Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4hhp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-16T05:05:23.744Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Claude Lévi-Strauss as a humanist forerunner of cultural macroevolution studies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 July 2022

Marcelo R. Sánchez-Villagra*
Affiliation:
Paleontological Institute and Museum, University of Zurich, Karl Schmid Strasse 4, 8006 Zurich, Switzerland
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: m.sanchez@pim.uzh.ch

Abstract

Cross-cultural studies of humans using methods developed in evolutionary biology and comparative linguistics are flourishing. ‘Cultural macroevolution’ has great potential to address fundamental questions of cultural transformation and human history. However, this field is poorly integrated with core cultural anthropology, although both aim in part at addressing similar issues. Claude Lévi-Strauss established a comparative approach searching for universals and documentation of diversity to bring understanding to cultural phenomena. Recognizing the nomothetic nature of Lévi-Strauss’ work, his abstraction and modelling, provides an example within anthropology of the search for universals and the study of big data, akin to cultural macroevolution studies. The latter could benefit, beyond the sophisticated analyses of big data mined from ethnographic work, from the integration with the intellectual legacy and practice of core anthropology and thus propitiate the synergistic interaction of disciplines. Attempts at rapprochement of disciplines from the natural sciences that lack pluralism and present a narrow view are deemed examples of ‘Wilson's effect’.

Type
Perspective
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: Lévi-Strauss’ anthropological work in its abstraction and modelling provides an example of the search for universals and big data analyses

Introduction

The relatively new field of cultural evolution aims at understanding different aspects of human cumulative culture (Cavalli-Sforza & Feldman, Reference Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman1981; Mesoudi, Reference Mesoudi2011; Antweiler, Reference Antweiler2012; Lewens, Reference Lewens2015; Micheletti et al., Reference Micheletti, Brandl and Mace2022). The majority of studies of cultural evolution concern the processes that lead to cultural change within and among populations. Here methodological tools and concepts from psychology, cognitive biology and behavioural ecology are applied to decipher mechanisms and patterns at the level of individuals and populations, in many cases with experimental approaches. This is analogous to microevolutionary studies in biology. The approach may involve mathematical modelling, examining the gene–culture dynamics for short time spans (Mace, Reference Mace2009; Creanza et al., Reference Creanza, Kolodny and Feldman2017). Other studies for example concern the ‘transmission biases’ or modes of influence when information is exchanged between human groups (Kirby et al., Reference Kirby, Cornish and Smith2008).

A less explored but significant area of cultural evolution is cross-cultural and phylogenetic studies (Youngblood & Lahti, Reference Youngblood and Lahti2018), concerning the longer-term fate of cultural phenomena or broader categorizations of cultural practices and materials over longer periods of time (Perreault, Reference Perreault2012; Gray & Watts, Reference Gray and Watts2017; Lambert et al., Reference Lambert, Kontonatsios, Mauch, Kokkoris, Jockers, Ananiadou and Leroi2020; Leroi et al., Reference Leroi, Lambert, Mauch, Papadopoulou, Ananiadou, Lindberg and Lindenfors2020; Lukas et al., Reference Lukas, Towner and Borgerhoff Mulder2021). The aim is to document the patterns and generate hypotheses explaining the processes behind the rich cultural human diversity as reflected in languages, belief systems and myths, modes of subsistence, music, kinship systems and myriad material cultural artefacts. As such, cultural macroevolution (Mesoudi, Reference Mesoudi2011) can address fundamental aspects of deep and more recent history (Smail, Reference Smail2007).

In this essay I discuss how questions and approaches of cultural macroevolution studies find parallels in the work of one of the most prominent anthropologists of the twentieth century, Claude Lévi-Strauss (CLS). Recognizing these parallels raises some issues concerning anthropology and academic traditions. I refer to diverse works of CLS, including Tristes tropiques (1955) deemed by Susan Sontag in 1963 ‘one of the great books of our century’. Both A view from afar’ and Myth and meaning offer succinct but rich summaries in English of the main tenets of Lévi-Strauss’ prolific career.

The search for nomothetic explanations

The study of human affairs is traditionally approached from diverse disciplines coming from both the humanities (Geisteswissenchaften) and the natural sciences (Naturwissenschaften). The following quote about anthropology is generally attributed to Alfred L. Kroeber, one of the many prominent students of Franz Boas: ‘Anthropology is the most humanistic of the sciences and the most scientific of the humanities’. Perhaps another dichotomy is more useful here. In 1894 the German philosopher Wilhelm Windelband introduced the notion of two different perspectives. Historical processes can be approached with ideographic and nomothetic explanations. Ideographic explanations are about specific events and their causes, while nomothetic ones aim at providing general principles or laws. Nomothetic explanations are ubiquitous in the natural sciences, but they also occur in the humanities. The main difference between the humanities and natural sciences may reside in the approach – whereas humanities are discursive, argue with words, natural scientists use measurement, data and quantitative analyses (Leroi, Reference Leroi2022). Social sciences such as economics are a third kind with elements of both (Kagan, Reference Kagan2007).

In his cross-cultural studies of kinship, art, forms of classification and myths, CLS compared cultural diversity and searched for nomothetic explanations, aiming at establishing ‘the intellectual unity of humankind’ (Doja, Reference Doja2008: 325). He thus tried to document and account for cultural diversity while identifying commonalities and the principles that govern them. Furthermore, he introduced approaches that aimed at the study of culture becoming more mathematical and subject to abstraction and categorization.

Structuralism and cultural change

Lévi-Strauss developed for anthropology the approach of structuralism, in which within a certain realm, parts of the structure are connected in a network in which each part makes sense and responds to all others within that system. If one wishes to understand that part by itself, one is lost, but when considering its many interactions, one can. Thus, each structure is unique and has its own network, definitions and references. Yet the principles or mechanisms that operate in structures, in the relations of parts, are universal. This universality derives from the way the ‘mind’ (neurobiology) operates, the result of biology, which follows the laws of chemistry and physics. CLS was not afraid of reductionism when leading to productive understanding.

