Hostname: page-component-76dd75c94c-ccc76 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T07:16:03.430Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Civilian moral injury: associations with trauma type and high-frequency heart rate variability in two trauma-exposed community-based samples

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2022

Emma C. Lathan*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
Abigail Powers
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
Anna Kottakis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
Alfonsina Guelfo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
Greg J. Siegle
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
Jessica A. Turner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA
Matthew D. Turner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA
Vijwala Yakkanti
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
Jahnvi Jain
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, TX, USA
Yara Mekawi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, USA
Andrew P. Teer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
Joseph M. Currier
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of South Alabama, Mobile, AL, USA
Negar Fani
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Emma C. Lathan, E-mail: emma.catherine.lathan-powell@emory.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Moral injury exposure (MIE) and distress (MID) may indirectly affect the relationship between trauma exposure and alterations in autonomic regulation [assessed via high-frequency heart rate variability (hfHRV)] in civilians, but this has not been tested in prior research. We conducted two exploratory studies to examine trauma types' associations with MIE and MID among civilian medical patients (Study 1) and explore how these facets may indirectly affect the relationship between trauma type and hfHRV among civilians seeking mental health services (Study 2).

Methods

Participants recruited from a public hospital and/or community advertisements (Study 1, n = 72, 87.5% Black, 83.3% women; Study 2, n = 46, 71.7% Black, 97.8% women) completed measures assessing trauma type, MIE, and MID. In Study 1, trauma types that emerged as significant correlates of MIE and MID were entered into separate linear regression analyses. Trauma types identified were included as predictors in indirect effects models with MIE or MID as the mediator and resting hfHRV (assayed via electrocardiography) as the outcome.

Results

Childhood sexual abuse emerged as the only significant predictor of MIE, b = 0.38, p < 0.001; childhood sexual abuse, b = 0.26, p < 0.05, and adulthood sexual assault, b = 0.23, p < 0.05 were significant predictors of MID. Participants with greater MIE and MID demonstrated lower hfHRV. Adulthood sexual assault showed an indirect effect on hfHRV through MID, B = −0.10, s.e. = 0.06, 95%CI (−0.232 to −0.005).

Conclusions

Moral injury was uniquely associated with sexual violence and lower hfHRV in civilians. Data highlight moral injury as a pathway through which autonomic dysregulation may emerge and its salience for trauma treatment selection.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Exposure to traumatic events, or situations that involve actual or threatened death, serious injury, or sexual violence, is an established risk factor for various physical and mental health conditions including chronic pain, cardiovascular disease, and posttraumatic stress, depressive and anxiety disorders, among others (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Cardoso and Koenen2017; Liu, Petukhova, & Sampson, Reference Liu, Petukhova and Sampson2017). The onset, severity, and course of trauma-related outcomes, however, are largely dependent on the ways in which a traumatic event is appraised (Woud, Verwoerd, & Krans, Reference Woud, Verwoerd and Krans2017). Indeed, trauma appraisals, defined as the cognitive interpretations of or meaning assigned to trauma, often better predict psychological symptoms than the event itself (DePrince, Chu, & Pineda, Reference DePrince, Chu and Pineda2011; Lathan, Selwyn, & Langhinrichsen-Rohling, Reference Lathan, Selwyn and Langhinrichsen-Rohling2021; Martin, Cromer, DePrince, & Freyd, Reference Martin, Cromer, DePrince and Freyd2013), suggesting how an individual thinks about or processes the event can foster or impede recovery. As such, negative trauma appraisals, such as self-blame, guilt, shame, or betrayal, are thought to play a crucial role in the development and maintenance of post-trauma pathology (Woud et al., Reference Woud, Verwoerd and Krans2017; Woud, Kleim, & Cwik, Reference Woud, Kleim and Cwik2019).

Moral injury, or a sense of one's moral or ethical code being violated, is a relatively new and evolving construct that has been conceptualized as a type of trauma appraisal (Hoffman & Nickerson, Reference Hoffman and Nickerson2021). Moral injury is characterized by severe psychological distress and functional impairments that may occur after experiencing, witnessing, perpetrating, or failing to prevent stressful events that transgress core values and moral beliefs (McEwen, Alisic, & Jobson, Reference McEwen, Alisic and Jobson2021); namely, distinct psychological symptoms, such as guilt, shame, self-blame, disgust, betrayal, and existential issues, may arise when an individual experiences a traumatic event that conflicts with their deeply held moral beliefs about themselves, others, and the world (Litz et al., Reference Litz, Stein, Delaney, Lebowitz, Nash, Silva and Maguen2009).

Although moral injury often co-occurs with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), recent clinical and neurobiological data support moral injury as a unique, transdiagnostic construct (Barnes, Hurley, & Taber, Reference Barnes, Hurley and Taber2019; Sun et al., Reference Sun, Phillips, Mulready, Zablonski, Turner, Turner and Morey2019) that essentially involves transgressions of an individual's moral beliefs and expectations, i.e., their conceptualization of ‘what's right and wrong.’ Moral injury is often preceded by an event that evokes later dissonance regarding the situation and the morality of the actions taken by the self or others. For example, if a person views themselves as benevolent and altruistic, the moral injury may develop if the individual directly contributed (even unintentionally) to an event that endangered the life or well-being of another person. The moral injury was originally conceptualized to better understand service members and veterans' combat-related experiences (e.g. perpetrating violence, being betrayed by a trusted authority figure) and resultant symptoms (e.g. guilt, shame, betrayal, disgust) that were not fully captured by existing PTSD criteria (Drescher et al., Reference Drescher, Foy, Kelly, Leshner, Schutz and Litz2011; Litz et al., Reference Litz, Stein, Delaney, Lebowitz, Nash, Silva and Maguen2009). As such, moral injury has been primarily studied in military populations to date (Currier, Drescher, & Nieuwsma, Reference Currier, Drescher, Nieuwsma, Currier, Drescher and Nieuwsma2021).

