Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T10:02:49.333Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The psychology of task management: The smaller tasks trap

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Zohar Rusou
Affiliation:
The Open University of Israel
Moty Amar
Affiliation:
Ono Academic College
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

When people are confronted with multiple tasks, how do they decide which task to do first? Normatively, priority should be given to the most efficient task (i.e., the task with the best cost/benefit ratio). However, we hypothesize that people consistently choose to address smaller (involving less work) tasks first, and continue to focus on smaller tasks, even when this strategy emerges as less efficient, a phenomenon we term the “smaller tasks trap”. We also hypothesize that the preference for the smaller tasks is negatively related to individual differences in the tendency for rational thinking. To test these hypotheses, we developed a novel paradigm consisting of an incentive-compatible task management game, in which participants are saddled with multiple tasks and have to decide how to handle them. The results lend weight to the smaller tasks trap and indicate that individual differences in rational thinking predict susceptibility to this trap. That is, participants low in rational thinking preferred to start with a smaller (vs. larger) task and focused more on the smaller tasks regardless of their efficiency. Consequently, their overall performance in the task management game was significantly lower. We discuss the theoretical and practical implications of these findings and suggest possible interventions that may help people improve their task management.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2020] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

In modern life, individuals are swamped with tasks. To cope with this overload, people must handle multiple tasks efficiently (Reference Neal, Ballard and VancouverNeal, Ballard & Vancouver, 2017). Nevertheless, a large body of evidence in behavioral economics indicates that people manage their tasks in suboptimal ways (e.g., Reference Ariely and WertenbrochAriely & Wertenbroch, 2002; Reference O’Donoghue and RabinO’Donoghue & Rabin, 2000; Reference Rabin, Fogel and Nutter-UphamRabin, Fogel & Nutter-Upham, 2011; Reference SteelSteel, 2007), and that this behavior has fundamental implications for both work settings and daily life (e.g., Reference Steel, Brothen and WambachSteel, Brothen & Wambach, 2001; Reference Tice and BaumeisterTice & Baumeister, 1997). Many of us are familiar with suboptimal task management. Take for instance researchers who reserve time to handle an important task (e.g., writing a research proposal for an important grant), which are essential for their career but instead end up wasting a great deal of valuable time on marginal less important tasks. These situations are not confined to academic settings, but are also prevalent in business, among managers and other workers (Reference De Meza, Irlenbusch and ReyniersDe Meza, Irlenbusch & Reyniers, 2008; Reference SteelSteel, 2007).

The current research examined the role of task size (smaller vs. larger) and individual differences in cognitive style (rational and intuitive) in shaping task management decisions. We juxtaposed normative (cost/benefit) considerations against the preference to avoid effort and prioritize smaller tasks over larger tasks. The discussion puts forward ways to help people prioritize tasks, by suggesting that different perspectives on effort evaluation may lead to differential behavioral patterns.

1.1 Normative considerations for prioritizing smaller tasks

When confronted with multiple tasks (or multiple goals), the normative approach is to rely on a cost/benefit analysis, and to schedule tasks according to their relative utility to maximize the overall expected utility (e.g., Reference von Neumann, Morgenstern and Kuhnvon Neumann, Morgenstern & Kuhn, 2007). For example, if the target is to maximize the monetary return within a given timeframe, a normative approach would be to start with the task that yields the highest monetary return per unit of time. When this task is completed, the next step would be to move to the task with the next highest ratio of return, and so on. In other words, individuals are expected to prioritize the tasks with the highest rewards and postpone others. In line with these assertions, descriptive models of multiple goal pursuit generally assume that people direct resources toward the goal with the highest expected utility (Reference Ballard, Vancouver and NealBallard, Vancouver & Neal, 2018; Reference Klein, Austin and CooperKlein, Austin & Cooper, 2008; Reference Kanfer, Chen and PritchardKanfer, Chen & Pritchard, 2008; Reference Neal, Ballard and VancouverNeal et al., 2017; Reference Schmidt and DeShonSchmidt & DeShon, 2007; Reference Shenhav, Musslick, Lieder, Kool, Griffiths, Cohen and BotvinickShenhav et al., 2017; Reference Vancouver, Gullekson, Morse and WarrenVancouver, Gullekson, Morse & Warren, 2014; Reference Vancouver, Weinhardt and SchmidtVancouver, Weinhardt & Schmidt, 2010).

Although the immediate cost/benefit ratio of a task is the obvious normative criterion for utility maximization in the short term, people may also employ strategies that are more sophisticated in order to increase utility over time. One approach may direct individuals to start with a task that yields a lower immediate benefit, but has some advantages to the overall benefit. Ample research lends credence to the benefits of starting with smaller tasks (proximal sub-goals). The main rationale is that smaller tasks enable the activation of thinking capacities (i.e., warm up), and lead to a higher level of efficiency before approaching more demanding tasks (Reference Anshel and WrisbergAnshel & Wrisberg, 1993; Reference Kahol, Satava, Ferrara and SmithKahol, Satava, Ferrara & Smith, 2009; Reference Shellock and PrenticeShellock & Prentice, 1985). Smaller tasks can serve as a vehicle for facilitating self-efficacy and motivation (e.g., Reference BanduraBandura, 1982, 1986; Reference Bandura and LockeBandura & Locke, 2003; Reference HullHull, 1932; Reference Kivetz, Urminsky and ZhengKivetz, Urminsky & Zheng, 2006; Reference Kozlowski and BellKozlowski & Bell, 2006; Reference Latham and SeijtsLatham & Seijts, 1999; Reference Manderlink and HarackiewiczManderlink & Harackiewicz, 1984; Reference Louro, Pieters and ZeelenbergLouro, Pieters & Zeelenberg, 2007; Reference Nunes and DrèzeNunes & Drèze, 2006; Reference SeijtsSeijts, 1998). In addition, smaller tasks can provide temporal feedback, which might encourage effective task learning, and consequently increase the likelihood of success (Reference Northcraft, Schmidt and AshfordNorthcraft, Schmidt & Ashford, 2011; Reference Schmidt and DolisSchmidt & Dolis, 2009). Finally, starting with smaller tasks might help minimize the number of pending tasks, and “clear the deck” so as to better concentrate on larger tasks. Pending tasks might be perceived as background noise or distractions, which increase stress and reduce the attention span (Reference BaronBaron, 1986), and hence, may impair performance on larger tasks that involve more information processing (Reference Speier, Vessey and ValacichSpeier, Vessey & Valacich, 2003).

These works suggest that in certain situations, a thorough normative consideration of task scheduling could encourage people to start with smaller tasks to enhance their overall performance. According to this approach, a rational agent would thus be expected to start with smaller tasks whenever it is more effective, compared to the other tasks at hand in terms of cost/benefit ratio, or when the smaller tasks help to increase utility in the long term. In both cases, rational agents would be expected to attend to their pending tasks in a way that would maximize their overall utility.

