gaps in the existing PoM literature for at least third significant reasons. First, whereas past studies have explored the association of PoM with academic engagement (Datu, Valdez et al., Reference Datu, King and Valdez2016) and well-being outcomes (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Lin, Huang and Fredrickson2013), the current study assessed the link of PoM to academic motivation and academic achievement. To the best of my knowledge, this was the first research which examined the theoretical linkage of PoM with academic motivation and academic achievement. Second, the study explored the indirect influence of PoM on academic achievement through the mediating role of academic motivational orientations to offer a potential mechanism on why PoM cultivates successful learning in a collectivist context. Third, whereas the research of Lee et al. (Reference Lee, Lin, Huang and Fredrickson2013) assessed the role of peace of mind across Taiwanese and American undergraduate students, findings from their three inter-related studies may not be always generalizable in other non-undergraduate student populations. Investigating the association of PoM with key academic outcomes in Filipino high school students is an important research aim because previous studies have indicated that students who belong to this developmental period are vulnerable to experience a significant decline in academic motivation (Fredricks & Eccles, Reference Fredricks and Eccles2002; Van de gaer et al., 2009).

The present study tested the following conjectures:

  • H1: PoM would positively predict academic achievement.

  • H2: PoM would positively predict autonomous and controlled motivation.

  • H3: PoM would negatively predict amotivation.

  • H4: Autonomous motivation and controlled motivation would positively predict academic achievement.

  • H5: Amotivation would negatively predict academic achievement.

  • H6: Amotivation, controlled motivation, and autonomous motivation would mediate the association between PoM and academic achievement.

Methods

Participants

The total sample was composed of 525 Filipino high school students from a private secondary school institution in Metro Manila which is considered an urban area. The ages of the participants ranged from 11 to 19 (M = 13.85; SD = 1.27). There were 268 girls and 257 boys participants in the study. In terms of the academic year level, the sample was comprised of 201 Grade 7, 122 Grade 8, 59 Grade 9, and 142 Grade 10 students. However, 1 student failed to report the year level.

Instruments

Peace of mind

The Peace of Mind Scale (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Lin, Huang and Fredrickson2013) is a 7-item questionnaire which measured the degree to which the participants feel calmness, internal peace, and harmony. Sample items in the scale involved: “My mind is free and peaceful” and “I have peace and harmony in mind”. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the scale in the current study was .72.

Academic motivation

The modified version of Academic Motivation Scale (Caleon et al., Reference Caleon, Wui, Tan, Chiam, Soon and King2015) was utilized in the present research to assess the extent to which the participants espouse amotivation, controlled motivation, and autonomous motivation. Here are the sample items in each dimension: amotivation (“I don’t know; I can’t understand what I am doing in school” and “In the past, I had good reasons for going in school; however, now I don’t know whether I should continue”); controlled motivation (“Because I want to get a more prestigious job” and “To prove to myself that I am capable of completing my high school education”); and autonomous motivation (“Because I feel happy and satisfied while learning new things” and “Because I think that secondary school education will help me better prepare for the job that I like”). The dimensions of the scale had the following Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient: .85 (amotivation), .77 (controlled), and .84 (autonomous).

The English versions of the Peace of Mind Scale and Academic Motivation Scale were administered to the participants because past investigations have shown that the English versions of psychological questionnaires are considered valid even in the Philippine context (e.g., Datu, Reference Datu2015; Datu et al., Reference Datu, King and Valdez2017.

Procedures

Before administering the packet of surveys to the participants, the author sought approval from the principal of the secondary school institution where the study was conducted. Passive consent forms were administered to the students across year levels. Afterwards, students were requested to answer the questionnaires and to report their respective general quarterly average during the second grading quarter based on their report cards. The quarterly average ranged from 0 to 100 with higher marks indicating better academic performance.

Data analysis

The descriptive statistical values like mean, standard deviation, and measures of normality were computed. As no missing data was detected, inferential statistical analyses were carried out. Then, correlational coefficients among PoM, academic motivation, and academic achievement were also calculated. To assess the indirect effects of PoM on academic achievement via amotivation, controlled motivation, and autonomous motivation, regression analyses was conducted based on the INDIRECT Macro (Preacher & Hayes, Reference Preacher and Hayes2008) using SPSS v23. Bias-corrected bootstrapping analysis at 95% confidence interval (CI) was carried out with reference to 5,000 bootstrapped samples. The coefficient of determination values (R 2) were also reported to assess the effect sizes between the explanatory and outcome variables in the study.

Results

Preliminary data analysis

The results of descriptive statistics and correlations analysis are shown in Table 1. Consistent with H1, PoM was positively associated with academic achievement. PoM was also positively correlated with autonomous motivation and controlled motivation while PoM was negatively associated with amotivation. Furthermore, autonomous motivation was positively correlated with academic achievement but amotivation was negatively linked to academic achievement.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlational coefficients between the variables

Note: **p < .001, *p < .05.