More recently, researchers have begun to assess the moral injury and its effects in civilian populations. Fani et al. (Reference Fani, Currier, Turner, Guelfo, Kloess, Jain and Turner2021) found that greater trauma exposure across the lifespan was linked to greater exposure to morally injurious events (i.e. moral injury exposure, MIE) and the presence of moral injury distress (MID) in a chronically trauma-exposed community sample; a history of childhood maltreatment was associated with MIE, while both childhood maltreatment and interpersonal trauma in adulthood were associated with MID (Fani et al., Reference Fani, King, Powers, Hardy, Siegle, Blair and Bradley2021). Findings provide preliminary support for MIE and MID as distinct components of moral injury development among civilians (Currier et al., Reference Currier, Farnsworth, Drescher, McDermott, Sims and Albright2017; Currier, Holland, Rojas-Flores, Herrera, & Foy, Reference Currier, Holland, Rojas-Flores, Herrera and Foy2015; Fani et al., Reference Fani, King, Powers, Hardy, Siegle, Blair and Bradley2021). While a growing body of literature suggests greater cumulative trauma exposure is related to higher levels of moral injury among trauma-exposed civilians (Currier et al., Reference Currier, Holland, Rojas-Flores, Herrera and Foy2015; Fani et al., Reference Fani, King, Powers, Hardy, Siegle, Blair and Bradley2021; Hoffman, Liddell, Bryant, & Nickerson, Reference Hoffman, Liddell, Bryant and Nickerson2018), no prior study has examined the associations between specific trauma types, MIE, and MID. A more thorough understanding of which trauma types best predict MIE and MID is needed to better detect and target moral injury and its effects during patient encounters with trauma-exposed civilians.

Deleterious outcomes, including suicide attempts, anger, and symptoms of PTSD, anxiety, depression, and burnout, have been noted across civilians who endorse MIE and MID (Currier et al., Reference Currier, Holland, Rojas-Flores, Herrera and Foy2015; Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Liddell, Bryant and Nickerson2018; Nickerson et al., Reference Nickerson, Schnyder, Bryant, Schick, Mueller and Morina2015, Reference Nickerson, Hoffman, Schick, Schnyder, Bryant and Morina2018; Plouffe et al., Reference Plouffe, Nazarov, Forchuk, Gargala, Deda, Le and Richardson2021). Going beyond psychological symptoms, a crucial trauma-related outcome that remains unstudied in relation to moral injury is autonomic nervous system (ANS) dysfunction. The ANS comprises parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous systems, which work in conjunction to maintain homeostasis by regulating the cardiovascular, respiratory, and digestive systems and allow adaptive adjustment to internal and external stimuli (Jänig, Reference Jänig2022). It is well-established that trauma exposure disrupts the rhythmic balance between the parasympathetic and sympathetic branches of the ANS (Schauer & Elbert, Reference Schauer and Elbert2010) such that activation of the parasympathetic branch, which is responsible for restful and adaptive emotion regulatory responses, is suppressed (Gillie & Thayer, Reference Gillie and Thayer2014).

High-frequency heart rate variability (hfHRV) is a physiological marker of parasympathetic nervous system tone (Berntson et al., Reference Berntson, Bigger, Eckberg, Grossman, Kaufmann, Malik and van der Molen1997; Berntson, Quigley, & Lozano, Reference Berntson, Quigley, Lozano, Cacioppo, Tassinary and Berntson2007) that reflects the fluctuation in the length of intervals between heart beats (Kim, Cheon, Bai, Lee, & Koo, Reference Kim, Cheon, Bai, Lee and Koo2018). Low resting hfHRV has been associated with greater vulnerability to physical and psychological stressors and disease, including cardiovascular disorders and inflammation (Alen, Parenteau, Sloan, & Hostinar, Reference Alen, Parenteau, Sloan and Hostinar2021; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Cheon, Bai, Lee and Koo2018; Kubota, Chen, Whitsel, & Folsom, Reference Kubota, Chen, Whitsel and Folsom2017). Low resting hfHRV has been consistently associated with greater trauma exposure (Liddell et al., Reference Liddell, Kemp, Steel, Nickerson, Bryant, Tam and Silove2016) and PTSD (Gillie & Thayer, Reference Gillie and Thayer2014; Schneider & Schwerdtfeger, Reference Schneider and Schwerdtfeger2020); alterations in hfHRV in adults have the strongest associations with early life maltreatment or associated psychological factors (e.g. emotion dysregulation), as opposed to psychological diagnoses per se (Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Müller, Zastrow, Schmidinger, Bohus, Herpertz and Bertsch2016).

The ability to contextualize experiences by using adaptive appraisals is related to reduced levels of psychophysiological distress (Schartau, Dalgleish, & Dunn, Reference Schartau, Dalgleish and Dunn2009). Previous findings link shame and blame appraisals with low HRV and other markers of autonomic arousal (Freed & D'Andrea, Reference Freed and D'Andrea2015; León, Hernández, Rodríguez, & Vila, Reference León, Hernández, Rodríguez and Vila2008), suggesting moral injury appraisals may be a key target for psychological intervention. Given established connections between hfHRV and both trauma and other appraisals, it is possible that specific trauma types (e.g. interpersonal trauma) are indirectly associated with lower hfHRV through the effects of MIE and MID. However, to our knowledge, physiological correlates of MIE and MID have not yet been explored, including racially marginalized individuals (e.g. Black individuals), who may experience moral injury more frequently due to witnessing and directly experiencing community violence and racial injustice. Further, Black individuals have also been historically excluded from psychophysiological research (Bradford et al., Reference Bradford, DeFalco, Perkins, Carbajal, Kwasa, Goodman and Joyner2022). Given the heterogeneity in neurobiological responses to trauma (Schmidt, Kaltwasser, & Wotjak, Reference Schmidt, Kaltwasser and Wotjak2013) and how psychophysiological profiles (i.e. hfHRV) associated with PTSD appear to predict response to first-line trauma treatments (Thomaes et al., Reference Thomaes, Dorrepaal, Draijer, Jansma, Veltman and van Balkom2014), it is important to assess whether moral injury may have an indirect impact on these responses; moral injury is a potential mechanism through which autonomic dysregulation occurs among trauma-exposed civilians, but this possibility has not yet been tested. If the moral injury is a mechanism for the development of autonomic dysregulation in the aftermath of trauma, it merits attention as a trauma treatment target.