1.2 Irrational considerations for prioritizing smaller tasks

The heuristic and bias approach provides a wide range of examples in which limited cognitive capacities, intuitions and emotions lead individuals to rely on rules of thumb, which under certain conditions can lead to cognitive biases and suboptimal behavior (e.g., Reference ArielyAriely, 2008; Reference Latham and SeijtsKahneman & Tversky, 2013; Reference HseeTversky & Kahneman, 1974). Cognitive biases may also impair individuals’ task prioritization strategies. One putative antecedent that could encourage people to start with smaller tasks might be an attempt to avoid cognitive load. People treat cognitive effort as aversive and costly, and hence may endeavor to avoid or procrastinate what they perceived as effortful processing (Reference Allport, Clark and PettigrewAllport, Clark & Pettigrew, 1954; Reference Gigerenzer and GoldsteinGigerenzer & Goldstein, 1996; Reference SimonSimon, 1956). Studies utilizing a variety of methods have identified these types of behavioral tendencies that often lead to biases. For example, Reference Kool, McGuire, Rosen and BotvinickKool, McGuire, Rosen and Botvinick (2010) reported that participants preferred a less demanding course of action (operationalized as the lower likelihood of a task switch), even when they thought that their choice would result in a longer session. Similarly, Reference Westbrook, Kester and BraverWestbrook, Kester and Braver (2013) found that participants preferred to relinquish rewards in order to avoid a high-effort task. The central premise of these studies was that mental effort entails subjective costs that could be dissociated from objective task costs, as quantified by indirect measures such as “time on task” and actual error rates.

Even when people want to behave rationally and adhere to normative considerations, they may fail to make efficient use of internal and external cues to determine when to attend to a task (Reference Kool, McGuire, Rosen and BotvinickKool & Botvinick, 2013, 2014; Reference Rabin, Fogel and Nutter-UphamRabin et al., 2011). Recently, Reference Dunn and RiskoDunn and Risko (2016) suggested that individuals’ estimations of task effort are based on metacognition (Reference Dunn, Lutes and RiskoDunn, Lutes & Risko, 2016; Reference Dunn and RiskoDunn & Risko, 2016), and as such, are largely inferential and are sensitive to preconceived biases, beliefs, and intuitive theories (Reference KoriatKoriat, 2006). In a similar vein, Reference Gray, Sims, Fu and SchoellesGray, Sims, Fu and Schoelles, (2006) argued that given the wide range of possible variations in natural and artificial environments, individuals carry out actions in an interactive manner. Instead of choosing an optimal strategy to achieve their global goal, they rely on local cues from the environment, and choose the least effort micro-strategies to accomplish sub-goals. These researchers suggested that patterns of behavior chosen to optimize efficiency and effectiveness at the local level do not necessarily lead to optimal performance at the global level (Reference Fu and GrayFu & Gray, 2004; Reference Gray, Sims, Fu and SchoellesGray et al., 2006; Reference Gray and FuGray & Fu, 2004). Although Gray and colleagues referred to low order cognitive activities such as perceptual-motor activities and memory retrieval, the same logic may also apply to higher order cognitive processes, such as judgment and decision making. In particular, people who make recurring choices on which sub-goal to address may base their evaluations on information pertaining to each proximal local sub-goal (single choice) rather than on information relating to the global goal. This strategy is likely to focus individuals on achieving the proximal sub-goals instead of attaining the objectives of the global goal.

People are motivated not only to minimize the cognitive effort needed to achieve task objectives, but also to minimize the “time on task” (Reference Bogacz, Brown, Moehlis, Holmes and CohenBogacz, Brown, Moehlis, Holmes & Cohen, 2006; Reference Gray, Sims, Fu and SchoellesGray et al., 2006). Here as well, a “local” focus on the choices between a small and a large task may lead to a preference for smaller tasks over larger tasks, which require more time. In line with this notion, Reference Amar, Ariely, Ayal, Cryder and RickAmar, Ariely, Ayal, Cryder and Rick (2011) found that people saddled with multiple monetary debts are primarily motivated to pay off small debts first, regardless of their interest rate. This tendency was also evident in situations in which “closing the small debts first” led to sub-optimal performance in reducing the total debt. Achieving sub goals may provide an illusion of control and a sense of tangible progress toward the goal (e.g., Reference HullHull, 1932; Reference Kivetz, Urminsky and ZhengKivetz et al., 2006; Reference Nunes and DrèzeNunes & Drèze, 2006), but may actually diminish people’s ability to pursue superordinate goals (e.g., Reference Amir and ArielyAmir & Ariely, 2008; Reference Fishbach and DharFishbach & Dhar, 2005; Reference Fishbach, Dhar and ZhangFishbach, Dhar & Zhang, 2006; Reference Kool and BotvinickHeath, Larrick & Wu, 1999).

These studies suggest that when pursuing multiple goals, flawed evaluations of task information might yield a sub-optimal preference for smaller tasks. In particular, focusing on the local (rather than on the global) level of the goal might foster a tendency to start with the smaller tasks and stick with them, even when this strategy turns out to be less rewarding than prioritizing the larger tasks.

1.3 Individual differences in rational thinking and the “smaller tasks trap”

A large body of research has shown that individual differences in thinking style measured by the Rational Experiential Inventory (REI) (Reference Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj and HeierEpstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj & Heier, 1996) have a crucial effect on choice behavior (Bruine de Reference Bruine de Bruin, Parker and FischhoffBruin, Parker & Fischhoff, 2007, 2012; Reference Lauriola, Panno, Levin and LejuezLauriola, Panno, Levin & Lejuez, 2014; Reference Soane and NicholsonSoane & Nicholson, 2008; Reference Yechiam, Busemeyer, Stout and BecharaYechiam, Busemeyer Stout & Bechara, 2005; Reference ZakayZakay, 1990), judgmental biases (e.g., Reference Ayal, Hochman and ZakayAyal, Hochman & Zakay, 2011; Reference Ayal, Rusou, Zakay and HochmanAyal, Rusou, Zakay & Hochman, 2015) and task management (Reference Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein and JarvisCacioppo, Petty, Feinstein & Jarvis, 1996; Reference Kool, McGuire, Rosen and BotvinickKool et al., 2010; Reference Westbrook, Kester and BraverWestbrook et al., 2013). Specifically, research has indicated that the value of cognitive effort is likely to vary across individuals (Reference Kool, McGuire, Rosen and BotvinickKool et al., 2010; Reference Westbrook, Kester and BraverWestbrook et al., 2013). Individuals high in the “need for cognition” personality trait are more likely to engage in effortful information-processing activities, while individuals low in “need for cognition” are more likely to avoid effort (Reference Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein and JarvisCacioppo et al., 1996; Reference Kool, McGuire, Rosen and BotvinickKool et al., 2010; Reference Westbrook, Kester and BraverWestbrook et al., 2013), and are less task oriented (Reference Witkin and GoodenoughWitkin & Goodenough, 1977).