CLS expressed his admiration for natural sciences in their mathematical and predictive power, and was instrumental in attempts to make anthropology more like them. CLS’ structuralism was inspired by linguistics, a complex and rich subject (Maniglier, Reference Maniglier2002). Lévi-Strauss (e.g. Reference Lévi-Strauss1969, Reference Lévi-Strauss1978, Reference Lévi-Strauss1985) repeatedly stated that anthropology has to become like linguistics, a more quantitative and analytical science. CLS’ structuralism was influenced by Saussure's and Jakobson's works – in them, the different parts of a language build a structure in their reciprocal relationships. From the structural phonology of Jakobson he took the concept of phonemes as inspiration to an analogous concept in kinship terms (Lévi-Strauss, Reference Lévi-Strauss, Jacobson and Schoepf1958).

In Britain a school of ‘neo-structuralists’ developed within cultural anthropology which generally followed CLS in the comparative approach but diverged in its main goals (Kuper, Reference Kuper1996). The ultimate concern of CLS was to find universal principles in human culture; the functionalist British anthropologists, in contrast, were mostly trying to decipher the organization of particular societies or group of societies (Leach, Reference Leach1970; Hugh-Jones, Reference Hugh-Jones2008) – an idiographic approach.

It has been claimed that structuralism is not transformative, it cannot explain change, as it offers no method to reconstruct the origin of a system. This is a valid critical remark – in particular to ‘British structuralism’ (Hugh-Jones, Reference Hugh-Jones2008). Structuralism lacks a formal, numerical method of historical reconstruction, something several kinds of phylogenetic analyses in biology provide. Yet the studies of CLS were comparative, and one of their main goals was to decipher common patterns of different systems – as in the canonical formula of myths – and with that reconstruct the ancestral one. CLS aimed at historical reconstructions and structuralism was a tool that he used pragmatically. With this approach he generated a hypothesis of historical transformation and continuity, as exemplified by the study of exchange systems in relation to kinship (Lévi-Strauss, Reference Lévi-Strauss1969; Rosman & Rubel, Reference Rosman, Rubel and Wiseman2009). As in structural linguistics examining the relationships of phonemes and not their composition (Maniglier, Reference Maniglier2002), Lévi-Strauss’ (Reference Lévi-Strauss1969) study of kinship stressed the relationships between groups. His work developed the subjects of marriage rules and kinship terminology, now addressed with cultural macroevolution approaches (Passmore & Jordan, Reference Passmore and Jordan2020).

The structural approach led to many different applications (e.g. Burnham, Reference Burnham1973), but CLS was critical of or distanced himself from many of those associated with ‘postmodernity’ (Lévi-Strauss, Reference Lévi-Strauss and Weightman1981: 641), such as Roland Barthes (Loyer, Reference Loyer2018). On the other hand, the general comparative approach and the general principle of the structural approach of CLS also inspired analytical works of relevance to cultural evolution studies. An example is that of how indigenous knowledge is lost with the decrease of interactions among groups owing to extinction: the impoverishment of networks of indigenous culture (Cámara-Leret et al., Reference Cámara-Leret, Fortuna and Bascompte2019).

The study of myths

In his approach of structural analyses of myths, CLS identified their parts and elements in order to discover the form of relations among them and look for universal patterns of such relations. CLS used abstraction by breaking down myths into minimal units of narrative structure he called mythemes, analogous to phonemes of structural linguistics in that they exist in relation to parts of a system. Variation around mythemes concerns their becoming negated, inverted or recoded (Schwimmer, Reference Schwimmer and Wiseman2009). New myths are formed by putting together pieces of stories that are recycled, reused: the bricolage, leading to an almost unlimited number of combinations (Doniger, Reference Doniger and Wiseman2009). CLS performed large-scale studies of myths, making comparisons of them from quite distant geographic regions, as from different continents. He was after ‘universal cognitive biases’ (van Schaik, Reference van Schaik2019: 87), such as those predicting responses in supernatural beliefs to large-scale problems such as social inequity, floods and droughts (van Schaik & Michel, Reference van Schaik and Michel2016).

CLS also postulated a ‘canonical formula’ that expressed mathematically the relation of parts in myths that presumably exist universally across cultures. This formula has since then has been repeatedly examined and explored in its significance as an attempt at abstraction (Mosko, Reference Mosko1991; Petitot, Reference Petitot and Pierre2016) and even at prediction (Darányi et al., Reference Darányi, Wittek and Kitto2013). Fundamentally, the canonical formula is just an instrument of expressing mathematically some simple relations of correspondence in which elements of myths can be analyzed. The challenge is to identify what those parts are and to assume relations that may be spurious – here is where the limitation of the approach lies (Turner, Reference Turner2009).

As described by Philippe Descola (Reference Descola1996), CLS’ general approach to the study of myths is analogous to what the Achuar, an Amazonian Jivaroan group living along either side of the border between Ecuador and Peru, do with images that appear in their dreams: they reduce them to ‘minimal logical units in order to derive practical information from them’ (p. 118). The Achuar use dream elements not at face value, but to extract the logical operations that they reveal.

The peculiar associations of myths from different regions postulated by Lévi-Strauss and the lack of a transparent and reproduceable method beyond the principles of structuralism led to substantial critiques of CLS’ approach (Turner, Reference Turner2009). Analytical methods have been used to study myths worldwide with a level of generality that avoids some of the issues raised by the work of CLS (Thuillard et al., Reference Thuillard, Le Quellec, d'Huy and Berezkin2018). The study of folktales, also following a long previous tradition of scholarship and hypotheses to test, has been productively developed using cultural macroevolutionary approaches, as in the study of oral traditions and plots associated with the story of Little Red Riding Hood (Tehrani, Reference Tehrani2013).

An example of the power of the study of myths in cultural evolution is that concerning the history of dogs in the Americas as to their origin and past diffusion as revealed by biological data (Segura et al., Reference Segura, Geiger, Monson, Flores and Sánchez-Villagra2022). The application of the neighbour-joining tree method based on Jaccard distances on a database of 23 myths concerning dogs and 22 geographic areas showed a correlation between history and geography (d'Huy, 2022). The approach hypothesized two waves of settlement in America and made it possible to reconstruct ancestral mythologies around dogs.