Study 1 and study 2

Given the paucity of research on moral injury occurring among civilian trauma survivors, particularly among individuals of color, this exploratory study examined trauma-related and physiological correlates (specifically, hfHRV) of MIE and MID in two trauma-exposed, community-based samples that predominantly include Black Americans, extending extant research in two distinct ways. Study 1 extends the findings of Fani et al. (Reference Fani, King, Powers, Hardy, Siegle, Blair and Bradley2021) by examining which type(s) of traumatic events are most predictive of the exposure and distress components of moral injury in a sample of majority Black American civilians seeking medical care from an urban, publicly funded (i.e. safety net) hospital. We hypothesized that interpersonal traumas would be most strongly associated with MIE and MID, given our prior research (Fani et al., Reference Fani, King, Powers, Hardy, Siegle, Blair and Bradley2021).

Study 2 explores associations between moral injury and hfHRV in a demographically similar sample of civilians (majority Black American) seeking mental health services for PTSD. We hypothesized that higher MIE and MID would be associated with lower hfHRV. Where significant associations were observed between trauma type and MIE and MID in Study 1, we examined these trauma types as predictors of hfHRV in Study 2 and tested for indirect effects of MIE and/or MID on the association between trauma types and hfHRV.

Study 1 method

Participants and procedure

Participants (n = 72; M age = 40.6 years; s.d.age = 12.5 years) were primarily Black (90.3%, n = 65), women (84.7%, n = 61), high school graduates (or equivalent; 84.7%, n = 61), and experiencing significant economic disadvantage (65.2%, n = 47; i.e. reported a monthly household income of less than $2000). See Table 1 for sample characteristics. Participants were recruited from a large, publicly funded healthcare system in Atlanta, Georgia for involvement in an ongoing study of trauma exposure and related symptoms in underserved, primarily Black communities Grady Trauma Project. As part of the larger study, trained experimenters approached patients in medical clinics regarding potential participation. During the COVID-19 pandemic, patients with medical appointments were contacted via telephone and invited to participate. The experimenters verbally administered a variety of self-report measures to consenting individuals. All procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. Informed consent was obtained from all study participants after the nature of the procedures was explained. The MIE and MID data of 39 Study 1 participants (54%) have been previously published elsewhere (Fani et al., Reference Fani, King, Powers, Hardy, Siegle, Blair and Bradley2021).

Table 1. Study 1 and Study 2 sample characteristics

Measures

Trauma exposure

The Traumatic Events Inventory (TEI; Gillespie et al., Reference Gillespie, Bradley, Mercer, Smith, Conneely, Gapen and Ressler2009; Mekawi et al., Reference Mekawi, Kuzyk, Dixon, McKenna, Camacho, de Andino and Powers2021) was used to assess participants' self-reported exposure to 14 different types of traumatic events. Each item was rated on a binary scale (0 = No, 1 = Yes). Six trauma type variables were generated by collapsing similar traumas into broad binary categories: experiencing childhood abuse (three items; e.g. physical, sexual, emotional), sexual assault in adulthood (one item), witnessing violence (six items), experiencing physical assault by a non-partner (two items), experiencing intimate partner violence (IPV; two items), and exposure to a serious accident, injury, or illness (three items). For example, if a participant endorsed witnessing an attack with or without a weapon, witnessing violence between caregivers, witnessing the murder of a family member or friend, they were coded as having witnessed violence (0 = Did not witness violence, 1 = Witnessed violence). A cumulative trauma exposure variable was generated by summing the total number of trauma types endorsed.

Moral injury

The Moral Injury Exposure and Symptom Scale – Civilian (MIESS-C; Fani et al., Reference Fani, King, Powers, Hardy, Siegle, Blair and Bradley2021) is a 10-item self-report questionnaire measuring moral injury development across two subscales: MIE (five items; e.g. ‘I saw things that were morally wrong’) and MID (five items; e.g. ‘I am troubled because I violated my morals by failing to do something that I felt I should have done’). In the current study, participants were briefed with the following, ‘In this scale we will be asking you about events that might have conflicted with your morality or your sense of right and wrong.’ Responses ranged from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 6 (Strongly Agree). Internal consistencies of the MIESS-C subscales were in the acceptable range (MIE, α = 0.71; MID, α = 0.65; Ursachi, Horodnic, and Zait, Reference Ursachi, Horodnic and Zait2015).

Data analyses

Seventy-five individuals were assessed for inclusion in the current study. Of these, three individuals denied exposure to one or more of the trauma types assessed, and therefore, were removed from additional analyses. Thus, 72 participants comprised the Study 1 sample, though sample sizes differ by analyses based on the availability of data. Per trauma type, between 1.4% and 2.8% of all cases had missing data (n = 1–2). Only one participant (1.4%, n = 1) had missing data on the MID variable. Because missing data were minimal, they were handled via mean imputation.

Pearson's, point-biserial, and phi-coefficient correlations were conducted to determine bivariate associations among trauma types, MIE, and MID, examined as different families of tests. Bonferroni correction was applied to correct for error due to multiple comparisons; statistical significance was set at p < 0.008 for each family of tests. Trauma types that emerged as significant correlates of MIE and MID were entered into two separate linear regression analyses with MIE and MID serving as the dependent variables. Given the exploratory nature of this study, statistical significance was set at p < 0.05 for regression analyses.

Study 1 results

All participants (n = 72) reported exposure to at least one of six trauma types [e.g. experiencing childhood abuse (i.e. physical, sexual, emotional), experiencing sexual assault in adulthood, witnessing violence, experiencing physical assault by non-partner, experiencing IPV, exposure to a serious accident, injury, or illness; M types = 3.7; s.d.types = 1.5]. Notably, the majority of participants endorsed witnessing violence (81.9%, n = 59), exposure to a serious accident or injury (73.6%, n = 53), experiencing childhood abuse (66.7%, n = 48), experiencing physical assault by someone other than a romantic partner (61.1%, n = 44), and experiencing IPV (55.6%, n = 40). See Table 2 for descriptive statistics.

Table 2. Study 1 and Study 2 descriptive statistics

Presence/absence of childhood abuse, rpb(72) = 0.45 p < 0.001, was positively associated with MIE; this relationship remained significant following correction for multiple comparisons. See Table 3 for bivariate associations. As such, we conducted a follow-up regression analysis to examine the strength of associations of MIE with specific types of childhood abuse (i.e. physical, sexual, emotional) as predictors. Regression analysis revealed a significant overall model, F (3,68) = 9.63, p < 0.001, accounting for 29.8% of the variance in MIE. Childhood sexual abuse, b = 0.38, p < 0.001, emerged as the only significant predictor of MIE.

