Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T11:18:45.346Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Elucidating the role of negative parenting in the genetic v. environmental influences on adult psychopathic traits

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 August 2021

Hailey L. Dotterer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Alexandra Y. Vazquez
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA
Luke W. Hyde
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA Survey Research Center of the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Craig S. Neumann
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA
Pekka Santtila
Affiliation:
NYU-ECNU Institute for Social Development, NYU Shanghai, Shanghai, China
Patrizia Pezzoli
Affiliation:
Institute of Mental Health Research, University of Ottawa, Ontario, CA, Canada Faculty of Medicine, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada
Ada Johansson
Affiliation:
Faculty of Arts, Psychology, and Theology, Åbo Akademi University, Turku, Finland
S. Alexandra Burt*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA
*
Author for correspondence: S. Alexandra Burt, E-mail: burts@msu.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Psychopathic traits involve interpersonal manipulation, callous affect, erratic lifestyle, and antisocial behavior. Though adult psychopathic traits emerge from both genetic and environmental risk, no studies have examined etiologic associations between adult psychopathic traits and experiences of parenting in childhood, or the extent to which parenting practices may impact the heritability of adult psychopathic traits using a genetically-informed design.

Methods

In total, 1842 adult twins from the community reported their current psychopathic traits and experiences of negative parenting during childhood. We fit bivariate genetic models to the data, decomposing the variance within, and the covariance between, psychopathic traits and perceived negative parenting into their genetic and environmental components. We then fit a genotype × environment interaction model to evaluate whether negative parenting moderated the etiology of psychopathic traits.

Results

Psychopathic traits were moderately heritable with substantial non-shared environmental influences. There were significant associations between perceived negative parenting and three of four psychopathy facets (interpersonal manipulation, erratic lifestyle, antisocial tendencies, but not callous affect). These associations were attributable to a common non-shared environmental pathway and not to overlapping genetic effects. Additionally, we found that primarily shared environmental influences were stronger on psychopathic traits for individuals with a history of greater negative parenting.

Conclusions

Utilizing a genetically-informed design, we found that both genetic and non-shared environmental factors contribute to the emergence of psychopathic traits. Moreover, perceptions of negative parenting emerged as a clear environmental influence on the development of interpersonal, lifestyle, and antisocial features of psychopathy.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Psychopathic traits, including superficial charm, callousness, irresponsibility, and poor impulse control, are strongly predictive of violence, criminality, and recidivism (Hare & Neumann, Reference Hare and Neumann2008; Olver et al., Reference Olver, Neumann, Sewall, Lewis, Hare and Wong2018; Reidy et al., Reference Reidy, Kearns, DeGue, Lilienfeld, Massetti and Kiehl2015). Though historically thought to be a highly genetic/heritable disorder (Skeem, Polaschek, Patrick, & Lilienfeld, Reference Skeem, Polaschek, Patrick and Lilienfeld2011), researchers have moved to a more complex and nuanced understanding of the etiology of psychopathy, emphasizing the role of both genetic and environmental influences (Viding & McCrory, Reference Viding and McCrory2018). Indeed, although psychopathic traits are moderately heritable (e.g. A = 24–67%; Rhee & Waldman, Reference Rhee and Waldman2002), there is extensive evidence that they are also impacted by environmental factors (Tuvblad & Baker, Reference Tuvblad and Baker2011; Tuvblad, Fanti, Andershed, Colins, & Larsson, Reference Tuvblad, Fanti, Andershed, Colins and Larsson2017; Viding & McCrory, Reference Viding and McCrory2012; Waldman, Rhee, LoParo, & Park, Reference Waldman, Rhee, LoParo, Park and Patrick2018).

One potentially important environmental influence in the development of psychopathy is harsh parenting. Harsh parenting has been robustly linked to broader antisocial behavior in youth (Loeber, Burke, & Pardini, Reference Loeber, Burke and Pardini2009; Moffitt, Reference Moffitt2018; Waller, Gardner, & Hyde, Reference Waller, Gardner and Hyde2013) and adults (Farrington, Reference Farrington2000; Loeber & Hay, Reference Loeber and Hay1997; Moffitt, Reference Moffitt2018). Moreover, accumulating research in youth shows that harsh parenting is a robust predictor of callous-unemotional (CU) traits (Waller et al., Reference Waller, Gardner and Hyde2013). CU traits are a downward extension of interpersonal and affective components of psychopathy (Salekin, Reference Salekin2017) and are a risk factor for adult psychopathy (Lynam, Caspi, Moffitt, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber, Reference Lynam, Caspi, Moffitt, Loeber and Stouthamer-Loeber2007). Thus, although research on parental influences on CU traits can inform our understanding of the etiology of psychopathy, research is needed to confirm the role that parents play in the development of the broader manifestation of psychopathy in adulthood.

Consistent with youth studies of CU traits, emerging research suggests that parenting practices are correlated with psychopathic traits in adult offspring (Eisenbarth, Krammer, Edwards, Kiehl, & Neumann, Reference Eisenbarth, Krammer, Edwards, Kiehl and Neumann2018; Piquero et al., Reference Piquero, Farrington, Fontaine, Vincent, Coid and Ullrich2012). However, these findings may reflect a genotype–environment correlation (rGE). As parenting itself is moderately heritable (Klahr & Burt, Reference Klahr and Burt2014), parents with genetic risk for psychopathic traits may be more likely to utilize harsh parenting practices (passive rGE) (Beaver et al., Reference Beaver, da Silva Costa, Poersch, Freddi, Stelmach, Connolly and Schwartz2014; Cox, Kopkin, Rankin, Tomeny, & Coffey, Reference Cox, Kopkin, Rankin, Tomeny and Coffey2018). Alternately, children with genetic risk for psychopathic traits may evoke harsher parenting reactions (evocative rGE) (Hawes, Dadds, Frost, & Hasking, Reference Hawes, Dadds, Frost and Hasking2011). Research is thus needed to determine the extent to which associations between parenting and subsequent psychopathic traits are genetic v. environmental in origin.