Lévi-Strauss aimed at understanding the ecological environment of a society and identifying the traits of the natural habitat that influence symbolic thought. CLS devoted ‘meticulous attention to the flora, fauna and astronomical and climatic cycles particular to the places from which the myths that he studies originate’ (Descola, Reference Descola and Wiseman2009: 105). He used this information to understand sources of variation in details of myths among societies. This clarity of ideas is in contrast to the controversies about the effect of ecology on cultural phenomena, a central issue in South American anthropology (Raffles, Reference Raffles2002). An example of ecological determinism is the work of Betty Meggers (Reference Meggers1971), who stated that ‘the level to which a culture can develop … is dependent upon the agricultural potentiality of the environment it occupies’ (Meggers, Reference Meggers1954: 815). She postulated that the cultural consequences of the environment were predictable. These ideas were tied to diffusionist ones. Any evidence of a ‘complex’ culture in the Amazonian forest could only be the result of transmission from other, richer, productive areas. Recent work has brought a deeper understanding of the importance of human agency and the complexities of the landscape and environment in which this agency operates (Hecht, Reference Hecht2013; Rostain & Jaimes Betancourt, Reference Rostain and Jaimes Betancourt2017). This discussion should inform and enrichen important attempts within cultural macroevolution to address if ‘cultural history’ or ‘ecological environment’ determines human behaviour (Matthew & Perreault, Reference Matthew and Perreault2016). These studies usually make use of large amounts of data and databases.

Big data and ethnography

Cultural macroevolution studies use big data and sophisticated quantitative analyses (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Greenhill, Watts, List, Botero, Gray and Kirby2021). There are several global comparative cultural and linguistic databases, such as D-PLACE (https://d-place.org/), Glottobank (https://glottobank.org/) and Seshat (Turchin et al., Reference Turchin, Brennan, Currie, Feeney, Francois, Hoyer and Whitehouse2015), among others. The list includes the ‘human relations area files’ (https://hraf.yale.edu/), which CLS brought to France with his special filing cabinets in the early days, long before they became digital (Figure 1). CLS became the supporter and host of the main European Center for comparative, cross-cultural ethnographic documentation (Bucher, Reference Bucher2010; Loyer, Reference Loyer2018). CLS’ work is highly relevant to understanding, within classical anthropology, research agendas that also rely on cultural diversity using big data, while building on first-hand knowledge and collaboration with ethnographers.

Figure 1. Claude Lévi-Strauss and the HRAF in Paris. Lévi-Strauss posed fundamental questions on cultural diversity and origins, and advocated extensive examination of data to address them. Keystone-France/Gamma-Keystone via Getty Images, with permission.

Gray and Watts (2017: 7846) enthusiastically advocated for cultural macroevolution studies, with the ‘application of the type of sophisticated computational methods that are often used in the biological sciences such as network analysis of reticulate evolution, epidemiological models, and phylogenetic comparative methods. These methods can be used to compare the relative importance of different factors in the distribution of traits, model the underlying dynamics of evolutionary change, and infer the history of traits.’ In fact, computational methods can be used to examine different variables in the distribution of cultural traits and model the underlying dynamics of cultural change (e.g. Ranacher et al., Reference Ranacher, Neureiter, Gijn, Sonnenhauser, Escher, Weibel and Bickel2021). Graduate students of cultural evolution and related fields such as comparative linguistics are being trained to have an impressive set of quantitative skills. However, the strong focus on large analyses and methods raises issues (Grigoropoulou & Small, Reference Grigoropoulou and Small2022).

In macroevolutionary studies it is as important as or more important than the methodological know-how, to gain an understanding of primary data and the fieldwork or laboratory work that produces them (Slingerland et al., Reference Slingerland, Atkinson, Ember, Sheehan, Muthukrishna, Bulbulia and Gray2020). A scientist cannot conduct a statistical analysis while ignoring the underlying bias of the data or how they were collected – only with that it is possible to spot if there are obvious mistakes, impossible patterns or subtle batch effects. The abstraction involved in cultural macroevolution studies presents a challenge in the integration with anthropology (Whitehouse, Reference Whitehouse, Slingerland and Collard2012). The categorization of cultural phenomena in traits has been identified as an important issue (Fuentes, Reference Fuentes2006; Slingerland et al., Reference Slingerland, Atkinson, Ember, Sheehan, Muthukrishna, Bulbulia and Gray2020) that can be best confronted with first-hand knowledge of the primary data and their context. This approach could include a critical consideration of ethnographic data. These data could complement those just obtained from databases for cultural macroevolution studies, or as an alternative source to experimental approaches involving human response in specific situations and studies focused on specific variables (e.g. Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Boyd, Bowles, Camerer, Fehr, Gintis and Tracer2005). The latter has been argued by Tehrani (Reference Tehrani2006: 364), stating how ‘rather than focusing on isolated subsets of cultural complexes, ethnographers attempt to situate behaviours within wider contexts of cultural meaning, event histories, and social relationships’. Another insight from long-term ethnographic studies is that they can ‘help to establish which traditions are likely to be strongly affected by social change and rates of inter-group contact, and which ones are more stable and long-lasting’ (Tehrani, Reference Tehrani2006: 364). The dismissal of ethnographic data by most cultural macroevolution studies is at the core of the critique of Ingold (Reference Ingold2007) of evolutionary approaches in the study of culture.

In cultural anthropology it was a rite of passage, a common entrance into the field, to spend considerable time doing fieldwork. CLS encouraged, supported and in many cases supervised many such works (Descola, Reference Descola1996; Bucher, Reference Bucher2010). In linguistics there was an equivalent role for fieldwork work while deciphering the language of a human group, for example reconstructing the grammar of an unknown language with the help of informants. CLS spent most of his professional life conducting research that looked for nomothetic explanations of cultural phenomena, but his ethnographic work in Brazil, as described in ‘Tristes Tropiques’, provided first-hand experience of the data and their collection. In the opinion of experts, the ethnographic work of CLS itself was limited, but not in what it led to nor in the future ethnographic work it inspired (Wilcken, Reference Wilcken2010).

On ontologies

Whereas the Western European philosophers and historians had not transcended a particular and situated conceptual universe, the structural anthropologist approach proposed by CLS provided a much broader view on the range and nature of human experience, with ethnographic data being paramount. He was a leading intellectual figure in questioning ethnocentrism and the objectivity and universality of Western history, relativizing the ‘notion of progress and (…) the achievements of Western science and technology’ (Doja, Reference Doja2006: 21). CLS’ nomothetic work was ethnological, not based on just WEIRD people – Western, educated, industrialized, rich and democratic. As such, the important work by Henrich and colleagues (e.g. Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010) pointing out the biases of many evolutionary psychology works in the investigated people and the peculiarities of one cultural group is not without a long historical predecessor addressing analogous issues, among others, in CLS.