Findings also show that individuals low in rational thinking tend to rely more on heuristics and simplifying strategies that can lead to biased decision making (e.g., Reference Ayal, Zakay and HochmanAyal, Zakay & Hochman, 2012; Reference Banks and OldfieldBanks & Oldfield, 2007; Reference Pacini and EpsteinPacini & Epstein, 1999), while people with a higher tendency for rational thinking are more likely to seek diagnostic information (Reference Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein and JarvisCacioppo et al., 1996). These differences in information seeking and processing might lead to differences in reliance on a local vs. a global perspective when evaluating information for task prioritization. Reliance on simplifying strategies may encourage a focus on local information cues, whereas a tendency to seek information may draw attention to the global picture. Importantly, however, findings consistently indicate no statistical association between the Rational Thinking Scale and the Experiential Thinking Scale of the REI (e.g., Reference Ayal, Zakay and HochmanAyal et al., 2012; Reference Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj and HeierEpstein et al., 1996; Reference Pacini and EpsteinPacini & Epstein, 1999). In fact, in contrast to the rational thinking style, the experiential (intuitive) thinking style was not found to be a good predictor of biased decisions (for a review see Reference Ayal, Zakay and HochmanAyal et al., 2012; Reference Ayal, Rusou, Zakay and HochmanAyal et al., 2015).

1.4 The current research

Taken together, the literature reviewed here suggests that when managing multiple tasks, individuals, and in particular those scoring low in rational thinking may be more likely to prioritize smaller tasks over the larger tasks. Yet, it remains unclear whether, in environments that involve repeated task sequencing, this choice pattern would persist even when approaching the smaller (vs. larger) tasks yields lower benefit. To better understand the considerations driving tasks’ prioritization we developed a novel paradigm that consists of an incentive-compatible task management game for the laboratory.

1.4.1 Overview of the task management game

The task management game simulated a real-life multiple-tasks environment in a real estate company. Participants were presented with a work environment consisting of 30 tasks, each of which required the retrieval of information from the paper version of a price-list for apartments (see Appendix A). Participants were required to make repeated task scheduling choices within a fixed time interval of 15 minutes. During this timeframe, participants could earn points to enter lotteries for shopping coupons. The number of points earned was contingent on the number and type of task completed. The more points earned (relative to the other participants), the more lotteries they could take part in (see detailed information in Appendix B). Since participants were informed that they could not complete all the tasks within the allotted timeframe, their ultimate goal was to schedule the tasks in a way that would maximize the overall number of points earned.

The tasks varied only in size (number of data items to be retrieved) and profit (number of points awarded per unit of work). All other variables were kept constant. The amount of information retrieved in each task type, and the number of points awarded were designed to pit normative (cost-benefit) considerations against the smaller tasks trap. Fifteen out of the thirty tasks required the retrieval of 3 pieces of data, and were awarded 1 point (the smaller tasks) and fifteen tasks required the retrieval of 9 pieces of data, and were awarded 4 points (the larger tasks). Deliberately, the larger tasks were more taxing, but also more profitable than the smaller tasks. A single larger task in our game required more units of work (the retrieval of 9 vs. 3 data items) and more time than a single smaller task, and therefore involved more effort. However, a single larger task yielded higher benefits than three smaller tasks (4 points instead of 3 points in total), although both required the same effort (the retrieval of 9 pieces of data). Therefore, the larger tasks resulted in a higher profit per unit of work. A pilot study confirmed that only some of the presented tasks could be completed within the allotted time frame of 15 minutes (the 10 participants in the pilot completed 4–15 smaller tasks or 1–6 larger tasks). Thus, normative considerations would dictate focusing on the larger tasks, whereas the “smaller tasks trap” would direct participants to focus more on the smaller tasks.

The game was carried out in an interactive mode. On each trial, the list of 30 apartments (30 tasks: 15 smaller tasks and 15 larger tasks) was presented (Figure 1A). Each task was displayed on a separate line, in a random order. The following information was displayed: task type (basic or extended) apartment address (city and street) and number of rooms, and the number of points assigned to the task (1 or 4). In order to facilitate the distinction between the two categories (small vs. large), different colors were used for the display of each category. The specific colors were counterbalanced across participants.

Figure 1: Task management game screens: (A) List of apartments for information retrieval, (B) Apartment data for the smaller task, (C) Apartment data for the larger task.

Participants were required to choose an apartment from the list, and retrieve data related to its pricing (e.g., the basic price of the apartment and the effect of various factors such as building age, apartment type, and parking facilities on the price). After a specific apartment had been chosen, a screen that contained the detailed information on that apartment was presented. The amount and nature of this information was contingent on the task category (smaller or larger, see Figure 1B and 1C). The game forced the participants to correct all mistaken retrieval errors before completing a task. When the participant completed the data retrieval for an apartment, or chose to exit the evaluation, this screen was closed, and the list of apartments for evaluation immediately appeared again. Apartments for which the data had already been retrieved were marked and became non-selectable.

1.4.2 Hypotheses and General Design

H1. Participants will tend to fall into the smaller task trap. That is, they will address smaller tasks first resulting in suboptimal total performance compared to participants who address larger tasks first

H2. Participants scoring low in rational thinking on the REI should be more prone to the smaller task trap than participants high in rational thinking. However, experiential (intuitive) thinking style should not affect task prioritization or total performance.

Two studies were conducted to test these hypotheses. Study 1 examined (1) whether participants were prone to the small tasks trap bias, and (2) whether the impact of starting with a smaller or a larger task on overall performance. Study 2 examined the relationship between the smaller task trap and individual differences in thinking style.

2 Study 1

2.1 Method

2.1.1 Participants

The sample was composed of 111 psychology undergraduate students from a university in central Israel, aged 20–37 (88 females, M= 25.24, SD=3.72) who participated in the study as part of their academic requirements. Since we lacked a clear estimation of the expected effect size, we did not conduct an a- priori power analysis, but instead aimed to recruit as many participants as possible during the course of a full academic semester.

2.1.2 Design

The study was made up of 3 conditions: (1) the “smaller tasks first” condition, in which participants were instructed to start with smaller tasks, (2) The “larger tasks first” condition, in which participants were instructed to start with larger tasks, and (3) the “free choice condition” in which participants were free to choose which tasks to perform. The “smaller tasks first” and the “larger tasks first” conditions examined the impact of (manipulated) prioritizing smaller vs. larger tasks on the total number of points earned. The free choice condition aimed at examining actual task management behavior.

2.1.3 Procedure

The study was described as an experiment on task management. First, participants were given a detailed explanation on the tasks and on the task management game, as well as an explanation on how to find information on the price list. A preliminary session was administered to make sure the participants had understood the explanation. In this preliminary session participants were given the address of a specific apartment and were asked to locate apartment’s information in the price list. Then, participants were told that they would perform the tasks for a fixed period of 15 minutes, and that during that time they would be able to complete several tasks but not all of the tasks. They were explicitly informed that their goal was to maximize the number of points they earned, which would then allow them to participate in lotteries for shopping coupons, and that the number and types of lotteries were contingent on performance (the number of points accumulated), relative to the other participants.

Then, the participants were randomly assigned to one of three experimental conditions. Participants in the “smaller/larger tasks first” conditions were instructed to start with smaller/larger tasks, respectively. In both these conditions, they were told that “afterward they were free to choose which tasks to address”. Participants in the “free choice condition” were told that they should “feel free to choose which tasks to address”. Then, participants in all three conditions engaged in the tasks for 15 minutes.

2.2 Results and Discussion

The number of tasks addressed, the number items retrieved and the number of points earned by the participants in each of the three experimental conditions is presented in Table 1.

Table 1: Number of tasks addressed, number of items retrieved and number of points earned by the participants in each of the three experimental conditions in Study 1. (SD in parentheses).