Several studies of youth have used genetically informed designs to elucidate the origins of the association between parenting and CU traits. For example, positive parenting has been shown to predict CU traits using an adoption design that eliminates passive rGE (Hyde et al., Reference Hyde, Waller, Trentacosta, Shaw, Neiderhiser, Ganiban and Leve2016). Parental harshness and warmth were also associated with CU traits using a twin difference design that indexes non-shared environmental influences (Waller, Hyde, Klump, & Burt, Reference Waller, Hyde, Klump and Burt2018). In contrast, a longitudinal twin difference study found that differences in negative parental discipline (age 7) did not predict differences in CU traits (age 12), when accounting for earlier levels of CU traits (Viding, Fontaine, Oliver, & Plomin, Reference Viding, Fontaine, Oliver and Plomin2009). Moreover, other studies have suggested that parenting may moderate the etiology of CU traits. For example, Hyde et al. (Reference Hyde, Waller, Trentacosta, Shaw, Neiderhiser, Ganiban and Leve2016) found that positive parenting by adoptive mothers buffered the effects of genetic risk for CU traits inherited from biological mothers. Similarly, a recent twin study by Henry et al. (Reference Henry, Dionne, Viding, Vitaro, Brendgen, Tremblay and Boivin2018) found that positive parenting moderated the heritability of CU traits, such that CU traits were less heritable in the presence of positive parenting (i.e. positive parenting buffered against genetic risk).

Collectively, these studies suggest that parenting may be an environmental influence on CU traits and may moderate the genetic influences on the development of CU traits. Critically, however, most of the prior genetically informed studies have examined the impact of positive parenting practices on CU traits (e.g. Henry et al., Reference Henry, Dionne, Viding, Vitaro, Brendgen, Tremblay and Boivin2018; Hyde et al., Reference Hyde, Waller, Trentacosta, Shaw, Neiderhiser, Ganiban and Leve2016). This is an important distinction since positive and negative parenting are not mirror images of one another (i.e. positive parenting is not the absence of harsh parenting but the presence of warmth and praise, etc.). Consistent with this, extant literature suggests that associations with CU traits may differ for positive v. harsh parenting (Viding et al., Reference Viding, Fontaine, Oliver and Plomin2009; Waller et al., Reference Waller, Hyde, Klump and Burt2018). Further research is thus needed to determine the impact of negative parenting practices on CU traits, particularly potential genotype × environment interactions.

What's more, extant literature has largely focused on youth CU traits, leaving open the major question of whether any of these findings extend into adulthood and to a broader and more complex manifestation of psychopathy (Neumann, Hare, & Pardini, Reference Neumann, Hare and Pardini2015). This is a key issue since genetic and environmental influences on psychopathic traits and antisocial behavior appear to change over time (Ferguson, Reference Ferguson2010; Rhee & Waldman, Reference Rhee and Waldman2002). As such, etiologic correlations between parenting and psychopathic traits may differ in adults with psychopathic traits as compared to youth with CU traits. However, no studies have examined the extent to which associations between parenting received in childhood and adult psychopathic traits are explained by genetic v. environmental influences, or whether parenting moderates heritability estimates of adult psychopathic traits.

Finally, etiologic associations between early experiences of parenting and adult psychopathic traits may also differ across the different facets of psychopathy. Psychopathy has commonly been parsed into four related, but unique, facets (interpersonal manipulation, callous affect, erratic lifestyle, and antisocial tendencies). These facets are characterized by distinct external correlates, suggesting possibly distinct etiologies (Blonigen, Hicks, Krueger, Patrick, & Iacono, Reference Blonigen, Hicks, Krueger, Patrick and Iacono2006; Carré, Hyde, Neumann, Viding, & Hariri, Reference Carré, Hyde, Neumann, Viding and Hariri2013; Hare & Neumann, Reference Hare and Neumann2005; Hoppenbrouwers, Neumann, Lewis, & Johansson, Reference Hoppenbrouwers, Neumann, Lewis and Johansson2015; Witt, Donnellan, & Blonigen, Reference Witt, Donnellan and Blonigen2009). For example, previous findings suggest that an interpersonal-affective factor (combining interpersonal manipulation and callous-affect facets) may be equally influenced by genetic and environmental factors, whereas an impulsive-antisocial factor (combining erratic lifestyle and antisocial tendencies facets) may have to be more strongly influenced by the environment than by genetics (Brook et al., Reference Brook, Panizzon, Kosson, Sullivan, Lyons, Franz and Kremen2010). Previous work has also found differential phenotypic correlations between dimensions of parenting and the different facets of psychopathy (e.g. Vachon, Lynam, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber, Reference Vachon, Lynam, Loeber and Stouthamer-Loeber2012), highlighting the need to examine these questions at the facet level.

Current study

The current study addresses these gaps in the literature by the etiology of the association between psychopathic traits in adulthood and experiences of negative parenting in childhood. We first examined etiologic associations among each of the psychopathy facets (interpersonal manipulation, callous affect, erratic lifestyle, and antisocial tendencies) and retrospective reports of negative parenting using bivariate ACE modeling. We predicted that there would be a significant overlap between non-shared environmental contributions to negative parenting and those for psychopathic traits. We then examined whether negative parenting moderated genetic and environmental influences on psychopathic traits. In doing so, we were guided by bioecological models that suggest that environmental influences can take on a larger role in harsh environments because there is more ‘push’ in these environments toward maladaptive outcomes, an important model for characterizing G × E in antisocial behavior (Burt, Reference Burt2015; Raine, Reference Raine2002). That is, psychopathic traits may be more heritable in lower risk environments (i.e. no exposure to harsh parenting), but less heritable in higher risk environments (i.e. exposure to harsh parenting), potentially because there is more environmental risk in adverse contexts. Thus, we predicted that the genetic influence on psychopathic traits would be lower and environmental components of psychopathic traits would be higher for individuals who reported receiving more negative parenting.

Methods

Participants

The present study examined a population-based sample of Finnish twins who participated in the Genetics of Sexuality and Aggression study (Johansson et al., Reference Johansson, Jern, Santtila, von der Pahlen, Eriksson, Westberg and Sandnabba2013). Participants were recruited through the Central Population Registry of Finland and were 18–49 years of age [mean of 37.5 years (s.d. = 3.06)] at the time of data collection (Johansson et al., Reference Johansson, Jern, Santtila, von der Pahlen, Eriksson, Westberg and Sandnabba2013). The current sample consisted of 1842 adults in 921 twin pairs from the first wave of data collection: 91 male MZ pairs, 247 female MZ pairs, 110 male DZ pairs, 271 female DZ pairs, and 202 opposite-sex DZ pairs. Zygosity was determined with questionnaire items completed by the twins, a method which is 95% accurate when compared with blood typing analyses (Sarna, Kaprio, Sistonen, & Koskenvuo, Reference Sarna, Kaprio, Sistonen and Koskenvuo1978). All procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the Board for Research Ethics at Åbo Akademi University and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Measures

Self-reported psychopathic traits

Psychopathic traits were assessed using the 19-item experimental version of the Self-Report Psychopathy (SRP-E), a self-report measure of psychopathy derived from the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised and other versions of the Self-Report of Psychopathy (Neumann, Schmitt, Carter, Embley, & Hare, Reference Neumann, Schmitt, Carter, Embley and Hare2012; Paulhus, Neumann, & Hare, Reference Paulhus, Neumann and Hare2009). Items are grouped into four facets: interpersonal manipulation (e.g. ‘I think I can beat a lie detector’), affective callousness (e.g. ‘I never feel guilty over hurting others’), erratic lifestyle (e.g. ‘I've often done dangerous things just for the thrill of it’), and antisocial tendencies (e.g. ‘I have broken into a building or vehicle in order to steal something or vandalize’) (Neumann et al., Reference Neumann, Schmitt, Carter, Embley and Hare2012; Paulhus et al., Reference Paulhus, Neumann and Hare2009). Though these facets show low to acceptable internal consistency (ranging from 0.48–0.77), they loaded onto four factors with acceptable model fit using confirmatory factor analysis in Mplus vs 8.3 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2019) (see online Supplementary Fig. S1).