Table 3. Study 1 Pearson's, point biserial, and phi-coefficient correlations

**p ⩽ 0.008, ***p ⩽ 0.001.

Presence/absence of adulthood sexual assault, rpb(72) = 0.36, p = 0.002, and childhood abuse, rpb(72) = 0.34, p = 0.004, were both positively correlated with MID; these relationships remained significant following correction for multiple comparisons. A follow-up regression analysis was conducted to examine the strength of associations between MID, adulthood sexual assault, and childhood abuse types (i.e. physical, sexual, emotional). Regression analysis revealed a significant overall model, F (4,67) = 5.02, p < 0.001, accounting for 23.1% of the variance in MID. Childhood sexual abuse, b = 0.26, p < 0.05, and adulthood sexual assault, b = 0.23, p < 0.05, emerged as significant predictors of MID.

Study 2 method

Participants and procedure

Participants (n = 46; M age = 43.2 years; s.d.age = 11.4 years) were primarily Black (71.7%, n = 33), women (97.8%, n = 45), high school graduates (or equivalent; 89.1%, n = 41), and experiencing significant economic disadvantage (52.2%, n = 24; i.e. reported a monthly household income of less than $2000). Participants were enrolled in a PTSD intervention study (Fani et al., Reference Fani, King, Powers, Hardy, Siegle, Blair and Bradley2019) involving electrocardiogram data collected at rest; several of these participants were recruited via the urban hospital and community advertisements. The Study 1 and Study 2 samples were independent; none of the Study 1 participants was included in Study 2. Specific data collection and study procedures can be found in Study 1. MIE and MID data from 30 Study 2 participants (65%) have been previously published elsewhere (Fani et al., Reference Fani, King, Powers, Hardy, Siegle, Blair and Bradley2021).

All participants reported exposure to two or more of six types of trauma [e.g. experiencing childhood abuse (i.e. physical, sexual, emotional), experiencing sexual assault in adulthood, witnessing violence, experiencing physical assault by non-partner, experiencing IPV, exposure to a serious accident, injury, or illness; M types = 3.9; s.d.types = 1.3]. The trauma types most frequently endorsed included witnessing violence (84.8%, n = 39), experiencing childhood abuse (82.6%, n = 38), and exposure to a serious accident, injury, or illness (80.4%, n = 37). See Table 2 for descriptive statistics.

Measures

See Study 1 Method section for information on the measurement of trauma exposure and moral injury.

Electrocardiography (ECG)

ECG data were collected during intervention sessions to derive measures of hfHRV from a two-lead montage (right neck, left wrist; 2000 Hz, no filters), using a BIOPAC ECG100C Electrocardiogram amplifier (BIOPAC Systems, Inc) via their AcqKnowledge software version 4.0 (BIOPAC System, Inc).

Task design. During the first intervention visit, participants sat in a chair in a sound-attenuated chamber in front of a computer screen and microphone, while being monitored by a researcher. Instructions appeared on the screen, which instructed them to either engage in breath focus (10 trials, half of which were augmented with breath-synchronized vibration) or rest (five trials), each for one min. These conditions were randomized throughout a 15-min session to yield five one-min ‘rest’ segments (Baek, Cho, Cho, & Woo, Reference Baek, Cho, Cho and Woo2015), which were analyzed in the current manuscript.

ECG Processing. ECG data were exported to text files which were processed and cleaned using via custom Matlab code. R-wave peaks were extracted and converted to inter-beat interval series. Series were subjected to continuous Morelet waveform transforms to yield a task-wise running (100 Hz) estimate of power in the hfHRV band (0.18–0.4 Hz) using the HRV-AS package (Ramshur, Reference Ramshur2010, available at https://github.com/jramshur/HRVAS). Further information on processing techniques used can be found in Rabellino et al. (Reference Rabellino, D'Andrea, Siegle, Frewen, Minshew, Densmore and Lanius2017).

Quality assurance metrics included: heart rate between 40 and 120 beats per minute; no significant abrupt spikes or ectopic beats; electrocardiograms that appear normative with distinct R peaks and QRS complexes. Data that were flagged on quality assurance metrics (e.g. HR outside the 40–120 beats per minute range) were manually checked and corrected by hand-marking missed beats and hand-deleting erroneous heart-beat detections. Hand-deletion was relevant for eight participants, whose baseline electrocardiogram data were found to be unusable.

Mean hfHRV was calculated within each of the rest trials and averaged across these trials for the first assessment day per participant. hfHRV data were Winsorized (i.e. data outside the Tukey Hinges of 1.5 times the interquartile range from the 25th and 75th percentiles were rescaled to the last good value within the Tukey Hinges) and entered into group analyses.

Data analyses

For trauma type, 2.2 to 4.3% of all cases had missing data (n = 1–2). Eight participants' baseline electrocardiogram data (17.4%) were unusable due to noise. Mean imputation was used to handle missing-ness for trauma type and hfHRV.

Pearson's bivariate correlations were conducted to examine associations among MIE, MID, and hfHRV. Hayes' SPSS PROCESS Macro was used to conduct follow-up analyses (Zhao, Lynch, & Chen, Reference Zhao, Lynch and Chen2010). Trauma type(s) that emerged as significant predictors of MIE and MID via Study 1 were included in Study 2 analyses to better understand relations with hfHRV. Separate mediation models were conducted. In each model, trauma type was entered as the predictor, MIE or MID as the mediator, and hfHRV as the outcome. In each model (n = 46), unstandardized indirect effects were computed for each of 5000 bootstrapped samples. Bonferroni correction was applied to correct for error due to multiple comparisons; statistical significance was set at p < 0.025 for each family of tests.

Study 2 results

Moral injury exposure

As expected, results of bivariate correlations revealed that MIE was associated with lower hfHRV at the bivariate level, r(46) = −0.31, p < 0.05 (See Fig. 1). The indirect effects model revealed that childhood sexual abuse was not significantly associated with MIE, B = 0.48, s.e. = 1.77, p = 0.78. MIE was significantly associated with decreased hfHRV, B = −0.03, s.e. = 0.01, p = 0.03. No significant indirect effect of childhood sexual abuse on hfHRV was found, B = −0.01, s.e. = 0.05, 95% CI (−0.104 to 0.086).