Mediation analyses

The results of regression analyses were reported in Table 2. H1 was supported since PoM positively predicted academic achievement β = .16, p < .05. H2 was confirmed as PoM positively predicted autonomous motivation, β = .48, p < .001. Consistent with H3, PoM negatively predicted amotivation, β = –.19, p < .05. Moreover, PoM positively predicted controlled motivation, β = .25, p < .01. A review of the coefficient of determination (R 2) values indicated that PoM explained around 1.0% to 18% of the variance in students’ amotivation, controlled motivation, and autonomous motivation.

Table 2. Standardized regression weights of the regression analyses

* p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Partly supporting H4, autonomous motivation positively predicted academic achievement, β = .52, p < .01 while controlled motivation did not significantly predict achievement. H5 was not supported as amotivation did not predict achievement. The three academic motivational dimensions (i.e., amotivation, controlled motivation, and autonomous motivation) accounted for 1.90% of the variance in academic achievement. The results of bias-corrected bootstrapping analysis based on 5,000 bootstrapped samples at 95% CI supported the hypothesized indirect effects of peace of mind on academic achievement via autonomous motivation (See Table 3).

Table 3. Results of Indirect Effects of Peace of Mind on Academic Achievement via Academic Motivation

Discussion

The primary objective of the current investigation was to examine the associations of peace of mind with academic motivation and academic achievement. The mediating influence of academic motivation on the link between PoM and academic achievement was also assessed. Most aspects of the results confirmed the theoretical conjectures in the present study.

Consistent with H1 and H2, PoM was positively associated with academic achievement and autonomous motivation. H3 was also confirmed as PoM was negatively linked to amotivation. These results indicate that students who feel internal peace and harmony are more likely to get higher grades and espouse self-determined form of academic motivation. This corroborated the extant body of knowledge regarding the advantageous relations of positive affect with key academic outcomes such as academic achievement (Cheng & Furnham, Reference Cheng and Furnham2002; Nickerson et al., Reference Nickerson, Diener and Schwarz2011), intrinsic motivation (Isen & Reeve, Reference Isen and Reeve2005), and academic engagement (Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Huebner, Reschly and Valois2009). Yet, the study contributes to the literature through assessing the association of low-arousal positive affect like PoM with academic achievement and academic motivation in the academic context.

Supporting H4, autonomous motivation was positively associated with academic achievement. Hence, it appears that when students engage in academic tasks that are consistent with their aspiration, goals and values, it is likely that they can achieve higher grades. This was consistent with the theoretical postulations of the self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2000) and the findings from past investigations regarding the psychological benefits autonomous motivation on various educational outcomes such as academic performance and academic adjustment (i.e., Guay et al., Reference Guay, Ratelle, Roy and Litalien2010; Kusurkar et al., Reference Kusurkar, Ten Cate, Vos, Westers and Croiset2013; Ratelle et al., Reference Ratelle, Guay, Vallerand, Larose and Senécal2007).

The most notable theoretical contribution of the present study, however, points to the indirect influence of peace of mind to academic achievement through the mediating roles of autonomous motivation. This result seems to suggest that higher peace of mind could be associated with greater academic performance because this low-arousal affective state may be linked to adaptive forms of motivation (autonomous motivation). In other words, academic motivation can explain why peace of mind may be associated with higher academic achievement. Evidently, this provides support on the broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, Reference Fredrickson2001) which argues that positive affect may broaden action-thought repertoire that build adaptive resources and optimize key psychological outcomes.

Surprisingly, PoM was positively associated with controlled academic motivation which provided full support on H2. This implies that students with high levels of peace of mind would perform academic tasks because of external rewards and perceived sense of obligation. Whereas the self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2000) posits that autonomous motivation is a more adaptive than controlled motivation, some scholars assert that even controlled motivation may play an important role in facilitating key educational outcomes in collectivist cultures (Cheng & Lam, Reference Cheng and Lam2013; King & McInerney, Reference King and McInerney2014). To a large extent, this is because students in interdependent contexts also see the significance of embodying more extrinsic forms of motivation to achieve vital cultural goals (e.g., preserving harmonious relationship with significant others).

Furthermore, the study showed that controlled motivation and amotivation did not predict academic achievement. These findings suggest that extrinsic motivational orientations (controlled motivation) and lack of drive to study (motivation) may not be linked to academic performance which confirmed results from previous studies on the lack of association among such motivational types and educational outcomes (Areepattamannil, Freeman, & Klinger, Reference Areepattamannil, Freeman and Klinger2011; Baker, Reference Baker2004). However, it is likely that two reasons may have shaped the non-significant associations of controlled motivation and amotivation with achievement. First, it may be possible that these motivational orientations are indirectly linked to academic achievement through the mediating effects of academic variables (e.g., self-regulation or academic engagement). Second, the use of subjective measure of academic achievement in the present study may have resulted in biased estimates of relationships between motivation and academic performance.