Retrospective reports of parenting

Recalled negative parenting was assessed using a total sum score of the Measure of Parenting Style (α = 0.94), a 15-item self-report questionnaire (MOPS; Parker et al., Reference Parker, Roussos, Hadzi-Pavlovic, Mitchell, Wilhelm and Austin1997) used to assess participant perceptions of their parents' behaviors and attitudes until the participants were 16 years of age. Because twin-reported negative maternal and paternal parenting were highly correlated (r = 0.57; Table 1), we created composite perceptions of negative parenting via averages. Results were also run separately for maternal v. paternal parenting, and did not alter the conclusions (see Supplemental Materials).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and phenotypic correlations

Note. *p ⩽ 0.05. s.d. and N refer to standard deviation and sample size, respectively. Descriptives for the psychopathy facet scores are based on unstandardized values.

Statistical analyses

Classical twin studies leverage the difference in the proportion of genes shared between monozygotic or MZ twins (who share 100% of their genes) and dizygotic or DZ twins (who share an average of 50% of their segregating genes) to estimate the relative contributions of genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and non-shared environmental influences to the variance within observed behaviors or characteristics (phenotypes). More information on twin studies is provided elsewhere (Neale & Cardon, Reference Neale and Cardon1992).

We first fitted bivariate ACE models to characterize the associations between perceived parenting and the psychopathy facets. This model parses the phenotypic variance of perceived negative parenting and the phenotypic covariance between perceived parenting and a given psychopathy facet into that which is due to genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental factors. The genetic and environmental covariances were then standardized on their respective variances to produce genetic and environmental correlations. These statistics reveal the extent to which a specific effect (e.g. the genetic effect) on perceptions of negative parenting received is correlated with the same effect on each of three psychopathy facets. The bivariate model therefore enabled us to offer focused conclusions on the etiology of overlap between perceived parenting and psychopathic traits.

We also evaluated whether perceived negative parenting moderated the etiology of the psychopathy facets using the extended univariate genotype-by-environment interaction (G × E) model (van der Sluis, Posthuma, & Dolan, Reference van der Sluis, Posthuma and Dolan2012), an extension of the univariate G × E model (Purcell, Reference Purcell2002). Perceived negative parenting was first entered in a means model of each twin's psychopathy facet score. Linear moderation was then modeled on the residual psychopathy facet variance (i.e. that which does not overlap with perceived negative parenting), thereby controlling for any genotype–environment correlations. For each phenotype, we fitted a no moderation model, a full ACE moderation model, and a series of nested models in which small or non-significant moderators were constrained to 0. A variety of fit indices (detailed below) were then utilized to determine the best-fitting model. As recommended in prior work (van der Sluis et al., Reference van der Sluis, Posthuma and Dolan2012), we also fitted bivariate G × E models as an important clarification of our univariate G × E results (Purcell, Reference Purcell2002). Bivariate G × E models model the relationship between the moderator and the outcome using a Cholesky framework to distinguish between moderation of the covariance path and moderation of the residual path, thereby further clarifying the precise nature of the moderation.

Consistent with prior work (Burt, Clark, Pearson, Klump, & Neiderhiser, Reference Burt, Clark, Pearson, Klump and Neiderhiser2020; Burt, Pearson, Carroll, Klump, & Neiderhiser, Reference Burt, Pearson, Carroll, Klump and Neiderhiser2020; Burt, Wildey, & Klump, Reference Burt, Wildey and Klump2015; Klump, Perkins, Burt, McGue, & Iacono, Reference Klump, Perkins, Burt, McGue and Iacono2007), the moderator was floored at zero and divided by its highest value to have a range of 0–1 prior to analysis, an approach that facilitates the interpretation of the unstandardized model-fitting estimates. We statistically controlled for twin sex and age effects in both the bivariate and G × E biometric analyses via standard regression techniques (McGue & Bouchard, Reference McGue and Bouchard1984).

Mplus version 8.3 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2019) was used to perform the model-fitting analyses. Because of the small amount of missing data, we used Full-Information Maximum-Likelihood raw data techniques, which produce less biased and more efficient estimates than pairwise or listwise deletion in the face of missing data (Yuan & Bentler, Reference Yuan and Bentler2000). When fitting models to raw data, variances, covariances, and means are first freely estimated to get a baseline index of fit (minus twice the log-likelihood; −2lnL). The −2lnL under this unrestricted baseline model is then compared with −2lnL under more restrictive biometric models. This comparison provides a likelihood-ratio χ2 test of goodness of fit for the model, which is then converted to the Akaike's information criterion (Akaike, Reference Akaike1987; AIC = χ2–2df). The AIC measures model fit relative to parsimony; better fitting models have more negative values. Additionally, the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) and Comparative Fit Index (CFI) were utilized as additional indices of absolute and relative fit, respectively. Models with an RMSEA ⩽0.05 and CFI ⩾0.90 are judged to have a good fit (Cheung & Rensvold, Reference Cheung and Rensvold2002; Yuan & Bentler, Reference Yuan and Bentler2000). Confidence intervals were derived using non-parametric bootstrapping, which provides reliable confidence intervals for assessing parameter estimate precision under a variety of complex data conditions without concerns for violating the typical assumptions of structural equation models (Falk, Reference Falk2018). Significance was then determined via 95% confidence intervals that do not overlap with zero.