Fig. 1. Scatterplot of hfHRV by moral injury exposure (MIE).

Moral injury distress

MID was not significantly correlated with lower hfHRV at the bivariate level, r(46) = −0.28, p = 0.06 (See Fig. 2). When included in an indirect effects model, adulthood sexual assault was positively associated with MID, B = 4.17, s.e. = 1.90, p = 0.03, and MID was associated with decreased hfHRV, B = −0.02, s.e. = 0.01, p = 0.03. There was a small indirect effect from adulthood sexual assault to hfHRV via MID after correction for multiple comparisons, B = −0.10, s.e. = 0.06, 95% CI (−0.232 to −0.005) (See Fig. 3). In the second model, childhood abuse did not significantly predict MID, B = 1.31, s.e. = 2.03, p = 0.52. However, MID significantly predicted hfHRV, B = −0.02, s.e. = 0.01, p = 0.05. There was no significant indirect effect of childhood sexual abuse on hfHRV B = −0.03, s.e. = 0.04, 95% CI (−0.140 to 0.044).

Fig. 2. Scatterplot of hfHRV by moral injury distress (MID).

Fig. 3. Simple mediation model.

General discussion

The current studies examined relations among the exposure and distress components of moral injury, trauma type, and autonomic response in trauma-exposed civilians. In line with our first hypothesis, Study 1 found that exposure to certain traumatic events was tied to greater endorsement of moral injury appraisals, with slightly differing patterns across the MIE and MID components of moral injury development. Study 2 demonstrated that participants with higher MIE and MID demonstrated proportionally lower hfHRV, indicating that moral injury is linked to significant autonomic dysregulation in civilians. Follow-up analyses revealed that there was a nominal indirect effect from adulthood sexual assault to low hfHRV via MID, suggesting identifying and addressing distress appraisals may help disrupt the established pathway from sexual assault to low hfHRV among trauma-exposed Black women (Lee & Theus, Reference Lee and Theus2012).

Regarding specific trauma types, experiencing childhood abuse was correlated with MIE, while sexual assault in adulthood was correlated with both MIE and MID. Witnessing violence, experiencing physical assault by a non-partner, experiencing intimate partner violence, and exposure to serious accidents, injuries, or illnesses were related to neither MIE nor MID in this population. Follow-up analyses revealed that, when compared to all other trauma types assessed, childhood sexual abuse had the strongest association with MIE, and childhood sexual abuse and sexual assault in adulthood had stronger associations with MID.

Our results suggest that sexual violence may be a particularly potent risk factor for moral injury appraisals, especially distress related to morally injurious events. This may be, in part, due to the established relations between sexual violence and the core features of moral injury, such as guilt, shame, existential conflict, and mistrust (Jinkerson, Reference Jinkerson2016). More broadly, it is possible that the relationship between sexual violence and moral injury is influenced by the internalization of rape culture (i.e. widespread acceptance of rape myth beliefs, or misconceptions about sexual assault that shift the blame from perpetrator to victim), as survivors tend to blame themselves for the assault, feel ashamed, and question their own morality and dignity (Bhuptani & Messman-Moore, Reference Bhuptani, Messman-Moore, O'Donohue and Schewe2019). Because trauma appraisals can influence the onset, severity, and course of mental health outcomes, future research should examine the roles survivors' broad beliefs about sexual assault and particular negative cognitions about the traumatic event play in MIE and MID after sexual violence.

Moreover, sexual violence across the lifespan was differentially related to moral injury appraisals. In Study 1, experiencing childhood sexual abuse was related to both MIE and MID, while sexual assault in adulthood was related to MID. Therefore, it is possible that the risk for exposure and distress varies with the developmental stage. It is important to note that very little is known about the relations between sexual violence and moral injury, so the mechanisms underlying these lifespan differences remain unclear. It has been hypothesized, however, that moral injury has a ‘kindling effect with respect to trauma’ (Fani et al., Reference Fani, King, Powers, Hardy, Siegle, Blair and Bradley2019, p. 7); perhaps, sexual violence-related moral distress increases over time. In Study 2, however, childhood sexual abuse was related to neither MIE nor MID, although the relationship between adulthood sexual assault and MID observed in Study 1 remained. Altogether, adulthood sexual assault seems to be a more reliable correlate of moral injury across populations than childhood sexual abuse. Current results, in conjunction with similar findings (Fani et al., Reference Fani, King, Powers, Hardy, Siegle, Blair and Bradley2021), highlight the importance of further exploring if, how, and why sexual violence across the lifespan differentially relates to MIE and MID.

In addition, as most of our sample were Black women, it is likely that these participants have experienced other forms of pervasive violence and injustice – namely, racial trauma (Department of Justice, 2014). This term has been used to describe ongoing individual racial discrimination and systematic marginalization (Comas-Díaz, Hall, & Neville, Reference Comas-Díaz, Hall and Neville2019) that is experienced more frequently by Black Americans than any other racial group (Anderson & Stevenson, Reference Anderson and Stevenson2019). Because racial trauma is a particularly salient trauma type for Black Americans, it may be particularly relevant to their perceptions of MIE and MID. Experiences of gendered racial discrimination may be especially harmful for individuals with intersectional identities, such as Black women (Moody & Lewis, Reference Moody and Lewis2019). Further, recent evidence suggests an inverse effect between discrimination and resting HRV among African American students (Hill et al., Reference Hill, Hoggard, Richmond, Gray, Williams and Thayer2017). While present findings advance our knowledge of how childhood maltreatment and adult sexual trauma are associated with MIE and MID among Black Americans, there is much left to be learned about moral injury's potential role in the relationship between racial trauma and hfHRV.