First, in order to validate the task size manipulation, we compared the average amount of time it took to address a single task in the “smaller tasks first” vs. “the larger tasks first” conditions. The average amount of time was computed for each participant by dividing the session length (900 seconds) by the number of tasks completed. A one-way ANOVA yielded a significant effect for experimental condition (F[2,108]=8.12, p=.001, η2=0.13). A planned contrast analysis confirmed that a single larger task required more time than a single smaller task. The mean duration of a task in the “larger tasks first” condition (M= 364.407 seconds, SD=288.55) was significantly longer than the mean duration of a task in the “smaller tasks first” condition (M= 178.79, SD=179.61, t(108)=3.83, p=.000, d=.77).

Next, to test the effect of experimental condition on the number of items retrieved and on the number of points earned, we ran two separate one-way ANOVAs. The analyses yielded a significant effect for experimental condition on the number of points earned (F[2,108]=8.73, p<.001, η2=0.14), and a marginally significant effect for experimental condition on the number of items retrieved (F[2,108]=2.55, p=.083, η2 =0.05).

2.2.1 Impact of starting with smaller vs. larger tasks

To test whether it was more beneficial to start with the smaller or the larger tasks, we conducted a planned contrast analysis on the number of items retrieved and the number of points earned to compare the two conditions in which participants were explicitly directed to selectively start with a specific task type. The mean number of items retrieved in the “larger task first” condition was significantly higher than the mean number of items retrieved in the “smaller tasks first” condition (t(108)=2.23, p=0.03, d=0.51). Similarly, the mean number of points earned in the “larger task first” condition was significantly higher than the mean number of points earned in the “smaller tasks first” condition (t(108)=4.18, p<0.001, d=0.97). Hence, it was more beneficial for participants to start with the larger tasks, both in terms of the number of items retrieved and in terms of the number of points earned.

2.2.2 The “free choice” condition

Participants in the free choice condition were not given specific instructions. Hence, they could freely decide to prioritize either smaller or larger tasks. Planned contrasts showed that overall, the number of points earned in the free choice condition was significantly higher than the number of points in the “smaller tasks first” condition (t(108)=2.02, p=0.046, d=0.55) but significantly lower than in the “larger task first” condition (t(108)=2.32, p=0.02, d=0.47). The number of items retrieved in the free choice condition was higher than in the “smaller tasks first” condition and smaller than in the “larger task first” condition, but the differences between the free choice condition and the other two conditions did not reach significance.

Next, to probe these differences, we compared the performance of participants who chose to start with a smaller task and participants who chose to start with a larger task. Table 2 presents the total performance as a function of the choice of which task to do first in the free choice condition.

Table 2: Tasks’ handling patterns and performance (items retrieved and points earned) in the free choice condition in Study 1, according to the first type of task addressed. (SD in parentheses.)

Taken together, although it was clearly more profitable to focus on the larger tasks, 54% of participants (22 out of 41) in the free choice condition chose to start with a smaller task (χ2(1, n=42) = 0.22, n.s.). Importantly, the participants who started with a smaller task also tended to continue throughout the experiment with smaller tasks to a greater extent than participants who started with a larger task (t(39)=6.18, p<.001, d =1.93, 95% CI=[36.45,71.96]), and earned fewer points (t(39)=2.03, p=.05, d =.64, 95% CI=[−7.47,−.005]). No significant difference was found for the number of items retrieved (t(39)=1.29, p=.20). The correlation between the percetage of smaller tasks and the number of points earned was negative and significant (r=−.42, p=.007), but the correlation between the percentage of the smaller tasks and the number of items retrieved did not reach significance (r=−.26, p=.11).

This pattern of smaller tasks trap behavior emerged although the relative benefit of the tasks was highlighted by displaying the number of points associated with each task. Importantly, participants exhibited this behavior even though they were given a detailed explanation of the tasks and even though it was clear to them that both tasks involved the same type of activity (retrieving data from a price-list for apartments), and that they would perform the task for a fixed time interval.

However, the mechanism underlying the choices made in the free choice condition remains unclear. Specifically, although the larger tasks were more beneficial, only 46% of the participants in the free choice condition have started with a larger task, whereas 54% started with a smaller task. One possible explanation for this pattern, is that this behavior reflected a random selection of the first task. However, we assumed that the pattern of task selection was not random, but rather reflected systematic individual differences in the evaluation and prioritization of the smaller vs. larger tasks. Specifically, we assumed that individual differences in cognitive thinking style would predict the susceptibility to the smaller tasks trap. People who score low in rational thinking style would be more likely to adopt a local perspective and prefer to address the smaller tasks first, while people scoring high in rational thinking style would be more likely to adopt a global perspective and prefer the larger and more profitable tasks. That is, participants low in rational thinking style would be more prone to the smaller tasks trap. This assumption was examined in Study 2.

3 Study 2

Study 2 aimed at further exploring the smaller tasks trap by examining its relationship to individual differences in thinking style. In addition, the experiment served as a direct replication of the free choice condition in Study 1. In line with our theoretical claim that the smaller tasks trap is the outcome of irrational considerations, we predicted a negative relationship between the tendency to think rationally and the preference for the smaller (rather than the larger) tasks. Specifically, we hypothesized that participants who score higher on the rational thinking scale would be less susceptible to the smaller tasks trap than those who score lower on the rational thinking scale, and consequently would address larger tasks more and earn more points. Based on previous findings (Reference Ayal, Zakay and HochmanAyal et al., 2012, Reference Ayal, Rusou, Zakay and Hochman2015), we also predicted that differences on the experiential (intuitive) thinking scale would not be related to task management strategies.

3.1 Method

3.1.1 Participants

Sample size was determined a-priori, using G*Power (Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner and LangFaul, Erdfelder, Lang & Buchner, 2009). Our calculation relied on the effect size of the total points earned in the ’free choice’ condition (Study 1), with an α of .05 (one-tailed) and a power of .80. This calculation yielded a minimum target sample size of 120 participants. Hence, the actual sample was composed of 136 undergraduate students (83 females) from a private college in Israel, aged 18–46, M= 25.92, SD=4.93 who participated in the experiment as part of their academic requirements.

3.1.2 Materials

The 24-item Rational Experiential Inventory (REI) questionnaire (Reference Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj and HeierEpstein et al., 1996; Reference Pacini and EpsteinPacini & Epstein, 1999). The REI is a self-report inventory that assesses individuals’ tendencies to rely on rational or intuitive considerations. It consists of two unipolar scales (12 items each). One scale measure rational information processing style (e.g., “I have a logical mind”). The other scale measures intuitive processing style (e.g., “When it comes to trusting people, I can usually rely on my gut feelings”). Participants are required to state how true each statement is for them, on a scale from 1 (Definitely False) to 5 (Definitely True). As predicted by the dual system approach, the internal reliability of the REI questionnaire was high for both the rational (Cronbach’s α =0.862) and the intuitive (Cronbach’s α =0.871) thinking scales. The correlation between the rational and the intuitive scales was small and negligible (r =.085, p=.329). This pattern is consistent with previous findings of this inventory (Reference Ayal, Hochman and ZakayAyal et al., 2011; Reference Pacini and EpsteinPacini & Epstein, 1999).