Results

Descriptive statistics and correlations

See Table 1 for descriptive statistics. Negative perceived parenting was associated with all facets of psychopathy (rs = 0.17–0.23), except for callous affective (rs = 0.04–0.05) (Table 1). Intraclass and cross-twin cross-trait correlations are presented in the online Supplementary Table 1. The MZ intraclass correlations were greater than twice the DZ intraclass correlations for all facets of psychopathy, indicating a high degree of heritability for these traits. The MZ intraclass correlation for perceived negative parenting was slightly less than twice the DZ intraclass correlation, implying the presence of clear genetic influences but also the presence of shared environmental influences. Additionally, nearly all cross-twin cross-trait correlations among the psychopathy facets were larger in MZ twins compared to DZ twins, suggesting that the co-occurrence of psychopathy facets is attributable to genetic influences. In contrast, cross-twin cross-trait correlations between each psychopathy facet and negative perceived parenting were equivalent, suggesting that environmental influences play a key role in the co-occurrence of perceived negative parenting and psychopathic traits, with minimal genetic overlap.

Univariate and bivariate analyses

The ACE model fit the data well for each of the four sets of models (Table 2). Univariate ACE estimates (Table 3) revealed that perceived negative parenting was significantly influenced by genetics (42%) and shared (16%) and non-shared environment (42%). Because our measure of parenting indexes perceptions of parenting received as a child, these genetic effects are thought to reflect a genotype–environment correlational process in which people shape the parenting that they receive in part as a function of their genetically-influenced characteristics (Klahr & Burt, Reference Klahr and Burt2014; Pezzoli, Antfolk, Hatoum, & Santtila, Reference Pezzoli, Antfolk, Hatoum and Santtila2019). The univariate ACE estimates for psychopathic traits also consistently demonstrated significant, albeit moderate, heritability (28–36%, p < 0.05). By contrast, non-shared environmental influences were prominent (42–68%, p < 0.05) and shared-environmental influences were trivial (0–7%).

Table 2. Model fit statistics

Note. MZ, DZ, AIC, BIC, SBIC, χ2(df), RMSEA, and CFI refer to monozygotic, dizygotic, Akaike's information criterion, Bayesian information criterion, sample-size adjusted Bayesian information criterion, χ2 (degrees of freedom), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation, and Comparative Fit Index, respectively.

Table 3. ACE estimates and correlations

Note. Standardized ACE estimates are reported. Asterisks indicate that the estimate is significant at p ⩽ 0.05. rA, rC, and rE refer to additive genetic correlation, shared environment correlation, and non-shared environment correlation, respectively.

Genetic and environmental correlations between parenting and the three psychopathy facets are also reported in Table 3. As we cannot decompose non-significant phenotypic associations into their genetic and environmental components, we omitted callous affective from the bivariate analyses. Non-shared environmental correlations between psychopathic traits and perceived negative parenting were all significant, ranging from 0.29 to 0.34, indicating a moderate overlap in the non-shared environmental influences on psychopathic traits and perceived negative parenting. Conversely, additive genetic correlations between psychopathic traits and perceived negative parenting ranged from −0.17 to 0.004 and were not significant, suggesting that the genetic effects influencing these phenotypes are largely unique to each (i.e. associations between perceived negative parenting and psychopathic traits are not substantially genetic). Finally, shared environmental correlations between perceived negative parenting and psychopathic traits were estimated at 1.00 and were non-significant or marginally significant. Note that high, but non-significant, shared environmental correlations are relatively common when there is very little shared environmental variance within a trait (Klahr, Thomas, Hopwood, Klump, & Burt, Reference Klahr, Thomas, Hopwood, Klump and Burt2013). Though these results theoretically indicate that the shared environmental influences on perceived negative parenting overlap entirely with those on psychopathic traits, they have little practical meaning because there were minimal shared environmental influences on psychopathic traits.

Online Supplementary Fig. S1 illustrates these findings via a Cholesky decomposition framework, presenting standardized genetic and environmental path estimates for the variation within psychopathic traits as well as between these traits and perceived negative parenting. As suggested by the genetic and non-shared environmental correlations presented above, the genetic covariance paths across psychopathic traits were not significant, while the non-shared environmental covariance paths were all significant. This analysis further supports the conclusion that non-shared environmental influences on psychopathic traits overlap to some degree with those on perceived negative parenting (with negligible genetic etiology).

G × e interaction analyses

Next, we examined whether twins' perceptions of the negative parenting they received moderated the etiology of each of the four psychopathy facets (Tables 2 and 4; Fig. 1). Nested models fit best, and the etiology of all psychopathy facets varied as a function of perceived negative parenting. Across all psychopathy facets, absolute environmental contributions were more pronounced when participants reported greater levels of negative parenting. In particular, there were significantly greater shared-environmental contributions to all psychopathy facets and greater non-shared environmental contributions to antisocial tendencies when twins reported that they experienced particularly negative parenting in their childhoods. By contrast, absolute genetic contributions for all psychopathy facets were consistently equivalent across different levels of perceived negative parenting.

Fig. 1. Etiologic moderation of the psychopathy facets by perceived negative parenting. Note. A, C, and E represent genetic, shared, and non-shared environmental influences, respectively. *Significant slope. These estimates index the absolute (unstandardized) changes in genetic and environmental variance in psychopathy by perceived negative parenting in a linear model. The specific path estimates are presented in Table 5. Of note, both significant and non-significant moderators are included in this figure to demonstrate the trend of all moderators. The slope for C is significant for all four models, whereas the slope for A is not significant in any, and the slope for E is only significant in the Antisocial Tendencies model (significance is indicated by asterisks to the right of the slope as well as in the ledger).

Table 4. Unstandardized path and moderator estimates for extended univariate genotype-by-environment models

Note. A, C, and E (upper and lower case) respectively represent genetic, shared, and non-shared environmental parameters on psychopathy facets. Asterisks indicate that the estimate is significant at p ⩽ 0.05. Linear moderators (i.e. A1, C1, E1) were added to the paths using the following equation: Unstandardized VarianceTotal = (a + A1(PNP))2 + (c + C1(PNP))2 + (e + E1(PNP))2. The variance component estimates calculated this way are presented in the text. PNP, perceived negative parenting.

To clarify these results, we also fitted bivariate G × E models (Table 5; Purcell, Reference Purcell2002), as recommended by van der Sluis et al. (Reference van der Sluis, Posthuma and Dolan2012). For antisocial tendencies, the unique C and E moderators were estimated at 0.77 and 0.55 (p < 0.05), respectively, and the common C and E moderators were estimated at −0.61 and −0.23 (both ns), respectively. For interpersonal manipulation, the unique C moderator was estimated at 1.06 (p < 0.05), and the common C moderator was estimated at 0.04 (ns). For erratic lifestyle, the unique C moderator was estimated at 0.00, and the common C moderator was estimated at −1.24 (p < 0.05). These results indicate that the moderators for antisocial tendencies and interpersonal manipulation predominantly load onto the residual paths, a pattern of results that is interpreted as consistent with ‘true’ etiologic moderation. By contrast, the results for erratic lifestyle were less consistent with actual etiologic moderation of erratic lifestyle by parental negativity.