Moreover, racially marginalized women experience disproportionate rates of sexual assault and PTSD (Asnaani & Hall-Clark, Reference Asnaani and Hall-Clark2017; Gluck et al., Reference Gluck, Hartzell, Dixon, Michopoulos, Powers, Stevens and Gillespie2021; Merrick, Ford, Ports, & Guinn, Reference Merrick, Ford, Ports and Guinn2018); further, many of these individuals report experiencing moral injury, and MIE has been associated with emotion dysregulation (Ray, Hunsanger, Nagy, & Pickett, Reference Ray, Hunsanger, Nagy and Pickett2021) and past suicide attempt (Fani et al., Reference Fani, King, Powers, Hardy, Siegle, Blair and Bradley2021). However, little attention has been paid to the role of moral injury appraisals in functional impairment in the aftermath of trauma, and similarly, they are often ignored as a treatment target. While further investigation is warranted, results indicate a systematic assessment of moral injury in trauma-exposed civilians may help inform trauma-focused treatment selection and targets. For example, a mental health provider may consider Trauma Informed Guilt Reduction (TrIGR) therapy (Norman et al., Reference Norman, Allard, Browne, Capone, Davis and Kubany2019) or Adaptive Disclosure (Gray, Binion, Amaya, & Litz, Reference Gray, Binion, Amaya, Litz, Currier, Drescher and Nieuwsma2021) in addition to more traditional PTSD treatments (i.e. Cognitive Processing Therapy, Prolonged Exposure) when a client endorses experiencing one or more of the core components of moral injury.

Current results should be interpreted in light of specific study limitations, including reliance on self-report measures for trauma history as well as cross-sectional methodology, which bars causal conclusions. Underlying causal relationships may exist, such that the relationships are reversed or reciprocal, or an unmeasured factor confounds findings. Moreover, our studies' limited sample sizes (Study 1, n = 75; Study 2, n = 46) may have prevented statistically significant findings from being detected (i.e. type II error) given insufficient statistical power. The internal consistencies of MIE and MID were in the acceptable range but lower than expected. Additional research on the psychometric properties of the MIESS-C is needed to inform best practices related to the assessment of moral injury appraisals.

Further, the study measured hfHRV using five one-min intervals, which occurred randomly within a 15-min breath-focus task. While guidelines for hfHRV research generally recommend five-minutes of contiguous data, one-min intervals are considered acceptable (Baek et al., Reference Baek, Cho, Cho and Woo2015), particularly within the context of the employed wavelet analytic technique, which assesses hfHRV continuously throughout the task (Laborde, Mosley, & Thayer, Reference Laborde, Mosley and Thayer2017); the extent to which our resting assessments also incorporate recovery from previous task-blocks or anticipation of future task-blocks is unclear. Moving forward, further research examining moral injury's associations with other markers of autonomic function, such as pupil diameter and galvanic skin response, is warranted to obtain a more comprehensive understanding of the impact of moral injury on autonomic regulation.

Future research may benefit from attempting to replicate the current studies in larger treatment-seeking and community-based samples with a longer contiguous resting-state assessment. Additionally, our findings reflect the study populations, two groups of primarily low-income, trauma-exposed, Black women seeking physical or mental health treatment in the southeastern United States. Although results add to the limited number of studies examining hfHRV among Black women, it is possible that our results may not generalize to other race and gender civilians (e.g. white individuals, men) or in other geographic regions, non-treatment seeking civilians, and those who are insured or seeking care at private hospitals.

Despite noted limitations, these findings contribute to the literature by providing the first examination of moral injury's relations with trauma type and hfHRV among civilians. We found strong links between moral injury, adulthood sexual assault and autonomic disruptions among two samples of primarily Black women seeking health care. Overall, our data support the notion that targeting moral injury appraisals via culturally responsive, trauma-informed psychological interventions may help to reduce autonomic dysfunction, particularly among Black women with histories of sexual violence.

Acknowledgement

We wish to thank members of the Grady Trauma Project and the Fani Affective Neuroscience Lab for their assistance with data collection. We also thank all of our participants for their time and involvement in this study.