3.1.3 Procedure

The experimental procedure was similar to the free choice condition in Experiment 1 with one additional phase; namely, after completing the task management game, participants were administered the 24-item Rational Experiential Inventory (REI) questionnaire (Reference Pacini and EpsteinPacini & Epstein, 1999).

3.2 Results and Discussion

The main patterns that emerged in Study 2 closely paralleled the patterns in the free choice condition in Study 1 (Table 3).

Table 3: Tasks’ handling patterns and performance (items retrieved and points earnt) in Study 2, as a function of first type of task addressed. (SD in parentheses.)

Sixty-five percent of the participants (89 out of 136) in Study 2 chose to start with a smaller task. This choice ratio significantly deviated from chance (χ2(1, n=136)=12.97, p=.000). Participants who started with a smaller task tended to continue with smaller tasks throughout the experiment to a greater extent than participants who started with a larger task (t(134)=13.72, p=0.000, CI-Lower=53.24, CI-Upper=71.18, d=2.45), retrieved fewer items (t(134)=2.27, p=0.025, CI-Lower=.68, CI-Upper= 9.94, d=0.41) and earned fewer points (t(134) = 3.66, p=0.000, CI-Lower=1.65, CI-Upper=5.55, d=0.66).

In order to explore the relationships between participants’ thinking style and the tendency to fall into the smaller tasks trap, we calculated the correlations between rational/intuitive thinking styles, the percentage of smaller tasks addressed by the participants, number of items retrieved, and number of points earned. The correlation matrix is presented in Table 4.

Table 4: The correlations between rational and intuitive thinking styles (REI), the percentage of smaller tasks addressed, number of items retrieved and number of points earned by participants. (**, p<.01.)

In line with previous research (e.g., Reference Ayal, Hochman and ZakayAyal et al., 2011, Reference Ayal, Rusou, Zakay and Hochman2015), participants’ strategies and performance were related to their rational thinking style, but not to their intuitive thinking style, and there was no interaction between the rational thinking and intuitive thinking styles.

Based on the above findings, in the following analyses we focused exclusively on the rational scale, and ran two separate modifications of the Sobel tests using bootstrapping (5000 samples, implemented with the PROCESS macro Version 3 from Reference HayesHayes, 2017), to test mediation models with the rational thinking as an independent variable, the percentage of small tasks as a mediator and number of items retrieved and total points and as the dependent variables.

The mediation analysis indicated that the task management strategy, specifically the percentage of smaller tasks, mediated the effect of Rational Thinking on both the number of items retrieved and the number of points earned. The indirect effect of Rational Thinking on both the number of items retrieved and the number of points earned, through the smaller task percentage, was positive and significant (path a×b=−1.41, z=−2.16, p<.05; a×b=−.99, z=2.64, p<.01); respectively). Rational Thinking had a significant effect on the smaller task percentage (a=14.11, t=−2.85, p<.01). Both the number of items retrieved and the number of points, in turn, increased as a function of smaller task percentage (with Rational Thinking in the model; b=−.10, t=−3.77, p<.00; b=.07, t=−6.01, p<.00; respectively). Further, the total effect of Rational Thinking on both the number of items retrieved and the number of points earned was positive and significant (c=6.86, t=4.22, p<.01; c=3.08, t=4.39, p<.01, respectively). When the indirect effect was accounted for, the direct effect remained significant for both the number of items retrieved and the number of points earned (c’=5.41, t=3.39, p=.0009; c’=2.15, t=3.35, p=.001; respectively). Further, a bootstrap analysis with 5,000 resamples revealed that the 95% confidence intervals for the significant indirect effect excluded zero for both the number of items retrieved and the number of points earned (from .36 to 2.87; from .29 to 1.66; respectively).

These results are consistent with the mediational hypothesis. Thus overall, these results show that the pattern of task selection was not random, but rather reflected systematic individual differences in the evaluation and prioritization of tasks. Low rational participants tended to address the smaller tasks first, more than high rational participants, and handled more smaller tasks throughout the experiment. Consequently, their overall performance was hampered. This pattern suggests that they were more susceptible to the “smaller task trap” than the high rational participants.

4 General Discussion

The simultaneous pursuit of multiple tasks appears to be the norm in everyday life (Reference Louro, Pieters and ZeelenbergLouro et al., 2007) and the implications of scheduling tasks effectively, or failing to do so, are considerable. The current research examined the role of task size (smaller vs. larger), and individual differences in cognitive style (lower vs. higher rationality), in shaping task management strategies. We developed a novel paradigm in which participants faced multiple tasks and had to make repeated choices of which task to address. Our paradigm juxtaposed normative (cost/benefit) considerations against the suboptimal preference for smaller tasks.

The results of Study 1 showed that the overall performance of the participants who were instructed to start with the larger tasks were much better than the overall performance of the participants who were instructed to start with smaller tasks, both in terms of the number of items retrieved and the number of points earned. However, 54% of the participants in the free choice condition in Study 1 and 65% of the participants in Study 2 selected to start with and continued to address more smaller tasks, and thus were prone to the “smaller tasks trap” bias. In fact, when given free choice, the majority of the participants cued themselves into the bias, and intuitively behaved more similarly to participants who were directed to start with a smaller task.

The findings clearly indicate that the preference for the smaller tasks was not the outcome of comprehensive normative (rational) considerations and cannot be explained by random exploration. Support for this notion comes from several independent patterns that emerged in the data. First, normative considerations are expected to lead to the maximization of overall utility. In contrast, our data clearly show that starting with a smaller task impaired performance rather than improved it. Second, participants who started with a smaller task tended to persevere with the smaller tasks throughout the experiment, and ended up with a much higher percentage of smaller tasks, compared to the participants who started with a larger task. Hence, starting with a smaller task does not appear to be the outcome of a planned strategic move aimed at increasing the total utility in the long run (e.g., by warming up and enhancing the ability to address the larger and more beneficial tasks). Finally, if starting with a smaller task were the outcome of normative considerations, we would expect that participants higher in rational thinking would tend to start with a smaller task much more often than participants low in rational thinking. However, the findings of study 2 exhibited the opposite trend. Participants lower in rational thinking presented a stronger bias toward the smaller tasks. They started and persevered on the smaller tasks more than participants high in rational thinking, and consequently earned fewer points. That is, the positive correlation between rational thinking and the percentage of larger tasks addressed suggests that the patterns of task selection were not random, but rather reflected systematic differences in thinking style.

It remains unclear why the low-rational thinkers persevered on the smaller (rather than on the larger) tasks, even though the two courses of action involved the same activity and this choice disrupted their overall performance. In the following sections, we discuss several motivational factors that may account for this strategy and speculate on the processes that could underlie this decision.