Table 5. Unstandardized path and moderator estimates for bivariate genotype-by-environment models

Note. A, C, and E (upper and lower case) respectively represent genetic, shared, and non-shared environmental parameters on psychopathy facets. Asterisks indicate that the estimate is significant at p ⩽ 0.05. Linear moderators (i.e. A1, C1, E1) were added to the paths using the following equation: Unstandardized VarianceTotal = (a + A1(PNP))2 + (c + C1(PNP))2 + (e + E1(PNP))2. PNP, perceived negative parenting.

As a final set of robustness tests of our analyses, we sought to confirm that our interpretation did not vary when perceived negative maternal and paternal parenting were examined separately (see online Supplementary Tables 2 and 3), and to confirm that the etiology of the psychopathy facets did not vary with participant age (they did not; see online Supplementary Table 4). The pattern of environmental moderation reported above is fully replicated when examining parenting separately for mothers and fathers. Namely, pronounced and positively-signed shared environmental moderation by negative parenting was observed across all forms of psychopathy for both mothers and fathers. Similarly, we observed nearly identical levels of non-shared environmental moderation by negative parenting only for antisocial tendencies. That said, the patterns of genetic moderation differed across mothers and fathers relative to the composite, decreasing for two of the four forms of psychopathy when examining mothering and increasing for two of the four forms of psychopathy when examining fathering.

Discussion

In a large population-based sample of adult twins, we found that psychopathic traits were moderately heritable with substantial non-shared environmental influences. We also observed significant associations between three of four facets of psychopathy in adulthood (interpersonal manipulation, erratic lifestyle, and antisocial tendencies) and twin perceptions of negative parenting received during their childhood. These associations were attributable to an overlap in non-shared environmental effects, whereas the genetic overlap was negligible. Moreover, G × E interaction analyses revealed that environmental influences on the interpersonal, lifestyle, and antisocial features of psychopathy were stronger for individuals who reported experiencing higher levels of perceived negative parenting during childhood. Thus, our findings highlight the role of both genetic and environmental factors in the etiology of psychopathic traits, while also highlighting the prominent role of parenting as an environmental influence on certain features of adult psychopathy (i.e. interpersonal, lifestyle, and antisocial).

The most basic contribution of this study was examining the relative contribution of genetic and environmental influences on psychopathy by extending this investigation to the facet level. Consistent with the existing literature on adult psychopathy (e.g. Benning, Patrick, Blonigen, Hicks, & Iacono, Reference Benning, Patrick, Blonigen, Hicks and Iacono2005; Hunt, Bornovalova, & Patrick, Reference Hunt, Bornovalova and Patrick2015; Larsson, Andershed, & Lichtenstein, Reference Larsson, Andershed and Lichtenstein2006; Taylor, Loney, Bobadilla, Iacono, & McGue, Reference Taylor, Loney, Bobadilla, Iacono and McGue2003; Waldman et al., Reference Waldman, Rhee, LoParo, Park and Patrick2018), both genetic and non-shared environmental influences were significant. However, the current study found relatively genetic contributions (e.g. A = 28–36%) more consistent with lower bound estimates from prior studies of adult psychopathic traits (e.g. As = 24–67%) (Blonigen, Carlson, Krueger, & Patrick, Reference Blonigen, Carlson, Krueger and Patrick2003; Brook et al., Reference Brook, Panizzon, Kosson, Sullivan, Lyons, Franz and Kremen2010; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Loney, Bobadilla, Iacono and McGue2003; Tuvblad et al., Reference Tuvblad, Wang, Patrick, Berntsen, Raine and Baker2019). It is unclear what may account for these differences across prior studies. These heritability estimates were also relatively similar across facets, suggesting that the etiology of each facet, at least as measured via twin analyses parsing broad heritable and environmental influences, is not divergent.

We also found that retrospective reports of negative parenting were correlated with higher levels of the interpersonal manipulation, erratic lifestyle, and antisocial tendencies largely for environmental reasons. This finding emphasizes that associations between recalled parenting and current features of psychopathy (i.e. interpersonal, lifestyle, and antisocial) are not due to gene–environment correlation, but are instead largely environmental in origin. Collectively, these studies are in contrast to lay theories of psychopathy as a ‘bad seed’ that is ‘born’ and not ‘made’ (Skeem et al., Reference Skeem, Polaschek, Patrick and Lilienfeld2011). Instead, these results suggest that certain features of psychopathy (i.e. interpersonal, lifestyle, and antisocial), like aggression and antisocial behavior more broadly, have substantial, though moderate genetic influences, but with substantial non-shared environmental influences that include experiences such as negative parenting.

Somewhat surprisingly, however, perceived negative parenting was not associated with callous affect. This result conflicts with a larger body of previous work in youth that has linked negative parenting to CU traits, which most closely align with the callousness facet in adulthood (Waller et al., Reference Waller, Gardner and Hyde2013). In the current study, the strongest associations between negative parenting and psychopathic facets were with the lifestyle and antisocial tendencies facets, which overlap the most with broader forms of antisocial behavior. Consistent with some work in youth (Waller et al., Reference Waller, Gardner and Hyde2013, Reference Waller, Hyde, Klump and Burt2018), it may be that negative parenting is specifically important to antisocial behavior and antisocial behavioral components of psychopathy, whereas low positive parenting, such as parental warmth (which we did not measure here), may be key to understanding the interpersonal and affective features of psychopathy (Pasalich, Witkiewitz, McMahon, Pinderhughes, & Group, Reference Pasalich, Witkiewitz, McMahon, Pinderhughes and Group2016; Waller & Hyde, Reference Waller and Hyde2017, Reference Waller and Hyde2018). On the other hand, the current study may instead be indexing the well-established environmental overlap between antisocial behavior and parenting, at least to the extent to which the facets of psychopathy measured here index antisocial behavior. Notably, though, negative parenting was also associated with the interpersonal facet, which has little overlap with antisocial behavior. At the least, it is important to note that these findings in adults do not necessarily translate directly to studies of youth, since studies of youth have focused mostly on CU traits and the closest facet measures here (callous affect) did not correlate with parenting.