The effect sizes found as regards to the associations among PoM, academic motivation, and achievement in the study ranged from 1% to 18%. Although the effect sizes may indicate a relatively small degree of relationships among the variables, the values are comparable to what have been found in previous research regarding the link between personality variables and academic outcomes (Poropat, Reference Poropat2009). Hence, the results of the current investigation may still offer potential contributions to the existing literature regarding the correlates/predictors of academic performance.

The present study had some limitations. First, the cross-sectional nature of the current investigation poses restraints in terms of drawing causal inferences about the relations among PoM, academic motivation, and academic achievement. Future researchers are recommended to use longitudinal designs (i.e., cross-lagged panel or prospective longitudinal designs) to examine the causal ordering between PoM and academic outcomes. Second, the present study relied on self-report data to assess the variables of interest (e.g., peace of mind) which may be susceptible to common method variance. Future investigations are encouraged to use other-report data (peer-report and teacher-report data) to address this limitation. Third, the present research only recruited Filipino students from one high school institution which may offer limited generalizability in other cultures. Future studies can consider recruiting samples from other collectivist and individualist societies to the test the cross-cultural generalizability of the linkage between peace of mind and academic outcomes. Fourth, the study only focused on detecting the association of PoM with academic motivation and achievement. Future research can address this through exploring the linkage of low-arousal positive affective states to other academic outcomes like academic resilience and learning strategies.

Nonetheless, the present study had key theoretical and practical implications. In terms of theory, the investigation addressed research gaps in the positive affect literature through showing that low-arousal positive affect like PoM can be linked to academic achievement through the mediating effects of academic motivation, unlike previous studies which examined the relations of PoM to limited psychological outcomes such as academic engagement (Datu, Valdez et al., Reference Datu, King and Valdez2016) and well-being indices (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Lin, Huang and Fredrickson2013). Clearly, this corroborates the positive education paradigm (Seligman et al., Reference Seligman, Ernst, Gillham, Reivich and Linkins2009) which postulates that positive psychological states may promote better learning processes. Concerning practice, findings of the current research call for the need to conceptualize and carry out psychological interventions that can optimize low-arousal positive affect like peace of mind in the academic context. This is because cultivating these types of emotions may be associated with adaptive educational outcomes especially in collectivist sociocultural settings. Teachers are also recommended to create learning contexts that will enable students to feel calm and relaxed while performing various academic tasks.