4.1 Motivational factors for “the smaller tasks trap”

The picture that emerges from the data may have been driven by several different motivational factors. Two potential sources for the mishandling of pending tasks are demand avoidance (e.g., Reference Gold, Kool, Botvinick, Hubzin, August and WaltzGold, Kool, Botvinick, Hubzin, August & Waltz, 2015; Reference Kool, McGuire, Rosen and BotvinickKool et al, 2010; Reference Westbrook and BraverWestbrook & Braver, 2015) and a desire to minimize the time needed to achieve task objectives (Reference Bogacz, Brown, Moehlis, Holmes and CohenBogacz et al., 2006; Reference Gray, Sims, Fu and SchoellesGray et al., 2004, 2006). The patterns of behavior, that emerged in the current study are consistent with previous findings showing that when individuals are given a choice, they willingly avoid lines of action associated with more effort or more time. In the current studies, participants made repeated choices between smaller and larger tasks. Since each smaller task involved fewer units of work, and required less time, it is reasonable to assume that the smaller tasks were perceived as less effortful than the larger tasks (Reference Dunn, Inzlicht and RiskoDunn, Inzlicht & Risko, 2019). The negative correlation between individual differences in the tendency for rational thinking and the tendency to prioritize the smaller tasks is consistent with the well-established association in the literature between individual differences on “need for cognition” trait and demand avoidance (e.g., Reference Kool, McGuire, Rosen and BotvinickKool et al., 2010; Reference Westbrook, Kester and BraverWestbrook et al., 2013).

Another potential source for the relationship between individual differences in rational thinking style and the tendency to handle smaller (rather than larger) tasks might be a greater tendency of low rational thinkers to use the number of tasks addressed as a proxy for efficient performance (Reference Amar, Ariely, Ayal, Cryder and RickAmar et al., 2011). Difficult-to-evaluate attributes tend to receive less weight in decision-making and may be replaced by proxies (Reference Denes-Raj and EpsteinDenes-Raj & Epstein 1994; Reference Gigerenzer and HoffrageGigerenzer & Hoffrage, 1995; Reference HseeHsee, 1996; Reference Pacini and EpsteinPacini & Epstein 1999). People low in rational thinking are more predisposed to employing heuristics and adopting simplifying strategies than people high in rational thinking (Reference Ayal, Hochman and ZakayAyal et al., 2011, Reference Ayal, Rusou, Zakay and Hochman2015). Since the cost/benefit ratio for each specific task (1/3 vs. 4/9) may have been hard to evaluate, participants low in rational thinking may simply have endeavored to count tasks and focused on completing quantitatively more tasks, rather than resolve the cost/benefit tradeoff of the smaller vs. larger tasks (Reference Amar, Ariely, Ayal, Cryder and RickAmar et al., 2011). This could account for the higher number of smaller tasks, which could give a semblance of tangible progress and control.

4.2 Some insights into the mechanism governing task selection

Both the demand avoidance account and time minimization account suggest that the cost of engaging in the larger tasks was evaluated as higher than the cost of engaging in the smaller tasks. This difference in evaluation seems reasonable from a local perspective centered on a single proximal choice of a particular (smaller vs. larger) task. A single larger task in the game involved more units of work than a single smaller task (the retrieval of 9 vs. 3 pieces of data), and required more time. Hence, the larger tasks may have been evaluated as more taxing, and this evaluation may have guided the choice to do smaller tasks. However, from a global perspective that focuses on the entire session all the tasks involved the same type of activity (retrieving data), and the participants knew that they would perform the tasks for a fixed period of time. Thus, there is no reason to evaluate a strategy of engaging in the larger tasks as more taxing than a strategy of engaging in the smaller tasks. Thus, the current finding strengthens and extends Reference Gray, Sims, Fu and SchoellesGray’s et al. (2004, 2006) claim that irrational individuals tend to base their action selections on local optimization and demonstrate suboptimal performance at the global level (e.g., Reference Amar, Ariely, Ayal, Cryder and RickAmar et al., 2011; Reference Dunn and RiskoDunn & Risko, 2016; Reference Gray, Sims, Fu and SchoellesGray et al., 2004, 2006). The data here imply that the same logic as suggested by Gray for low order cognition might also apply to higher order cognitive activities. Further investigation of the local and the global perspectives for the evaluation of task effort might provide a window into the mechanism underlying task prioritization.

4.3 Limitations and future research

The distinction between the local and the global perspectives of task prioritization suggests that the mechanisms governing task management are largely based on information at the sub-goal (task) level. However, we cannot draw conclusive conclusions as to the differential contribution of each attribute of the tasks to these evaluations, or as to the exact weight ascribed to each attribute. The (smaller and larger) tasks in the current study varied on a number of dimensions, which are related to both the costs incurred by the task and the benefit entailed. The data imply that the participants (at least those low in rationality) focused on the cost information much more than on the benefit information, since a focus on the benefit was expected to yield exactly the opposite pattern of behavior. Nevertheless, the tasks we used included at least two different dimensions of cost: (1) set size, and (2) time demands (duration), which are related to different explanations for the observed behavior (e.g., demand avoidance vs. minimization of time on task). Although Reference Kool, McGuire, Rosen and BotvinickKool et al. (2010) reported that people preferred a less demanding course of action even when it involved an extended session length (and so ruled out the time minimization account), it is important to further explore this question in follow-up studies (for instance see Reference Dunn, Inzlicht and RiskoDunn et al., 2019).

Another related question for further investigation is what motivated the high rational thinkers in the current studies to attend to the larger tasks. The data revealed that, among the high rational thinking participants, 64% chose to start with a larger task and overall 61% of their tasks were large. Here also, we cannot pinpoint which specific task attributes guided their choices so that several explanations may apply. For example, individuals high in rational thinking are more likely to engage in effortful information-processing activity, and seek effort rather than avoid it (Reference Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein and JarvisCacioppo et al., 1996; Reference Inzlicht, Shenhav and OlivolaInzlicht, Shenhav & Olivola, 2018; Reference Kool, McGuire, Rosen and BotvinickKool et al., 2010; Reference Sandra and OttoSandra & Otto, 2018; Reference Westbrook, Kester and BraverWestbrook et al., 2013). Another possible explanation is that people high in rational thinking have better numeracy abilities (Reference PetersPeters, 2012; Reference Peters, Västfjäll, Slovic, Mertz, Mazzocco and DickertPeters, Västfjäll, Slovic, Mertz, Mazzocco & Dickert 2006) and are more inclined to adhere to the normative guidelines of cost/benefit considerations (e.g., Reference Ayal, Hochman and ZakayAyal, et al., 2011, 2015), and hence, may have prioritized the larger tasks, which are more effective in maximizing the total benefit than the smaller tasks. This explanation is challenged by recent finding that large reward incentives decrease the amount of effort exerted by high-rational individuals (Reference Sandra and OttoSandra & Otto, 2018). A third explanation is that people high in rational thinking are more likely to rely on a global (rather than on a local) perspective of the goal. Ample evidence shows that individuals high in need for cognition seek more information on complex issues, and on a wide range of tasks than individuals low in need for cognition (Reference Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein and JarvisCacioppo et al., 1996). Consequently, these individuals might be more likely to employ a wider perspective for task management. In future research, it would be worthwhile manipulating the cost benefit ratio and the local-global dissociation in task efficiency to further explore these possible drivers.

Finally, our design involved only a behavioral measure of actual task choice. A direct measure of effort estimations might shed more light on the process underlying these choices. One possibility would be to use self-reports. Previous studies have found an association between self- reports of the subjective cost of effort and demand avoidance behavior (Reference Westbrook, Kester and BraverWestbrook et al., 2013). Yet, other studies showed that assessment of preferences by effort discounting better predicted individual differences in need for cognition than self-reports of subjective effort evaluations (Reference Westbrook, Kester and BraverWestbrook et al., 2013).