Finally, similar to a recent study in youth focusing on CU traits and positive parenting (Henry et al., Reference Henry, Dionne, Viding, Vitaro, Brendgen, Tremblay and Boivin2018), we found that perceived negative parenting moderated the etiology of three facets of psychopathy (i.e. interpersonal, lifestyle, antisocial). Specifically, for individuals reporting greater exposure to negative parenting, environmental influences were stronger. Based on the bivariate G × E results, negative parenting moderated the etiology of two facets specifically, interpersonal manipulation and antisocial tendencies, whereas there was weaker support for environmental moderation of the erratic lifestyle facet (i.e. moderator only loaded onto the shared pathway and is therefore less indicative of true etiologic moderation). Overall, this finding is consistent with the bioecological model of G × E (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, Reference Bronfenbrenner and Ceci1994; Pennington et al., Reference Pennington, McGrath, Rosenberg, Barnard, Smith, Willcutt and Olson2009), which posits that environmental influences take on a larger role in harsh environments because there is more ‘social push’ toward maladaptive outcomes in these environments (Raine, Reference Raine2002). In this case, one interpretation is that parents who engaged in harsh social interactions with their children modeled and/or taught interpersonal approaches that include interpersonal manipulation, impulsive actions, and antisocial attitudes. Alternately, the children may have developed these attributes as a way of navigating the stress of harsh parenting. However, these results are also consistent with the differential susceptibility theory, which posits that some individuals are more sensitive to environment (both positive and negative) than others (Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009; Yan, Benner, Tucker-Drob, & Harden, Reference Yan, Benner, Tucker-Drob and Harden2017). In line with this hypothesis, we found greater genetic variance at the extreme end of environmental experience (i.e. high levels of perceived negative parenting) and minimal genetic variance for average environments. Further research will be important to better understand which theoretical framework most accurately reflects etiological mechanisms of psychopathic traits.

Notably, in a set of exploratory analyses examining perceived negative parenting by mothers v. fathers separately, the pattern of results largely replicated those reported for the parenting composite, pointing to potent environmental moderation of all four psychopathy facets regardless of whether parenting was assessed in regard to mothers or to fathers. That said, a few differences were observed. We specifically found that genetic influences on two of the four facets decreased as negative maternal parenting increased, the same as the directionality of associations when using the parent composite. However, we found that genetic influences on two of the four facets increased as perceived negative paternal parenting increased. It is unclear why these differed from each other or from the overall parenting composite. However, the inconsistency of the findings across operationalizations of negative parenting raises some doubt about their generalizability, and thus they should be interpreted with caution.

Strengths and limitations

Using a large sample of twins, we parsed genetic and environmental influences on adult psychopathic traits (including interpersonal, affective, antisocial, and lifestyle features) and address significant gaps in the literature by conducting a bivariate analysis to examine the genetic and environmental overlap between perceived negative parenting in childhood and psychopathic traits in adulthood, as well as to examine negative parenting perceptions as a moderator of heritability estimates of psychopathic traits. These approaches have not been used in the study of adult psychopathy to date, and thus constitute an important contribution to the literature. Even so, the study had several important limitations. Adult participants retrospectively reported their experiences of parenting, which likely impacted the accuracy of their responses and could have been shaped by their current levels of psychopathy and/or to a third variable that influences both psychopathy and their response style (i.e. personality traits are linked to psychopathy and to how the participant reports on the parenting they received). This is potentially problematic since previous research has found that recall bias is correlated with the degree of psychopathology (Brewin, Andrews, & Gotlib, Reference Brewin, Andrews and Gotlib1993). It is thus possible that the use of retrospective reports inflated environmental estimates, or genetic influences, in some way. Notably, however, the G × E models used here explicitly evaluated moderation only of that psychopathy variance that did not overlap with parenting. Because shared informant variance should act to artefactually increase correlations between parenting and psychopathy, we suspect that our G × E analyses may have largely circumvented the issue of shared informant effects. What's more, we note that retrospective reports of the family environment are at least moderately correlated with prospective reports (Bell & Bell, Reference Bell and Bell2018), suggesting that they have some utility for the current study. Indeed, the heritability estimates of parenting in the current study were similar to those found in a meta-analysis (Klahr & Burt, Reference Klahr and Burt2014).

Building on this point, although the validity of self-report measures of psychopathy has been historically questioned given that core features of the construct (e.g. deceitfulness, manipulation), more recent research has not been able to find associations between psychopathic traits and response style (Ray et al., Reference Ray, Hall, Rivera-Hudson, Poythress, Lilienfeld and Morano2013). In short, it is not clear what effect our use of retrospective self-reports of parenting may have had on our results. Future work should seek to replicate these findings using prospectively collected, multi-informant data for both parenting and psychopathy.

Next, as we only measured negative parenting, we were not able to examine the potentially different associations that may have emerged with a measure of positive parenting, and cannot directly compare our results to the previous literature on positive parenting (Henry et al., Reference Henry, Dionne, Viding, Vitaro, Brendgen, Tremblay and Boivin2018; Hyde et al., Reference Hyde, Waller, Trentacosta, Shaw, Neiderhiser, Ganiban and Leve2016). Finally, as is necessary to recruit large-scale twin samples, our sample was representative of the broader community and not enriched for those in forensic settings. Thus, our results may not be generalizable to clinical or offender samples with potentially more severe levels of psychopathic traits.

Conclusion

In conclusion, we identified a common non-shared environmental pathway between perceived negative parenting and three facets of adult psychopathy (interpersonal, lifestyle, and antisocial, but not callous affect). Further, we found that perceived negative parenting moderated environmental influences on interpersonal, lifestyle, and antisocial features of psychopathy by increasing their importance to psychopathy when perceptions of negative parenting were high. Additionally, we expanded on previous work by demonstrating that the four facets of psychopathy were all moderately heritable with significant non-shared environmental influences. Overall, our results suggest that both genetics and experience are important for the development of psychopathy, and that parenting may be a specific malleable risk factor for the interpersonal, lifestyle, and antisocial features of adult psychopathy that can be targeted in preventative interventions. Moreover, this study, combined with other genetically informed studies, supports the causal environmental role of parenting in the etiology of some facets within the psychopathy construct.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721002269.

Financial support

Funding was provided by a NARSAD Young Investigator Grant from the Brain and Behavior Foundation (to LWH) and the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01HD093334 (to S.A.B. and L.W.H.). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. H.L.D. and A.Y.V. were each supported by a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation. A.J. was supported by grant 308856 from the Academy of Finland.

Conflict of interest

None.