CLS was arguably the most prominent anthropologist in the second half of the twentieth century, as was Franz Boas in the first half (Zumwalt, Reference Zumwalt2019), in bringing the idea to academics and the general public in Europe and North America that ‘other cultures are not failed attempts at being you; they are unique manifestations of the human spirit’, as expressed by Wade Davis, a popularizer of ethnographic and ethnological work (www.ted.com). CLS was a great defender on academic and practical grounds of cultural diversity and of indigenous cultures. These issues mentioned above are as relevant for cultural evolution studies as they are to classical anthropology of course – it is the same people and cultures that are a stake.

CLS’ structuralism attempts to understand a society in its own terms, notwithstanding the conceptual baggage that a Western scientist brings to research – thus the importance of the work on ontologies and of perspectives (Wagner, Reference Wagner2012). Situating oneself in another human perspective is challenging. Perhaps the comparison with accomplishing this across animal species is appropriate. In a highly influential paper, the philosopher Tomas Nagel (Reference Nagel1974) argued that even when knowing all the details of a bat's sensory biology, it is impossible for us humans to ‘see’ the world as a bat, not being one oneself.

The subject of ontologies greatly developed in anthropology following CLS’ work, especially in scholarship on Amazonia. This includes the revision of animism (Descola, Reference Descola and Lloyd2013) and perspectivism (e.g. Viveiros de Castro, Reference Viveiros de Castro2014), with variations on the conceptualization of the world, humans and other living beings revolving around the dualism body–soul, anthropomorphism and intentionality across living beings (Ingold, Reference Ingold2000; Fausto, Reference Fausto2020). It remains to be seen how cultural macroevolution studies can incorporate these fundamental explorations of human cultural diversity in its workings.

CLS argued for universals in the way humans develop systems of thoughts and symbols. At least this helps to put all cultures on equal ground – each cultural system, with its logic derived from universal principles of the human ‘mind’ (meaning cognition, as determined by neurobiology and physiology) and celebrating and documenting the individual cases in their own right – notwithstanding the true and multivariate issues involved in any ethnographic work.

The consideration of different ontologies is important to understand cultural macroevolutionary phenomena that a purely Western science approach misses, as in the consideration of domestication by Jared Diamond (Reference Diamond1997). Diamond argued for the potential of species to become domesticated based on biological features and the geographical distribution of such species. As important as these are, studies have shown that cultural perspectives are more likely to explain the lack or low number of domestic species in some regions of the world (Descola, Reference Descola and Latour1994). Some South American canids fulfill the criteria for becoming domesticated but they have not been domesticated (Segura & Sánchez-Villagra, Reference Segura and Sánchez-Villagra2021). Likewise, multidisciplinary teams have explained how a dog-centred perspective can provide an insightful view to understand human cultural transformation and health in history (Sykes et al., Reference Sykes, Beirne, Horowitz, Jones, Kalof, Karlsson and Larson2020).

CLS argued that a rational approach of fundamentally the same kind was involved in the scientific developments of Western societies after the Renaissance and is involved in the operations of Indigenous people, as it was also involved in the Neolithic transition when domestication of plants and animals was developed, surely non-trivial cultural processes of rational thinking and planning, as were also those involving in the invention of ceramics (Lévi-Strauss, Reference Lévi-Strauss, Mehlman and Leavitt2020).

Dual inheritance theory

Dual inheritance theory is an important and foundational aspect of ‘cultural evolution’. It postulates that genetic and cultural evolution are intertwined, they interact (Boyd & Richerson, Reference Boyd and Richerson1980). The transmission of cultural traits is via social learning, and the mechanisms of genetic transmission are those figured out by evolutionary biologists. According the dual inheritance theory, cultural traits can be biased by genetic imperatives (Chekalin et al., 2018), and the same applies to genetic evolution as influenced by cultural traits (Mace, Reference Mace2009).

Lévi-Strauss (e.g. Reference Lévi-Strauss1985) recognized that the relation between genetic and cultural evolution is reciprocal. He stated how culturally mediated environments result in selective pressures that drive genetic evolution. He stressed the impact of culture on biological evolution (p. 14): ‘the cultural forms adopted in various places by human beings, their ways of life in the past or in the present, determine to a very great extent the rhythm of their biological evolution and its direction’. As discussed by Loyer, (Reference Loyer2018: 525), CLS ‘thus brought the biological and the cultural together but reversed the dynamic of determination of the old physical anthropology: it was not race that dictated culture, but cultural factors that sometimes influenced the course of natural selection’. Likewise, Lévi-Strauss (Reference Lévi-Strauss1985) recognized in his controversial essay on ‘race and culture’ of his book A view from afar how the evolutionary history of some populations has led to biological traits more suitable to some kinds of environments than to others.

Lost in translation and failed consilience

CLS’ concern with shared, universal workings of humans was based on empirical work, as in many of today's cognitive psychologists and neurobiologists. It is ironic that his writings ‘helped to make possible modernist ideas of deconstruction, reflexivity, and the transient nature of culture and identity’ (Doja, Reference Doja2006: 18). Many, unaware of CLS’ work and ramifications, have wrongly aligned CLS with critical theory. This is a brand of anthropology that cultural evolutionists have seen as detrimental to the field or to any consilience of naturals and social sciences (van Schaik, Reference van Schaik2019). In general, leaders of the cultural evolution field take a dismal view of social anthropology (e.g. Mesoudi et al., 2006, see Tehrani, Reference Tehrani2006 response).

CLS has had many critics within anthropology, not surprisingly given his long and prolific career. Rice (Reference Rice2017: 163) in his leading text on current ethnomusicology lumped CLS with ‘theorists’ and ‘postmoderns’ such as Adorno, Durkheim, ‘and more recently’ with Foucault. In Cooking, cuisine and class, Jack Goody (Reference Goody1982: 23) extensively criticized aspects of CLS work he identified with ‘Hegelian metaphysics’ (which CLS 1985 explicitly contradicted in A view from afar). The latter is an example of how some Anglo-Saxon anthropologists have related the Frenchman CLS to a brand of continental philosophy they deem detrimental to rational understanding, in this case wrongly. Different intellectual traditions may indeed be behind the well-documented differences between CLS and the British structuralists (Hugh-Jones, Reference Hugh-Jones2008). It is CLS’ insights into the different systems of thought across societies, with different inner logics, and his questioning of Western thought hegemony that paved the way for the postmodernism movement, with which he did not identify (Lévi-Strauss Reference Lévi-Strauss and Weightman1981: 641). Too simply put, CLS’ work was not ‘postmodern’, if one wishes to use such a crude and abused term.