References

Anjum, M. A., Ahmed, S. J., & Karim, J. (2014). Do psychological capabilities really matter? The combined effects of psychological capital and peace of mind on work centrality and in-role performance. Pakistan Journal of Commerce and Social Sciences, 8, 502520.Google Scholar
Areepattamannil, S., Freeman, J. G., & Klinger, D. A. (2011). Intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and academic achievement among Indian adolescents in Canada and India. Social Psychology of Education, 14, 427439. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-011-9155-1 Google Scholar
Ariyabuddhiphongs, V., & Pratchawittayagorn, A. (2014). Peace of mind and organizational citizenship behavior: The mediating effect of loving kindness. Archive for the Psychology of Religion, 36, 233252. https://doi.org/10.1163/15736121-12341284 Google Scholar
Baker, S. R. (2004). Intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivational orientations: Their role in university adjustment, stress, well-being, and subsequent academic performance. Current Psychology, 23, 189202. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-004-1019-9 Google Scholar
Caleon, I. S., Wui, M. G. L., Tan, J. P. L., Chiam, C. L., Soon, T. C., & King, R. B. (2015). Cross-cultural validation of the Academic Motivation Scale: A Singapore investigation. Child Indicators Research, 8, 925942. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-014-9298-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, H., & Furnham, A. (2002). Personality, peer relations, and self-confidence as predictors of happiness and loneliness. Journal of Adolescence, 25, 327339. https://doi.org/10.1006/jado.2002.0475 Google Scholar
Cheng, R. W., & Lam, S. F. (2013). The interaction between social goals and self-construal on achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38, 136148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.01.001 Google Scholar
Datu, J. A. D. (2015). Validating the revised self-construal scale in the Philippines. Current Psychology, 34, 626633. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-014-9275-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Datu, J. A. D. (2016). The synergistic interplay between positive emotions and maximization enhances meaning in life: A study in a collectivist context. Current Psychology, 35, 459466. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-015-9314-1 Google Scholar
Datu, J. A. D., & Valdez, J. P. M. (2016). Psychological capital predicts academic engagement and well-being in Filipino high school students. The Asia Pacific Education Researcher, 25, 399405. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40299-015-0254-1 Google Scholar
Datu, J. A. D., King, R. B., & Valdez, J. P. M. (2016). The benefits of socially-oriented happiness: Validation of the interdependent happiness scale in the Philippines. Child Indicators Research, 9, 631649. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-015-9333-3 Google Scholar
Datu, J. A. D., King, R. B., & Valdez, J. P. M. (2017). The academic rewards of socially-oriented happiness: Interdependent happiness promotes engagement. Journal of School Psychology, 61, 1931. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2016.12.004 Google Scholar
Datu, J. A. D., Valdez, J. P. M., & King, R. B. (2016). Peace of mind promotes academic engagement: Cross-sectional and longitudinal evidence. (Manuscript submitted for publication).Google Scholar
Fox, S., & Spector, P. E. (2000). Relations of emotional intelligence, practical intelligence, general intelligence, and trait affectivity with interview outcomes: It’s not all just ‘G’. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 203220. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2%3C203::AID-JOB38%3E3.3.CO;2-Q 3.0.CO;2-Z>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). Children’s competence and value beliefs from childhood through adolescence: Growth trajectories in two male-sex-typed domains. Developmental Psychology, 38, 519533.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218226. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218 Google Scholar
Guay, F., Ratelle, C. F., Roy, A., & Litalien, D. (2010). Academic self-concept, autonomous academic motivation, and academic achievement: Mediating and additive effects. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 644653. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.08.001 Google Scholar
Heffner, A. L., & Antaramian, S. P. (2016). The role of life satisfaction in predicting student engagement and achievement. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 16811701. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9665-1 Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences – Second edition: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations. London, UK: Sage.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M., & Reeve, J. (2005). The influence of positive affect on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Facilitating enjoyment of play, responsible work behavior, and self-control. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 295323. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-006-9019-8 Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., & Yuen, M. (2007). Whether highly curious students thrive academically depends on perceptions about the school learning environment: A study of Hong Kong adolescents. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 260270. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007-9074-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, R. B., & McInerney, D. M. (2014). Culture’s consequences on student motivation: Capturing universality and specificity through personal investment theory. Educational Psychologist, 49, 175198. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2014.926813 Google Scholar
Kusurkar, R. A., Ten Cate, T. J., Vos, C. M. P., Westers, P., & Croiset, G. (2013). How motivation affects academic performance: A structural equation modelling analysis. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 18(1), 5769. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-012-9354-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, Y. C., Lin, Y. C., Huang, C. L., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). The construct and measurement of peace of mind. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14, 571590. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-012-9343-5 Google Scholar
Lewis, A. D., Huebner, E. S., Reschly, A. L., & Valois, R. F. (2009). The incremental validity of positive emotions in predicting school functioning. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27, 397408. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282908330571 Google Scholar
MacKinnon, A., Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Korten, A. E., Jacomb, P. A., & Rodgers, B. (1999). A short form of the positive and negative affect schedule: Evaluation of factorial validity and invariance across demographic variables in a community sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 405416. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00251-7 Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224253. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224 Google Scholar
Morling, B., Kitayama, S., & Miyamoto, Y. (2002). Cultural practices emphasize influence in the United States and adjustment in Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 311323. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167202286003 Google Scholar
Nickerson, C., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (2011). Positive affect and college success. Journal of Happiness Studies, 12, 717746. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-010-9224-8 Google Scholar
Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 322338. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014996 Google Scholar
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879891. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 Google Scholar
Ratelle, C. F., Guay, F., Vallerand, R. J., Larose, S., & Senécal, C. (2007). Autonomous, controlled, and amotivated types of academic motivation: A person-oriented analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 734746. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.4.734 Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 6878. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68 Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P., Ernst, R. M., Gillham, J., Reivich, K., & Linkins, M. (2009). Positive education: Positive psychology and classroom interventions. Oxford Review of Education, 35, 293311. https://doi.org/10.1080/03054980902934563 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H. (2006). Cultural variation in affect valuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 288307. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.288 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van de gaer, E., De Fraine, B., Pustjens, H., Van Damme, J., De Munter, A., & Onghena, P. (2009). School effects on the development of motivation toward learning tasks and the development of academic self-concept in secondary education: A multivariate latent growth curve approach. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 20, 235253. https://doi.org/10.1080/09243450902883920 Google Scholar
Vansteenkiste, M., Zhou, M., Lens, W., & Soenens, B. (2005). Experiences of autonomy and control among Chinese learners: Vitalizing or immobilizing? Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 468483. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.468 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlational coefficients between the variables

Figure 1

Table 2. Standardized regression weights of the regression analyses

Figure 2

Table 3. Results of Indirect Effects of Peace of Mind on Academic Achievement via Academic Motivation