Footnotes

*

Indicates shared first authorship

References

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaver, K. M., da Silva Costa, C., Poersch, A. P., Freddi, M. C., Stelmach, M. C., Connolly, E. J., & Schwartz, J. A. (2014). Psychopathic personality traits and their influence on parenting quality: Results from a nationally representative sample of Americans. Psychiatric Quarterly, 85(4), 497511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, D. C., & Bell, L. G. (2018). Accuracy of retrospective reports of family environment. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27(4), 10291040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 885908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., & Iacono, W. G. (2005). Estimating facets of psychopathy from normal personality traits a step toward community epidemiological investigations. Assessment, 12(1), 318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blonigen, D. M., Carlson, S. R., Krueger, R. F., & Patrick, C. J. (2003). A twin study of self-reported psychopathic personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(1), 179197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., Krueger, R. F., Patrick, C. J., & Iacono, W. G. (2006). Continuity and change in psychopathic traits as measured via normal-range personality: A longitudinal-biometric study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(1), 8595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Gotlib, I. H. (1993). Psychopathology and early experience: A reappraisal of retrospective reports. Psychological Bulletin, 113(1), 8298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101, 568586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brook, M., Panizzon, M. S., Kosson, D. S., Sullivan, E. A., Lyons, M. J., Franz, C. E., … Kremen, W. S. (2010). Psychopathic personality traits in middle-aged male twins: A behavior genetic investigation. Journal of Personality Disorders, 24(4), 473486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, S. A. (2015). Evidence that the gene–environment interactions underlying youth conduct problems vary across development. Child Development Perspectives, 9(4), 217221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burt, S. A., Clark, D. A., Pearson, A. L., Klump, K. L., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2020). Do neighborhood social processes moderate the etiology of youth conduct problems? Psychological Medicine, 50(9), 15191529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, S. A., Pearson, A. L., Carroll, S., Klump, K. L., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2020). Child antisocial behavior is more environmental in origin in disadvantaged neighborhoods: Evidence across residents’ perceptions and geographic scales in two samples. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 48(2), 265276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, S. A., Wildey, M. N., & Klump, K. L. (2015). The quality of the inter-parental relationship does not moderate the etiology of child conduct problems. Psychological Medicine, 45(2), 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carré, J. M., Hyde, L. W., Neumann, C. S., Viding, E., & Hariri, A. R. (2013). The neural signatures of distinct psychopathic traits. Social Neuroscience, 8(2), 122135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(2), 233255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, J., Kopkin, M. R., Rankin, J. A., Tomeny, T. S., & Coffey, C. A. (2018). The relationship between parental psychopathic traits and parenting style. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27(7), 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenbarth, H., Krammer, S., Edwards, B. G., Kiehl, K. A., & Neumann, C. S. (2018). Structural analysis of the PCL-R and relationship to BIG FIVE personality traits and parenting characteristics in an Hispanic female offender sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 129, 5965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falk, C. F. (2018). Are robust standard errors the best approach for interval estimation with nonnormal data in structural equation modeling? Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 25(2), 244266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farrington, D. P. (2000). Psychosocial predictors of adult antisocial personality and adult convictions. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 18(5), 605622.3.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, C. J. (2010). Genetic contributions to antisocial personality and behavior: A meta-analytic review from an evolutionary perspective. The Journal of Social Psychology, 150(2), 160180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, R. D., & Neumann, C. S. (2005). Structural models of psychopathy. Current Psychiatry Reports, 7(1), 5764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, R. D., & Neumann, C. S. (2008). Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 217246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawes, D. J., Dadds, M. R., Frost, A. D., & Hasking, P. A. (2011). Do childhood callous-unemotional traits drive change in parenting practices? Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 40(4), 507518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, J., Dionne, G., Viding, E., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Tremblay, R. E., & Boivin, M. (2018). Early warm-rewarding parenting moderates the genetic contributions to callous–unemotional traits in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(12), 12821288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoppenbrouwers, S. S., Neumann, C. S., Lewis, J., & Johansson, P. (2015). A latent variable analysis of the Psychopathy Checklist–Revised and behavioral inhibition system/behavioral activation system factors in North American and Swedish offenders. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 6(3), 251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, E., Bornovalova, M., & Patrick, C. (2015). Genetic and environmental overlap between borderline personality disorder traits and psychopathy: Evidence for promotive effects of factor 2 and protective effects of factor 1. Psychological Medicine, 45(7), 14711481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyde, L. W., Waller, R., Trentacosta, C. J., Shaw, D. S., Neiderhiser, J. M., Ganiban, J. M., … Leve, L. D. (2016). Heritable and nonheritable pathways to early callous-unemotional behaviors. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(9), 903910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johansson, A., Jern, P., Santtila, P., von der Pahlen, B., Eriksson, E., Westberg, L., … Sandnabba, N. K. (2013). The genetics of sexuality and aggression (GSA) twin samples in Finland. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16(1), 150156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klahr, A. M., & Burt, S. A. (2014). Elucidating the etiology of individual differences in parenting: A meta-analysis of behavioral genetic research. Psychological Bulletin, 140(2), 544586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klahr, A. M., Thomas, K. M., Hopwood, C. J., Klump, K. L., & Burt, S. A. (2013). Evocative gene–environment correlation in the mother–child relationship: A twin study of interpersonal processes. Development and Psychopathology, 25(1), 105118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klump, K. L., Perkins, P. S., Burt, S. A., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2007). Puberty moderates genetic influences on disordered eating. Psychological Medicine, 37(5), 627634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, H., Andershed, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2006). A genetic factor explains most of the variation in the psychopathic personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(2), 221230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loeber, R., Burke, J. D., & Pardini, D. A. (2009). Development and etiology of disruptive and delinquent behavior. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 291310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loeber, R., & Hay, D. (1997). Key issues in the development of aggression and violence from childhood to early adulthood. Annual Review of Psychology, 48(1), 371410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynam, D. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2007). Longitudinal evidence that psychopathy scores in early adolescence predict adult psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 155165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGue, M., & Bouchard, T. J. J. (1984). Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behavior Genetics, 14, 325343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moffitt, T. E. (2018). Male antisocial behaviour in adolescence and beyond. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(3), 177816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2019). Mplus 8.3. In Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Neale, M. C., & Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumann, C. S., Hare, R. D., & Pardini, D. A. (2015). Antisociality and the construct of psychopathy: Data from across the globe. Journal of Personality, 83(6), 678692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumann, C. S., Schmitt, D. S., Carter, R., Embley, I., & Hare, R. D. (2012). Psychopathic traits in females and males across the globe. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 30(5), 557574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olver, M. E., Neumann, C. S., Sewall, L. A., Lewis, K., Hare, R. D., & Wong, S. C. (2018). A comprehensive examination of the psychometric properties of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised in a Canadian multisite sample of indigenous and non-indigenous offenders. Psychological Assessment, 30(6), 779792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, G., Roussos, J., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., Mitchell, P., Wilhelm, K., & Austin, M.-P. (1997). The development of a refined measure of dysfunctional parenting and assessment of its relevance in patients with affective disorders. Psychological Medicine, 27(5), 11931203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pasalich, D. S., Witkiewitz, K., McMahon, R. J., Pinderhughes, E. E., & Group, C. P. P. R. (2016). Indirect effects of the fast track intervention on conduct disorder symptoms and callous-unemotional traits: Distinct pathways involving discipline and warmth. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(3), 587597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulhus, D. L., Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (2009). Manual for the self-report psychopathy scale. Toronto: Multi-health systems.Google Scholar