The search for the ancestral myths (never truly managed, as discussed by Turner Reference Turner2009) and other intellectual pursuits in CLS’ studies of indigenous people could be interpreted both as indicative of a naive ethnographic analogy (Currie, Reference Currie2016) and as taking ancestral as ‘primitive’ in a value system of progress. Both notions are wrong. The misreading of some of the translations of his work lack the ironic twist of the use of ‘primitive’ and ‘savage’ in some of his writings (Loyer, Reference Loyer2018). A telling example of an attempt to correct this: the recent translation of La pensée sauvage (2020) by Jeffrey Mehlman and John Leavitt is entitled Wild thought and not The savage mind, as in the Reference Lévi-Strauss1966 version.

Historical and societal sensitivity in cultural evolutionary studies – on Darwin

Many prominent practitioners in the field of cultural evolution have not been afraid to express explicit association of the field with ‘Darwinian evolution’. The subtitle of Mesoudi's (Reference Mesoudi2011) important ‘cultural evolution’ textbook is How Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. Laland's (Reference Laland2017) book is titled Darwin's unfinished symphony – How culture made the human mind. In fact, these two authors, as many others, have made significant contributions to evolutionary theory in ideas and matters that go beyond Darwin and Wallace's natural selection. Laland, for example, has led efforts to propose that current evolutionary theory is significantly different from that of the Neodarwinian synthesis of the first half of the twentieth century (Laland et al., Reference Laland, Uller, Feldman, Sterelny, Müller, Moczek and Strassmann2014). Charles Darwin postulated evolution by natural selection without any idea of later discovered inheritance mechanisms. The Neodarwinian synthesis (and much of mainstream evolutionary biology today) has come to focus almost exclusively on genetic inheritance and processes that change gene frequencies (Mayr & Provine, Reference Mayr and Provine1980). The new evolutionary biology identifies numerous processes by which organisms grow and develop and influence evolution (Diogo, Reference Diogo2017). To call cultural evolution ‘Darwinian’ is a misnomer. Cultural macroevolution is informed by a pluralistic conceptual and methodological field, of which Darwin and Wallace were fundamental early contributors – no less, no more.

The theoretical advances in cultural evolution studies concern matters that are informed by diverse ideas and fields that came after Darwin. These include among others dual inheritance theory discussed above, as well as information on macroevolution informed by studies of extinction (Zhang & Mace, Reference Zhang and Mace2021), phylogenetic systematics and the comparative method (Nunn, Reference Nunn2011), and matters of biases of the record of the past (Perreault, Reference Perreault2019).

To call cultural evolution ‘Darwinian’ is surely detrimental to making it an appealing field for anthropologists. The history of the use of Darwinism in the social sciences is mostly nefarious (Diogo, Reference Diogo2022), and that alone justifies using another term. Discussing Charles Darwin, the man himself (Fuentes, Reference Fuentes2021) and views in his times (Braun et al., Reference Braun, Christensen and Hans2017), can be controversial, but this is not the matter at hand here.

Darwin looms so large that he reshaped the history of biology and made us forget other intellectual prior giants such as Goethe (search for universals, comparisons) and Cuvier (extinction) that built ultimately on Aristotle (Leroi, Reference Leroi2014). That Darwin is so idolized responds surely to his insights, his ‘genius’ (Wilkins, Reference Wilkins2009) and influence, but surely also to the cultural evolutionary process of the development of the tale of the science in which Anglo-Saxon science became dominant. To deny the brilliant and diverse insights of Darwin would be absurd, but one can easily dismiss his significantly wrong ideas on inheritance (Darwin, Reference Darwin1868), for example. To limit the historical references of cultural evolution to Darwin and ignore a much richer historical background is a missed chance.

For cultural evolution to contribute to an intellectual breakthrough, this field needs to synthesize and bring consilience (Shore, Reference Shore, Slingerland and Collard2012); it should also become more sophisticated and sensitive to the issues of the past and the present. There are important historical figures within anthropology that conducted comparative work and historical research, even borrowing concepts from evolutionary biology, as in Margaret Mead's discussion on micro- and macroevolution in biology and in culture (Schwartz & Mead, Reference Schwartz and Mead1961). Here I have argued that CLS is one such central figure in anthropology.

This essay does not aim at making of Lévi-Strauss a hero to replace Darwin. CLS argued that the glorification of individual creativity is an illusion, in different contexts, including his own persona and in his consideration of cultural transformation in general (Levi-Strauss, Reference Levi-Strauss1955). This included the study of Amerindian art, devoid of the individualistic self-display of Western art (Fausto, Reference Fausto2020), so prevalent in Western thought.

Quo Vadis ‘cultural macroevolution’? Avoiding ‘Wilson's effect’

‘Cultural evolution’ is developing as its own field, with its own society, conference and journals. This indicates a maturity and critical mass, but it comes at a cost of a lack of integration with anthropology, when in fact it is concerned with issues that have been addressed by this discipline. The practice of cultural macroevolutionary studies could benefit from more than a century of scholarship by incorporating some aspects of the humanistic tradition of anthropology. This may not translate one-to-one in any specific method of analysis being incorporated nor in any new big-data source, but it could bring a sense of depth and scholarship and with that a more critical, nuanced and integrative practice in the discipline. It would help to bring about the consilience of natural and social sciences that has eluded previous attempts. It would avoid what I would call ‘the Wilson effect’, after the author of Sociobiology (Wilson, Reference Wilson1975) and Consilience (Wilson, Reference Wilson1998), namely what happens when there is an attempt for rapprochement coming from the natural sciences with ideas that are actually narrow in the lack of pluralism and devoid of historical nuance and consideration, leading actually to distancing among disciplines (Shweder, Reference Shweder, Slingerland and Collard2012).