Financial support

This work was primarily supported by the National Institutes of Mental Health (MH101380 to NF) and the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (AT011267 to NF and GJS). Support was also received from the Emory Medical Care Foundation and American Psychological Association, Society for Clinical Neuropsychology.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Alen, N. V., Parenteau, A. M., Sloan, R. P., & Hostinar, C. E. (2021). Heart rate variability and circulatory inflammatory markers in midlife. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity Health, 15, 100273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R. E., & Stevenson, H. C. (2019). RECASTing racial stress and trauma: Theorizing the healing potential of racial socialization in families. The American Psychologist, 74(1), 6375. doi: 10.1037/amp0000392CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asnaani, A., & Hall-Clark, B. (2017). Recent development in understanding ethnocultural and race differences in trauma exposure and PTSD. Current Opinion in Psychology, 14, 96101. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.12.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baek, H. J., Cho, C. H., Cho, J., & Woo, J. M. (2015). Reliability of ultra-short-term analysis as a surrogate of standard 5-min analysis of heart rate variability. Telemedicine and e-Health, 21(5), 404414. doi: 10.1089/tmj.2014.0104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, H. A., Hurley, R. A., & Taber, K. H. (2019). Moral injury and PTSD: Often co-occurring yet mechanistically different. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 31(2), 98103. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.19020036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berntson, G. G., Bigger, J. T. Jr., Eckberg, D. L., Grossman, P., Kaufmann, P. G., Malik, M., … van der Molen, M. W. (1997). Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology, 34(6), 623648. doi: 10.1111/j.14698986.1997.tb02140.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berntson, G. G., Quigley, K. S., & Lozano, D. (2007). Cardiovascular psychophysiology. In Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G. & Berntson, G. G. (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 182210). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511546396.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhuptani, P. H., & Messman-Moore, T. L. (2019). Blame and shame in sexual assault. In O'Donohue, W. & Schewe, P. (Eds.), Handbook of sexual assault and sexual assault prevention (pp. 309322). Cham, Switzerland: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-23645-8_18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradford, D. E., DeFalco, A., Perkins, E., Carbajal, I., Kwasa, J., Goodman, F. R., … Joyner, K. (2022). Whose signals are we amplifying? Towards a more equitable clinical psychophysiology. Clinical Psychological Science. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/c2nafCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comas-Díaz, L., Hall, G. N., & Neville, H. A. (2019). Racial trauma: Theory, research, and healing: Introduction to the special issue. American Psychologist, 74(1), 15. doi: 10.1037/amp0000442CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Currier, J. M., Drescher, K. D., & Nieuwsma, J. (2021). Introduction to moral injury. In Currier, J. M., Drescher, K. D. & Nieuwsma, J. (Eds.), Addressing moral injury in clinical practice (pp. 318). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/0000204-015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Currier, J. M., Farnsworth, J. K., Drescher, K. D., McDermott, R. C., Sims, B. M., & Albright, D. L. (2017). Development and evaluation of the expressions of moral injury scale – military version. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 25, 474488. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2170CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Currier, J. M., Holland, J. M., Rojas-Flores, L., Herrera, S., & Foy, D. (2015). Morally injurious experiences and meaning in Salvadorian teachers exposed to violence. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 7(1), 2433. doi: 10.1037/a0034092CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Department of Justice. (2014). Hate crime statistics. Washington, DC: Department of Justice. Retrieved May 23, 2022, from https://ucr.fbi.gov/hate-crime/2014.Google Scholar
DePrince, A. P., Chu, A. T., & Pineda, A. S. (2011). Links between specific posttrauma appraisals and three forms of trauma-related distress. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 3, 430441. doi: 10.1037/a0021576CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drescher, K. D., Foy, D. W., Kelly, C., Leshner, A., Schutz, K., & Litz, B. (2011). An exploration of the viability and usefulness of the construct of moral injury in war veterans. Traumatology, 17, 813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fani, N., Currier, J. M., Turner, M. D., Guelfo, A., Kloess, M., Jain, J., … Turner, J. A. (2021). Moral injury in civilians: associations with trauma exposure, PTSD, and suicide behavior. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 12(1), 1965464. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2021.1965464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fani, N., King, T. Z., Powers, A., Hardy, R. A., Siegle, G. J., Blair, R. J., … Bradley, B. (2019). Cognitive and neural facets of dissociation in a traumatized population. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 19(5), 863875. doi: 10.1037/emo0000466CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freed, S., & D'Andrea, W. (2015). Autonomic arousal and emotion in victims of interpersonal violence: Shame proneness but not anxiety predicts vagal tone. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 16(4), 367383. doi: 10.1080/15299732.2015.1004771CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillespie, C. F., Bradley, B., Mercer, K., Smith, A. K., Conneely, K., Gapen, M., … Ressler, K. J. (2009). Trauma exposure and stress-related disorders in inner city primary care patients. General Hospital Psychiatry, 31(6), 505514. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2009.05.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillie, B. L., & Thayer, J. F. (2014). Individual differences in resting heart rate variability and cognitive control in posttraumatic stress disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 758. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00758CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gluck, R. L., Hartzell, G. E., Dixon, H. D., Michopoulos, V., Powers, A., Stevens, J. S., … Gillespie, C. F. (2021). Trauma exposure and stress-related disorders in a large, urban, predominantly African-American, female sample. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 24(6), 893901. doi: 10.1007/s00737-021-01141-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, M. J., Binion, K., Amaya, S., & Litz, B. T. (2021). Adaptive disclosure: A novel evidence-based treatment for moral injury. In Currier, J. M., Drescher, K. D. & Nieuwsma, J. (Eds.), Addressing moral injury in clinical practice (pp. 183201). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/0000204-011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, L. K., Hoggard, L. S., Richmond, A. S., Gray, D. L., Williams, D. P., & Thayer, J. F. (2017). Examining the association between perceived discrimination and heart rate variability in African Americans. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 23(1), 514. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000076CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, J., Liddell, B., Bryant, R. A., & Nickerson, A. (2018). The relationship between moral injury appraisals, trauma exposure, and mental health in refugees. Depression and Anxiety, 35, 10301039. doi: 10.1002/da.22787CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, J., & Nickerson, A. (2021). The impact of moral-injury cognitions on psychological outcomes in refugees: An experimental investigation. Clinical Psychological Science, 10(4), 603–621. doi: 10.1177/21677026211039516.Google Scholar
Jänig, W. (2022). The integrative action of the autonomic nervous system: Neurobiology of homeostasis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jinkerson, J. D. (2016). Defining and assessing moral injury: A syndrome perspective. Traumatology, 22(2), 122130. doi: 10.1037/trm0000069CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Benjet, C., Bromet, E. J., Cardoso, G., … Koenen, K. C. (2017). Trauma and PTSD in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 8(sup.5), 1353383. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2017.1353383CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H. G., Cheon, E. J., Bai, D. S., Lee, Y. H., & Koo, B. H. (2018). Stress and heart rate variability: A meta-analysis and review of the literature. Psychiatry Investigation, 15(3), 235245. doi: 10.30773/pi.2017.08.17CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kubota, Y., Chen, L. Y., Whitsel, E. A., & Folsom, A. R. (2017). Heart rate variability and lifetime risk of cardiovascular disease: The atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Annals of Epidemiology, 27(10), 619625, e2. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2017.08.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laborde, S., Mosley, E., & Thayer, J. F. (2017). Heart rate variability and cardiac vagal tone in psychophysiological research: Recommendations for experiment planning, data analysis, and data reporting. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 213. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lathan, E. C., Selwyn, C. N., & Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J. (2021). The “3 Es” of trauma-informed care in a federally qualified health center: Traumatic event- and experience-related predictors of physical and mental health effects among female patients. Journal of Community Psychology, 49, 703724. doi: 10.1002/jcop22488CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, E. A., & Theus, S. A. (2012). Lower heart rate variability associated with military sexual trauma rape and posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Research for Nursing, 14(4), 412418. doi: 10.1177/1099800412454453CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