It is important to note that although our game simulated a real-life environment of multiple tasks, it constitutes a simplified version of reality. The paradigm was designed to capture specific components of task management to keep the data tractable and interpretable. Several complexities present outside the lab were not reflected in the game (e.g., a more diverse set of tasks). In addition, the cost/ benefit of the tasks was explicitly displayed. The fact that our participants fell prey to the smaller tasks trap even in this transparent setting implies that task management in the more complex and uncertain real world would be even less optimal.

4.4 Conclusion

The current study compared rational (cost/benefit) considerations to the initial decision to address smaller task first. We examined the role of task size (smaller vs. larger), and individual differences in cognitive style (rational and intuitive), in shaping task management behavior. The findings suggest that the “smaller tasks trap” can lead to the completion of local sub-goals, and produce a sense of tangible progress, but impede achieving the larger more beneficial goal.

Appendix A — Structure of the Price List for Apartments in Israel

The paper version of the price list was taken from a publication by the Levi Izchak group published in Feb. 2016, which is considered the most popular price list in Israel. This price list is used both by real estate experts and by lay people to buy or sell apartments.

The first part of the price list is the street index. This section indicates the location of the apartments. All the streets in Israel are listed by city (town). Cities and towns are presented in alphabetical order. The city’s zip code appears opposite its name. Under each city name, all the streets of the city are presented in alphabetical order. Opposite each street name, the zip-code for its neighborhood is displayed, as is the page number on the price list (where information pertaining to it can be found) (see Figure 2).

Figure 2: Structure of the street index.

The second part of the price list contains comparative pricing information for apartments. This part is broken down by regions of Israel (from north to south). For each region, the data are presented by city (in alphabetical order), then neighborhood (by neighborhood zip-code) and street name (in alphabetical order). For each street, each row lists the street name and a rough estimate of the prices of apartments (in thousands of Israeli shekels), displayed according to the number of rooms in the apartment (see Figure 3).

Figure 3: The names of streets in a specific neighborhood in the city of Tel Aviv. Street names are listed in alphabetical order, with rough estimates of apartment prices. For example, a rough estimate for a 3-room apartment on Alexander St. is 1,670,000 Israeli shekels.

For each neighborhood, other parameters influencing the price evaluation (e.g., age of the building, elevators, balconies, etc.) are listed below the list of street names (and the rough estimate of prices). Next to each parameter, the difference in price is displayed (for example: “Age of building under 10 years +10%”).

Appendix B — Structure of the Lotteries

The number and types of lotteries that each participant was qualified to participate in were contingent on performance (the number of points accumulated), relative to the other participants. Table 5 presents the lotteries and the criteria for qualification.

Table 5: Shopping voucher lotteries and the qualification criteria. Note that participants could qualify for multiple lotteries.