Many cultural evolutionists complain of ‘postmodernity’ in anthropology – a crude and simplistic umbrella term that is actually referring to different currents and approaches in the social sciences and humanities. The integration of disciplines will require a long-run and collective effort. Integrating the cultural evolution meeting as a part or as a preceding conference to a cultural anthropology one would be a way to facilitate bringing people together.

Gray and Watts (2017: 7846) stated that ‘The combination of big(ish) data and computational methods has the potential to transform the social sciences and humanities by enabling powerful quantitative tests of hypotheses that would have previously only been analyzable in much more limited ways’. Although I concur with the enthusiasm on the importance of this approach, I think it is quite clear that ‘transformation’ (perhaps expansion is a better term) will not occur unless there is an engagement with matters and concepts developed in the social sciences and the humanities. The rich intellectual history of anthropology provides links and clues to develop such communication, and CLS is one if not the most appropriate central figure to which to refer to in this endeavour.

One may argue that the CLS abstractions and big data approach did not use evolutionary approaches. CLS did not practise ‘cultural macroevolution’ as it is understood today (Mesoudi, Reference Mesoudi2011), in the same way that Aristotle was not a scientist and Goethe was not Darwinian – it is anachronistic to use those terms in such contexts.

I much doubt that there can be a simple, straightforward integration in cultural macroevolution studies of the structuralist approach to study myths from CLS, including the identification of mythemes as units of comparison, with the standard cultural macroevolution methods. Yet CLS brought a contextual understanding of myths and a consideration of different ontologies that, although relevant, the typical cultural macroevolution approach lacks.

The main point of this essay is to emphasize the comparative and historical approaches of CLS and of many other cultural anthropologists, as ‘they force the investigators to define terms, use consistent categories, and in general discipline their data’ (Bridgeman, Reference Bridgeman2006: 351). There is a compelling case to be made that ‘It is not evolutionary models, but models in general that social science needs’ (Bridgeman, Reference Bridgeman2006). As stated by Bridgeman (Reference Bridgeman2006: 351), ‘the value of the models may stem not so much from their link to evolutionary theory as from the way that they force the investigators to define terms, use consistent categories, and in general discipline their data’. It would seem that the more we learn about biology, the more multi-modelled it is; and the more we learn about culture, the more we realize that we need creative new ways to understand the data beyond standard biological models (Matsumae et al., Reference Matsumae, Ranacher, Savage, Blasi, Currie, Koganebuchi and Bickel2021).

If a method is just a tool and not an end in itself, it follows that cultural macroevolution studies are just anthropology with new tools. Ignoring the rich historical background of studies of culture would make ‘cultural evolution’ parochial. Incorporating efforts into anthropology on the other hand would help circumvent the false association of cultural evolution with social Darwinism.

Acknowledgements

I thank Tomás Bartoletti for stimulating discussions of ideas and reading tips (in particular the brilliant paper by Turner Reference Turner2009), Chiara Barbieri, Gabriel Aguirre-Fernández, Cristiana Bertazoni and Carel van Schaik for insightful comments, Judith Recht for editorial help, Eduardo Jorge de Oliveira for the impetus to write this essay, and Evelyne Heyer and Ruth Mace for editorial work. Two anonymous reviewers provided thoughtful and constructive reviews that were much appreciated and present challenging ideas for future work.

Author contributions

MRSV conceived and designed the study and wrote the article.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflicts of interest

Marcelo R. Sánchez-Villagra declares none.

Research transparency and reproducibility

Data availability statement: there are no data accompanying the manuscript.