Pennington, B. F., McGrath, L. M., Rosenberg, J., Barnard, H., Smith, S. D., Willcutt, E. G., … Olson, R. K. (2009). Gene X environment interactions in reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Psychology, 45, 7789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pezzoli, P., Antfolk, J., Hatoum, A. S., & Santtila, P. (2019). Genetic vulnerability to experiencing child maltreatment. Frontiers in Genetics, 10, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piquero, A. R., Farrington, D. P., Fontaine, N. M. G., Vincent, G., Coid, J., & Ullrich, S. (2012). Childhood risk, offending trajectories, and psychopathy at age 48 years in the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 18(4), 577598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purcell, S. (2002). Variance components models for gene–environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 5(6), 554571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A. (2002). Biosocial studies of antisocial and violent behavior in children and adults: A review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30(4), 311326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ray, J. V., Hall, J., Rivera-Hudson, N., Poythress, N. G., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Morano, M. (2013). The relation between self-reported psychopathic traits and distorted response styles: A meta-analytic review. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 4(1), 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reidy, D. E., Kearns, M. C., DeGue, S., Lilienfeld, S. O., Massetti, G., & Kiehl, K. A. (2015). Why psychopathy matters: Implications for public health and violence prevention. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 24, 214225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128(3), 490529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salekin, R. T. (2017). Research review: What do we know about psychopathic traits in children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(11), 11801200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarna, S., Kaprio, J., Sistonen, P., & Koskenvuo, M. (1978). Diagnosis of twin zygosity by mailed questionnaire. Human Heredity, 28(4), 241254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skeem, J. L., Polaschek, D. L. L., Patrick, C. J., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). Psychopathic personality: Bridging the gap between scientific evidence and public policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 12(3), 95162. doi: 10.1177/1529100611426706CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, J., Loney, B. R., Bobadilla, L., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on psychopathy trait dimensions in a community sample of male twins. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(6), 633645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tuvblad, C., & Baker, L. A. (2011). Human aggression across the lifespan: Genetic propensities and environmental moderators. Advances in genetics, 75, 171214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuvblad, C., Fanti, K. A., Andershed, H., Colins, O. F., & Larsson, H. (2017). Psychopathic personality traits in 5 year old twins: The importance of genetic and shared environmental influences. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 26(4), 469479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuvblad, C., Wang, P., Patrick, C. J., Berntsen, L., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2019). Genetic and environmental influences on disinhibition, boldness, and meanness as assessed by the triarchic psychopathy measure in 19–20-year-old twins. Psychological Medicine, 49(9), 15001509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vachon, D. D., Lynam, D. R., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2012). Generalizing the nomological network of psychopathy across populations differing on race and conviction status. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(1), 263269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Sluis, S., Posthuma, D., & Dolan, C. V. (2012). A note on false positives and power in G×E modelling of twin data. Behavior Genetics, 42(1), 170186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viding, E., Fontaine, N. M., Oliver, B. R., & Plomin, R. (2009). Negative parental discipline, conduct problems and callous–unemotional traits: Monozygotic twin differences study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 195(5), 414419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viding, E., & McCrory, E. (2018). Understanding the development of psychopathy: Progress and challenges. Psychological Medicine, 48(4), 566577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viding, E., & McCrory, E. J. (2012). Genetic and neurocognitive contributions to the development of psychopathy. Development and Psychopathology, 24(3), 969983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waldman, I. D., Rhee, S. H., LoParo, D., & Park, Y. (2018). Genetic and environmental influences on psychopathy and antisocial behavior. In Patrick, C. J. (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 335353). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Waller, R., Gardner, F., & Hyde, L. W. (2013). What are the associations between parenting, callous–unemotional traits, and antisocial behavior in youth? A systematic review of evidence. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(4), 593608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, R., & Hyde, L. W. (2017). Callous–unemotional behaviors in early childhood: Measurement, meaning, and the influence of parenting. Child Development Perspectives, 11(2), 120126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, R., & Hyde, L. W. (2018). Callous-unemotional behaviors in early childhood: The development of empathy and prosociality gone awry. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 1116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, R., Hyde, L. W., Klump, K. L., & Burt, S. A. (2018). Parenting is an environmental predictor of callous-unemotional traits and aggression: A monozygotic twin differences study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 57(12), 955963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witt, E. A., Donnellan, M. B., & Blonigen, D. M. (2009). Using existing self-report inventories to measure the psychopathic personality traits of fearless dominance and impulsive antisociality. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(6), 10061016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yan, N., Benner, A., Tucker-Drob, E., & Harden, K. P. (2017). Mothers’ early depressive symptoms and preschoolers’ behavioral problems: The moderating role of genetic influences. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 48(3), 434443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yuan, K.-H., & Bentler, P. M. (2000). Three likelihood-based methods for mean and covariance structure analysis with nonnormal missing data. Sociological Methodology, 30(1), 165200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and phenotypic correlations

Figure 1

Table 2. Model fit statistics

Figure 2

Table 3. ACE estimates and correlations

Figure 3

Fig. 1. Etiologic moderation of the psychopathy facets by perceived negative parenting. Note. A, C, and E represent genetic, shared, and non-shared environmental influences, respectively. *Significant slope. These estimates index the absolute (unstandardized) changes in genetic and environmental variance in psychopathy by perceived negative parenting in a linear model. The specific path estimates are presented in Table 5. Of note, both significant and non-significant moderators are included in this figure to demonstrate the trend of all moderators. The slope for C is significant for all four models, whereas the slope for A is not significant in any, and the slope for E is only significant in the Antisocial Tendencies model (significance is indicated by asterisks to the right of the slope as well as in the ledger).

Figure 4

Table 4. Unstandardized path and moderator estimates for extended univariate genotype-by-environment models

Figure 5

Table 5. Unstandardized path and moderator estimates for bivariate genotype-by-environment models

Supplementary material: File

Dotterer et al. supplementary material

Dotterer et al. supplementary material

Download Dotterer et al. supplementary material(File)
File 368.9 KB