References

Allport, G. W., Clark, K., & Pettigrew, T. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books.Google Scholar
Amar, M., Ariely, D., Ayal, S., Cryder, C. E., & Rick, S. I. (2011). Winning the battle but losing the war: The psychology of debt management. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(SPL), S38S50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amir, O., & Ariely, D. (2008). Resting on laurels: the effects of discrete progress markers as subgoals on task performance and preferences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(5), 11581171.Google ScholarPubMed
Anshel, M. H., & Wrisberg, C. A. (1993). Reducing warm-up decrement in the performance of the tennis serve. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 15(3), 290303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariely, D. (2008). Predictably irrational (p. 20). New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Ariely, D., & Wertenbroch, K. (2002). Procrastination, deadlines, and performance: Self-control by precommitment. Psychological Science, 13(3), 219224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayal, S., Hochman, G., & Zakay, D. (2011). Two sides of the same coin: Information processing style and reverse biases. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(4), 295306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ayal, S., Rusou, Z., Zakay, D., & Hochman, G. (2015). Determinants of judgment and decision making quality: the interplay between information processing style and situational factors. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1088.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayal, S., Zakay, D., & Hochman, G. (2012). Deliberative adjustments of intuitive anchors: the case of diversification behavior. Synthese, 189(1), 131145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ballard, T., Vancouver, J. B., & Neal, A. (2018). On the pursuit of multiple goals with different deadlines. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(11), 12421264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Bandura, A., & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(1), 8799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banks, J., & Oldfield, Z. (2007). Understanding pensions: Cognitive function, numerical ability and retirement saving. Fiscal Studies, 28(2), 143170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, R. S. (1986). Distraction-conflict theory: Progress and problems. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogacz, R., Brown, E., Moehlis, J., Holmes, P., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. Psychological Review, 113(4), 700765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruine de Bruin, W., Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2007). Individual differences in adult decision-making competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(5), 938956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruine de Bruin, W., Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2012). Explaining adult age differences in decision-making competence. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(4), 352360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119(2), 197253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Meza, D., Irlenbusch, B., & Reyniers, D. (2008). Financial capability: A behavioural economics perspective. Consumer Research, 69, 192193.Google Scholar
Denes-Raj, V., & Epstein, S. (1994). Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: When people behave against their better judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(5), 819829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, T. L., Inzlicht, M., & Risko, E. F. (2019). Anticipating cognitive effort: roles of perceived error-likelihood and time demands. Psychological Research, 83(5), 10331056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, T. L., Lutes, D. J., & Risko, E. F. (2016). Metacognitive evaluation in the avoidance of demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(9), 13721387.Google ScholarPubMed
Dunn, T. L., & Risko, E. F. (2016). Toward a metacognitive account of cognitive offloading. Cognitive Science, 40(5), 10801127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive–experiential and analytical–rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 390405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 11491160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fishbach, A., & Dhar, R. (2005). Goals as excuses or guides: The liberating effect of perceived goal progress on choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(3), 370377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fishbach, A., Dhar, R., & Zhang, Y. (2006). Subgoals as substitutes or complements: the role of goal accessibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(2), 232242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fu, W. T., & Gray, W. D. (2004). Resolving the paradox of the active user: Stable suboptimal performance in interactive tasks. Cognitive Science, 28(6), 901935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103(4), 650669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gigerenzer, G., & Hoffrage, U. (1995). How to improve Bayesian reasoning without instruction: frequency formats. Psychological Review, 102(4), 684704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gold, J. M., Kool, W., Botvinick, M. M., Hubzin, L., August, S., & Waltz, J. A. (2015). Cognitive effort avoidance and detection in people with schizophrenia. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(1), 145154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, W. D., & Fu, W. T. (2004). Soft constraints in interactive behavior: The case of ignoring perfect knowledge in-the-world for imperfect knowledge in-the-head. Cognitive Science, 28(3), 359382.Google Scholar
Gray, W. D., Sims, C. R., Fu, W. T., & Schoelles, M. J. (2006). The soft constraints hypothesis: a rational analysis approach to resource allocation for interactive behavior. Psychological Review, 113(3), 461482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Heath, C., Larrick, R. P., & Wu, G. (1999). Goals as reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 38(1), 79109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsee, C. K. (1996). The evaluability hypothesis: An explanation for preference reversals between joint and separate evaluations of alternatives. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67(3), 247257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1932). The goal-gradient hypothesis and maze learning. Psychological Review, 39(1), 2543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inzlicht, M., Shenhav, A., & Olivola, C. Y. (2018). The effort paradox: Effort is both costly and valued. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(4), 337349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (2013). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. In Handbook of the fundamentals of financial decision making: Part I (pp. 99127).Google Scholar
Kahol, K., Satava, R. M., Ferrara, J., & Smith, M. L. (2009). Effect of short-term pretrial practice on surgical proficiency in simulated environments: a randomized trial of the “preoperative warm-up” effect. Journal of the American College of Surgeons, 208(2), 255268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanfer, R., Chen, G., & Pritchard, R. D. (Eds.). (2008). Work motivation: Past, present and future. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kivetz, R., Urminsky, O., & Zheng, Y. (2006). The goal-gradient hypothesis resurrected: Purchase acceleration, illusionary goal progress, and customer retention. Journal of Marketing Research, 43(1), 3958.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, H. J., Austin, J. T., & Cooper, J. T. (2008). Goal choice and decision processes. Work motivation: Past, present, and future. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2013). The intrinsic cost of cognitive control. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 697698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2014). A labor/leisure tradeoff in cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 131141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 665682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koriat, A. (2006). Metacognition and consciousness. Institute of Information Processing and Decision Making, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Kozlowski, S. W., & Bell, B. S. (2006). Disentangling achievement orientation and goal setting: effects on self-regulatory processes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(4), 900916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Latham, G. P., & Seijts, G. H. (1999). The effect of proximal and distal goals on performance on a moderately complex task. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 421429.3.0.CO;2-#>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lauriola, M., Panno, A., Levin, I. P., & Lejuez, C. W. (2014). Individual differences in risky decision making: A meta-analysis of sensation seeking and impulsivity with the balloon analogue risk task. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 27(1), 2036.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Louro, M. J., Pieters, R., & Zeelenberg, M. (2007). Dynamics of multiple-goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(2), 174193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manderlink, G., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1984). Proximal versus distal goal setting and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 918928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neal, A., Ballard, T., & Vancouver, J. B. (2017). Dynamic self-regulation and multiple-goal pursuit. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 4, 401423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Northcraft, G. B., Schmidt, A. M., & Ashford, S. J. (2011). Feedback and the rationing of time and effort among competing tasks. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(5), 10761086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nunes, J. C., & Drèze, X, (2006). The endowed progress effect: How artificial advancement increases effort. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(4), 504512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Donoghue, T., & Rabin, M. (2000). The economics of immediate gratification. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13(2), 233250.3.0.CO;2-U>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, small beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 972987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, E. (2012). Beyond comprehension: The role of numeracy in judgments and decisions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 3135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, E., Västfjäll, D., Slovic, P., Mertz, C. K., Mazzocco, K., & Dickert, S. (2006). Numeracy and decision making. Psychological Science, 17(5), 407413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabin, L. A., Fogel, J., & Nutter-Upham, K. E. (2011). Academic procrastination in college students: The role of self-reported executive function. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 344357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandra, D. A., & Otto, A. R. (2018). Cognitive capacity limitations and Need for Cognition differentially predict reward-induced cognitive effort expenditure. Cognition, 172, 101106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, A. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2007). What to do? The effects of discrepancies, incentives, and time on dynamic goal prioritization. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(4), 928941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, A. M., & Dolis, C. M. (2009). Something’s got to give: The effects of dual-goal difficulty, goal progress, and expectancies on resource allocation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(3), 678691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seijts, G. H. (1998). The importance of future time perspective in theories of work motivation. The Journal of Psychology, 132(2), 154168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shellock, F. G., & Prentice, W. E. (1985). Warming-up and stretching for improved physical performance and prevention of sports-related injuries. Sports Medicine, 2(4), 267278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shenhav, A., Musslick, S., Lieder, F., Kool, W., Griffiths, T. L., Cohen, J. D., & Botvinick, M. M. (2017). Toward a rational and mechanistic account of mental effort. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 40, 99124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychological Review, 63(2), 129138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soane, E., & Nicholson, N. (2008). Individual Differences and Decision Making. New-York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Speier, c., Vessey, I., & Valacich, J. S. (2003). The effects of interruptions, task complexity, and information presentation on computer-supported decision-making performance. Decision Sciences, 34(4), 771797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: A meta-analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 6594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steel, P., Brothen, T., & Wambach, C. (2001). Procrastination and personality, performance, and mood. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(1), 95106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal study of procrastination, performance, stress, and health: The costs and benefits of dawdling. Psychological Science, 8(6), 454458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 11241131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vancouver, J. B., Gullekson, N. L., Morse, B. J., & Warren, M. A. (2014). Finding a between-person negative effect of self-efficacy on performance: Not just a within-person effect anymore. Human Performance, 27(3), 243261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vancouver, J. B., Weinhardt, J. M., & Schmidt, A. M. (2010). A formal, computational theory of multiple-goal pursuit: Integrating goal-choice and goal-striving processes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(6), 9851008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Neumann, J., Morgenstern, O., & Kuhn, H. W. (2007). Theory of games and economic behavior (commemorative edition). Princeton university press.Google Scholar
Westbrook, A., & Braver, T. S. (2015). Cognitive effort: A neuroeconomic approach. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(2), 395415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westbrook, A., Kester, D., & Braver, T. S. (2013). What is the subjective cost of cognitive effort? Load, trait, and aging effects revealed by economic preference. PloS one, 8(7), e68210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witkin, H. A., & Goodenough, D. R. (1977). Field dependence and interpersonal behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 84(4), 661689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yechiam, E., Busemeyer, J. R., Stout, J. C., & Bechara, A. (2005). Using cognitive models to map relations between neuropsychological disorders and human decision-making deficits. Psychological Science, 16(12), 973978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zakay, D. (1990). The role of personal tendencies in the selection of decision-making strategies. The Psychological Record, 40(2), 207213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1: Task management game screens: (A) List of apartments for information retrieval, (B) Apartment data for the smaller task, (C) Apartment data for the larger task.

Figure 1

Table 1: Number of tasks addressed, number of items retrieved and number of points earned by the participants in each of the three experimental conditions in Study 1. (SD in parentheses).

Figure 2

Table 2: Tasks’ handling patterns and performance (items retrieved and points earned) in the free choice condition in Study 1, according to the first type of task addressed. (SD in parentheses.)

Figure 3

Table 3: Tasks’ handling patterns and performance (items retrieved and points earnt) in Study 2, as a function of first type of task addressed. (SD in parentheses.)

Figure 4

Table 4: The correlations between rational and intuitive thinking styles (REI), the percentage of smaller tasks addressed, number of items retrieved and number of points earned by participants. (**, p<.01.)

Figure 5

Figure 2: Structure of the street index.

Figure 6

Figure 3: The names of streets in a specific neighborhood in the city of Tel Aviv. Street names are listed in alphabetical order, with rough estimates of apartment prices. For example, a rough estimate for a 3-room apartment on Alexander St. is 1,670,000 Israeli shekels.

Figure 7

Table 5: Shopping voucher lotteries and the qualification criteria. Note that participants could qualify for multiple lotteries.

Supplementary material: File

Rusou et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Rusou et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 8.5 KB
Supplementary material: File

Rusou et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 1.1 KB
Supplementary material: File

Rusou et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 1.6 KB