References

Antweiler, C. (2012). On cultural evolution: A review of current research toward a unified theory of societal change. Anthropos, 107, 217227. https://doi.org/10.5771/0257-9774-2012-1-217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1980). Sociobiology, culture and economic theory. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 1(2), 97121. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-2681(80)90005-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braun, S., Christensen, G., & Hans, B. (2017). American Indians: Historical and contemporary perspectives. Kendall Hunt.Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B. (2006). It is not evolutionary models, but models in general that social science needs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(4), 351352. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X06279081CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bucher, B. (2010). Claude Gustave Lévi-Strauss (1908–2009). American Anthropologist, 112(4), 693697. http://www.jstor.org/stable/40931293CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnham, J. (1973). The structure of art. George Braziller.Google Scholar
Cámara-Leret, R., Fortuna, M. A., & Bascompte, J. (2019). Indigenous knowledge networks in the face of global change. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(20), 99139918. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1821843116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Chekalin, E., Rubanovich, A., Tatarinova, T. V., Kasianov, A., Bender, N., Chekalina, M., … Morozova, I. (2019, January 1). Changes in biological pathways during 6,000 years of civilization in Europe. Mol Biol Evol, 36(1), 127140. https://doi.org/10.1093/molbev/msy201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creanza, N., Kolodny, O., & Feldman, M. W. (2017). Cultural evolutionary theory: How culture evolves and why it matters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 77827789. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620732114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Currie, A. (2016). Ethnographic analogy, the comparative method, and archaeological special pleading. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A, 55, 8494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darányi, S., Wittek, P., & Kitto, K. (2013). The Sphynx's new riddle: How to relate the canonical formula of myth to quantum interaction. International Symposium on Quantum Interaction. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-54943-4_5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. R. (1868). The variation of animals and plants under domestication. John Murray.Google Scholar
Descola, P. (1994). Pourquoi les Indiens d'Amazonie n'ont-ils pas domestiqué le pécari? Généalogie des objets et anthropologie de l'objectivation. In Latour, B (Ed.), De la préhistoire aux missiles balistiques. L'intelligence sociale des techniques. Paris: La Découverte.Google Scholar
Descola, P. (1996). The spears of twilight: Life and death in the Amazon jungle. HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Descola, P. (2009). The two natures of Lévi-Strauss. In Wiseman, B. (Ed.), The Cambridge companion to Lévi-Strauss. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CCOL9780521846301.006Google Scholar
Descola, P. (2013). Beyond nature and culture (Trans. Lloyd, J.). University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
d‘Huy, J. (2022). De l'origine du chien et de sa diffusion à l'aune de sa mythologie. Anthropozoologica, 57(7), 169182.Google Scholar
Diamond, J. M. (1997). Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies. W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Diogo, R. (2017). Etho-eco-morphological mismatches, an overlooked phenomenon in ecology, evolution and evo-devo that supports ONCE (organic nonoptimal constrained evolution) and the key evolutionary role of organismal behavior. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 5(3). https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2017.00003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diogo, R. (2022). Meaning of life, human nature, and delusions: How tales about love, sex, races, gods and progress affect our lives and Earth's splendor. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-70401-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doja, A. (2006). The predicament of heroic anthropology. Anthropology Today, 22(3), 1822.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doja, A. (2008). Claude Lévi-Strauss at his centennial: Toward a future anthropology. Theory, Culture & Society, 25(7–8), 321340. https://doi.org/10.1177/0263276408097810CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doniger, W. (2009). Claude Lévi-Strauss's theoretical and actual approaches to myth. In: Wiseman, B. (Ed.). The Cambridge companion to Lévi-Strauss. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, C., Greenhill, S., Watts, J., List, J.-M., Botero, C., Gray, R., & Kirby, K. (2021). The uses and abuses of tree thinking in cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0056CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fausto, C. (2020). Art effects: Image, agency, and ritual in Amazonia. University of Nebraska Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuentes, A. (2006). Evolution is important but it is not simple: Defining cultural traits and incorporating complex evolutionary theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(4), 355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuentes, A. (2021). ‘The Descent of Man’, 150 years on. Science, 372(6544), 769. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abj4606CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goody, J. (1982). Cooking, cuisine and class: A study in comparative sociology. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511607745CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, R. D., & Watts, J. (2017). Cultural macroevolution matters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 78467852. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620746114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigoropoulou, N., & Small, M. L. (2022). The data revolution in social science needs qualitative research. Nature Human Behaviour. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01333-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hecht, S. B. (2013). The scramble for the Amazon and the ‘lost paradise’ of Euclides da Cunha. University of Chicago Press. https://books.google.com/books?id=jSzHAdDKPMYCCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., … Tracer, D. (2005). ‘Economic man’ in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(6), 795815. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavior and Brain Science, 33(2–3), 6183. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525×0999152xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hugh-Jones, S. (2008). A courtship but not much of a marriage. Lévi-Strauss and British Americanist anthropology. Journal de la Société des américanistes, 94(2), 1727. http://www.jstor.org/stable/24606394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingold, T. (2000). The perception of the environment: Essays on livelihood, dwelling and skill. Routledge.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2007). The trouble with ‘evolutionary biology’. Anthropology Today, 23(2), 1317. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4620344CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J. (2007). The three cultures: Natural sciences, social sciences, and the humanities in the 21st century. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kirby, S., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(31), 1068110686. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0707835105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuper, A. (1996). Anthropology and anthropologists: The modern British school (3rd ed.). Routledge.Google Scholar
Laland, K., Uller, T., Feldman, M., Sterelny, K., Müller, G. B., Moczek, A., … Strassmann, J. E. (2014). Does evolutionary theory need a rethink? Nature, 514(7521), 161164. https://doi.org/10.1038/514161aCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N. (2017). Darwin's unfinished symphony – How Culture Made the Human Mind. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambert, B., Kontonatsios, G., Mauch, M., Kokkoris, T., Jockers, M., Ananiadou, S., & Leroi, A. M. (2020). The pace of modern culture. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(4), 352360. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0802-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leach, E. (1970). Claude Lévi-Strauss. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Leroi, A. M. (2014). The lagoon: How Aristotle invented science. Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Leroi, A. M. (2022). The nature of art. Inference, 6(4). https://inference-review.com/article/the-nature-of-artGoogle Scholar
Leroi, A. M., Lambert, B., Mauch, M., Papadopoulou, M., Ananiadou, S., Lindberg, S. I., & Lindenfors, P. (2020). On revolutions. Palgrave Communications, 6(1), 4. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-019-0371-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levi-Strauss, C. (1955). Tristes tropiques. Librairie Plon.Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1966). The savage mind. The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1969). The elementary structures of kinship. Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1978). Myth and meaning. University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1985). The view from afar. The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1958). Structural anthropology (Trans. Jacobson, C. & Schoepf, B. G.). Basic books, Inc.Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (2020) Wild thought (Trans. Mehlman, J. and Leavitt, J.). The University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1981). Mythologiques 4. The naked man (Trans. J. & Weightman, D.). The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lewens, T. (2015). Cultural evolution: Conceptual challenges. Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loyer, E. (2018). Lévi-Strauss: A biography. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Lukas, D., Towner, M. and Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (2021). The potential to infer the historical pattern of cultural macroevolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Socity B 376, 20200057. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0057CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mace, R. (2009). Update to Holden and Mace's ‘Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of lactose digestion in adults’ (1997): Revisiting the coevolution of human cultural and biological diversity. Human Biology, 81(5–6), 621624. https://doi.org/10.3378/027.081.0610CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maniglier, P. (2002). Le vocabulaire de Lévi-Strauss. Ellipses.Google Scholar