León, I., Hernández, J. A., Rodríguez, S., & Vila, J. (2008). When head is tempered by heart: Heart rate variability modulates perception of other-blame reducing anger. Motivation and Emotion, 33, 19. doi: 10.1007/s11031-008-9112-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liddell, B. J., Kemp, A. H., Steel, Z., Nickerson, A., Bryant, R. A., Tam, N., … Silove, D. (2016). Heart rate variability and the relationship between trauma exposure age, and psychopathology in a post-conflict setting. BMC Psychiatry, 16, 133. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-0850-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Litz, B. T., Stein, N., Delaney, E., Lebowitz, L., Nash, W. P., Silva, C., & Maguen, S. (2009). Moral injury and moral repair in war veterans: A preliminary model and intervention strategy. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(8), 695706. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.07.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, H., Petukhova, M. V., & Sampson, N. A. (2017). Association of DSM-IV posttraumatic stress disorder with traumatic experience type and history in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Journal of the American Medical Association Psychiatry, 74(3), 270281. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3783Google ScholarPubMed
Martin, C. G., Cromer, L. D., DePrince, A. P., & Freyd, J. J. (2013). The role of cumulative trauma, betrayal, and appraisals in understanding trauma symptomatology. Psychological Trauma, 52(2), 110118. doi: 10.1037/a0025686CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, C., Alisic, E., & Jobson, L. (2021). Moral injury and mental health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Traumatology, 27(3), 303315. doi: 10.1037/trm0000287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mekawi, Y., Kuzyk, E., Dixon, H. D., McKenna, B., Camacho, L., de Andino, A. M., … Powers, A. (2021). Characterizing typologies of polytraumatization: A replication and extension study examining internalizing and externalizing psychopathology in an urban population. Clinical Psychological Science, 9(6), 11441163. doi: 10.1177/21677026211000723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merrick, M. T., Ford, D. C., Ports, K. A., & Guinn, A. S. (2018). Prevalence of adverse childhood experiences from the 2011-2014 behavioral risk factor surveillance system in 23 states. JAMA Pediatrics, 172(11), 10381044. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.2537CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, P. W., Müller, L. E., Zastrow, A., Schmidinger, I., Bohus, M., Herpertz, S. C., & Bertsch, K. (2016). Heart rate variability in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder or borderline personality disorder: Relationship to early life maltreatment. Journal of Neural Transmission, 123(9), 11071118. doi: 10.1007/s00702-016-1584-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moody, A. T., & Lewis, J. A. (2019). Gendered racial microaggressions and traumatic stress symptoms among Black women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 43(2), 201214. doi: 10.1177/0361684319828288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nickerson, A., Hoffman, J., Schick, M., Schnyder, U., Bryant, R. A., & Morina, N. (2018). A longitudinal investigation of moral injury appraisals amongst treatment-seeking refugees. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 667. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00667CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nickerson, A., Schnyder, U., Bryant, R. A., Schick, M., Mueller, J., & Morina, N. (2015). Moral injury in traumatized refugees. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 84, 122123. doi: 10.1159/000369353CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, S., Allard, C., Browne, K., Capone, C., Davis, B., & Kubany, E. (2019). Trauma-informed guilt reduction therapy: Treating guilt and shame resulting from trauma and moral injury. Washington, DC: Elsevier Academic Press.Google Scholar
Plouffe, R. A., Nazarov, A., Forchuk, C. A., Gargala, D., Deda, E., Le, T., … Richardson, J. D. (2021). Impacts of morally distressing experiences on the mental health of Canadian health care workers during the COVID-19 pandemic. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 12(1). doi: 10.1080/20008198.2021.1984667CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabellino, D., D'Andrea, W., Siegle, G., Frewen, P. A., Minshew, R., Densmore, M., … Lanius, R. A. (2017). Neural correlates of heart rate variability in PTSD during sub- and supraliminal processing of trauma-related cues. Human Brain Mapping, 38(10), 48984907. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23702CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramshur, J. T. (2010). Design, evaluation, and application of Heart Rate Variability Analysis Software (HRVAS). Electronic Theses and Dissertations. 83. https://digitalcommons.memphis.edu/etd/83.Google Scholar
Ray, T. N., Hunsanger, J. A., Nagy, S. M., & Pickett, S. M. (2021). Potentially morally injurious events and depression symptoms among a trauma-exposed sample: Examining the roles of interpersonal needs and emotion dysregulation. Stress and Health: Journal of the International Society for the Investigation of Stress, 37(1), 151161. doi: 10.1002/smi.2981CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schartau, P. E. S., Dalgleish, T., & Dunn, B. D. (2009). Seeing the bigger picture: Training in perspective broadening reduces self-reported affect and psychophysiological response to distressing films and autobiographical memories. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 1527. doi: 10.1037/a0012906CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schauer, M., & Elbert, T. (2010). Dissociation following traumatic stress: Etiology and treatment. Journal of Psychology, 218(2), 109127. doi: 10.1027/0044-3409/a000018Google Scholar
Schmidt, U., Kaltwasser, S. F., & Wotjak, C. T. (2013). Biomarkers in posttraumatic stress disorder: Overview and implications for future research. Disease Markers, 35(1), 4354. doi: 10.1155/2013/835876CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, M., & Schwerdtfeger, A. (2020). Autonomic dysfunction in posttraumatic stress disorder indexed by heart rate variability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 50(12), 19371948. doi: 10.1017/S003329172000207X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, D., Phillips, R. D., Mulready, H. L., Zablonski, S. T., Turner, J. A., Turner, M. D., … Morey, R. A. (2019). Resting-state brain fluctuation and functional connectivity dissociate moral injury from posttraumatic stress disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 36(5), 442452. doi: 10.1002/da.22883CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomaes, K., Dorrepaal, E., Draijer, N., Jansma, E. P., Veltman, D. J., & van Balkom, A. J. (2014). Can pharmacological and psychological treatment change brain structure and function in PTSD? A systematic review. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 50, 115. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.11.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ursachi, G., Horodnic, I. A., & Zait, A. (2015). How reliable are measurement scales? External factors with indirect influence on reliability estimators. Procedia Economics and Finance, 20, 679686. doi: 10.1016/S2212-5671(15)00123-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woud, M. L., Kleim, B., & Cwik, J. C. (2019). Editorial for the special issue on negative appraisals in trauma: Current status and future directions for research. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 43, 139142. doi: 10.1007/s10608-018-09992-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woud, M. L., Verwoerd, J., & Krans, J. (2017). Modification of cognitive biases related to posttraumatic stress: A systematic review and research agenda. Clinical Psychology Review, 54, 8195. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(2), 197206. doi: 10.1086/651257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Study 1 and Study 2 sample characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2. Study 1 and Study 2 descriptive statistics

Figure 2

Table 3. Study 1 Pearson's, point biserial, and phi-coefficient correlations

Figure 3

Fig. 1. Scatterplot of hfHRV by moral injury exposure (MIE).

Figure 4

Fig. 2. Scatterplot of hfHRV by moral injury distress (MID).

Figure 5

Fig. 3. Simple mediation model.