Matsumae, H., Ranacher, P., Savage, P. E., Blasi, D. E., Currie, T. E., Koganebuchi, K., … Bickel, B. (2021). Exploring correlations in genetic and cultural variation across language families in northeast Asia. Science Advances, 7 (34), eabd9223. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abd9223CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthew, S., & Perreault, C. (2016). Cultural history, not ecological environment, is the main determinant of human behavior. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 283, https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2016.0177Google Scholar
Mayr, E., & Provine, W. B. (1980). The evolutionary synthesis (Vol. 231). Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meggers, B. J. (1954). Environmental limitation on the development of culture 1. American Anthropologist, 56(5), 801824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meggers, B. J. (1971). Amazonia: Man and culture in a counterfeit paradise. Atherton.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural evolution: How Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/doi:10.7208/9780226520452CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Micheletti, A. J. C., Brandl, E., & Mace, R. (2022). What is cultural evolution anyway? Behavioral Ecology. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arac011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mosko, M. S. (1991). The canonic formula of myth and nonmyth. American Ethnologist, 18(1), 126151. https://doi.org/10.1525/ae.1991.18.1.02a00060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? The Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435450. https://doi.org/10.2307/2183914CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunn, C. L. (2011). The comparative approach in evolutionary anthropology and biology. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Passmore, S. & Jordan, F. (2020). No universals in the cultural evolution of kinship terminology. Evolutionary Human Sciences. 2: e42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perreault, C. (2012). The pace of cultural evolution. PLoS ONE, 7(9), e45150. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045150CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perreault, C. (2019). The quality of the archaeological record. University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226631011.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petitot, J. (2016). A morphodynamical schematization of the canonical formula for myths. In Pierre, M. (Ed.), The double twist: From ethnography to morphodynamics. University of Toronto Press. https://doi.org/doi:10.3138/9781442681125-013Google Scholar
Raffles, H. (2002). In Amazonia: A natural history. Princeton University Press. https://books.google.com/books?id=5tKePcaagpwCGoogle Scholar
Ranacher, P., Neureiter, N., Gijn, R., Sonnenhauser, B., Escher, A., Weibel, R., … Bickel, B. (2021). Contact-tracing in cultural evolution: A Bayesian mixture model to detect geographic areas of language contact. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 18, 20201031. http://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2020.1031CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, T. (2017). Modeling ethnomusicology. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190616885.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosman, A., & Rubel, P. G. (2009). Structure and exchange. In: Wiseman, B. (Ed.). The Cambridge companion to Lévi-Strauss. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rostain, S., & Jaimes Betancourt, C. (Eds.) (2017). Las siete maravillas de la Amazonía precolombina. Bonner Amerikanistische Studien, No. 53. Bonn.Google Scholar
Schwartz, T., & Mead, M. (1961). Micro-and macro-cultural models for cultural evolution. Anthropological Linguistics, 17.Google Scholar
Schwimmer, E. (2009). The local and the universal. In Wiseman, B. (Ed.), The Cambridge companion to Lévi-Strauss. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Segura, V., Geiger, M., Monson, T. A., Flores, D., & Sánchez-Villagra, M. R. (2022). Biological and cultural history of domesticated dogs in the Americas. Anthropozoologica, 57(1): 118. doi: 10.5252/anthropozoologica2022v57a1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segura, V., & Sánchez-Villagra, M. R. (2021). Human–canid relationship in the Americas: An examination of canid biological attributes and domestication. Mammalian Biology, 101, 387406. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42991-021-00129-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shore, B. (2012). Unconscielience: Rethinking the two-cultures conundrum in anthropology. In Slingerland, E. & Collard, M. (Eds.), Creating consilience: Integrating the sciences and the humanities. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. (2012). The metaphysical realities of the unphysical sciences: Or why vertical integration seems unrealistic to ontological pluralists. In Slingerland, E. & Collard, M. (Eds.), Creating consilience: Integrating the sciences and the humanities. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199794393.003.0003Google Scholar
Slingerland, E., Atkinson, Q. D., Ember, C. R., Sheehan, O., Muthukrishna, M., Bulbulia, J., & Gray, R. D. (2020). Coding culture: Challenges and recommendations for comparative cultural databases. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 2, article e29. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2020.30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smail, D. L. (2007). On deep history and the brain. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sontag, S. (1963). A hero of our time. Review of ‘Structural Anthropology’ by Claude Lévi-Strauss. The New York Review of Books, November 28.Google Scholar
Sykes, N., Beirne, P., Horowitz, A., Jones, I., Kalof, L., Karlsson, E., … Larson, G. (2020). Humanity's best friend: A dog-centric approach to addressing global challenges. Animals 10, 502. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10030502CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tehrani, J. (2006). The uses of ethnography in the science of cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(4), 363364. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X06419087CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tehrani, J. J. (2013). The phylogeny of Little Red Riding Hood. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e78871. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078871CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thuillard, M., Le Quellec, J.-L., d'Huy, J., & Berezkin, Y. (2018). A large-scale study of world myths. Trames: A Journal of the Humanities and Social Sciences, 22(4), 407424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turchin, P., Brennan, R., Currie, T., Feeney, K., Francois, P., Hoyer, D., … Whitehouse, H. (2015). Seshat: The Global History Databank. Cliodynamics, 6. https://doi.org/10.21237/C7CLIO6127917Google Scholar
Turner, T. S. (2009). The crisis of late structuralism. Perspectivism and animism: rethinking culture, nature, spirit, and bodiliness. Tipití: Journal of the Society for the Anthropology of Lowland South America, 7(1), 1.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. (2019). Cultural anthropology's love–hate relationship with evolution: What will the future bring? Zeitschrift für Ethnologie, 144, 7792.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C., & Michel, K. (2016). The Good book of human nature: An evolutionary reading of the Bible. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Viveiros de Castro, E., (2014). Cannibal metaphysics. Univocal Publishing.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. (2012). Facts force you to believe in them; perspectives encourage you to believe out of them. An introduction to Viveiros de Castro's magisterial essay. In: E. Viveiros de Castro, Cosmological perspectivism in Amazonia and elsewhere. HAU Network of Ethnographic Theory.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2012). Whence and whither sociocultural anthropology. In Slingerland, E. & Collard, M. (Eds.), Creating consilience: Integrating the sciences and the humanities. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199794393.003.0007Google Scholar
Wilcken, P. (2010). Claude Lévi-Strauss: The poet in the laboratory. Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Wilkins, A. S. (2009). Charles Darwin: Genius or plodder? Genetics, 183(3), 773777. https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.109.110452CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology. The new synthesis. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O.. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. Thorndike Press.Google Scholar
Windelband, W. (1894). Geschichte und Naturwissenschaft. Rede zum Antritt des Rektorats der Kaiser-Wilhelms-Universität Strassburg, 1. Mai 1894. In: Präludien. Aufsätze und Reden zur Philosophie und ihrer Geschichte 2 (9). Edition Tübingen 1924. Mohr Publisher.Google Scholar
Youngblood, M., & Lahti, D. (2018). A bibliometric analysis of the interdisciplinary field of cultural evolution. Palgrave Communications, 4(1), 120. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-018-0175-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, H., & Mace, R. (2021). Cultural extinction in evolutionary perspective. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 3, article e30. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2021.25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zumwalt, R. L. (2019). Franz Boas: The emergence of the anthropologist. University of Nebraska Press. https://books.google.com/books?id=i5uzDwAAQBAJCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Claude Lévi-Strauss and the HRAF in Paris. Lévi-Strauss posed fundamental questions on cultural diversity and origins, and advocated extensive examination of data to address them. Keystone-France/Gamma-Keystone via Getty Images, with permission.