Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vfjqv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T16:21:10.827Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The genetic epidemiology of schizotypal personality disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 February 2024

Kenneth S. Kendler*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Henrik Ohlsson
Affiliation:
Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden
Jan Sundquist
Affiliation:
Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden University Clinic Primary Care Skane, Sweden Department of Population Health Science and Policy, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, NY, USA
Kristina Sundquist
Affiliation:
Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden University Clinic Primary Care Skane, Sweden Department of Population Health Science and Policy, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, NY, USA
*
Corresponding author: Kenneth S. Kendler; Email: Kenneth.Kendler@vcuhealth.org
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The concept of schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) emerged from observations of personality characteristics common in relatives of schizophrenic patients. While often studied in family designs, few studies and none with genetic measures, have examined SPD in epidemiological samples.

Methods

We studied individuals born in Sweden 1940–2000 with an ICD-10 diagnosis of SPD with no prior schizophrenia (SZ) diagnosis (n = 2292). Demographic features, patterns of comorbidity, and Family Genetic Risk Scores (FGRS) were assessed from multiple Swedish registries. Prediction of progression to SZ was assessed by Cox models.

Results

SPD was rare, with a prevalence of 0.044%, and had high levels of comorbidity with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), OCD, ADHD, and major depression (MD), and increased rates of being single, unemployed and in receipt of welfare. Affected individuals had elevated levels of FGRS for SZ (+0.42), ASD (+0.30), MD (+0.29), and ADHD (+0.20). Compared to cases of schizophrenia, they had significantly lower rates of FGRSSZ, but significantly elevated rates of genetic risk for ASD, MD, and ADHD. Over a mean follow-up of 8.7 years, 14.6% of SPD cases received a first diagnosis of SZ, the risk for which was significantly increased by levels of FGRSSZ, male sex, young age at SPD diagnosis and an in-patient SPD diagnosis and significantly decreased by comorbidity with MD, ASD, and ADHD.

Conclusions

Our results not only support the designation of SPD as a schizophrenia spectrum disorder but also suggest potentially important etiologic links between SPD and ASD and, to a lesser extent, ADHD, OCD, and MD.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

In many instances it has been observed that, while some members of a family are manifest lunatics, others are only eccentric in character, displaying peculiarities of habit, conduct, and disposition. (Prichard, Reference Prichard1837) p. 124.

Every psychiatrist is aware how frequently especially the relatives of dementia praecox patients present deviations from normal mental behavior, which can be described as ‘abnormal characteristics’, but who in no way are to be viewed as fully schizophrenic. (Hoffmann, Reference Hoffmann1921) p.7; (Kendler and Klee, Reference Kendler and Klee2022b)

… the studies on empirical hereditary prognosis have shown that in the sphere of relatives of schizophrenics, this type of [schizoid/schizotypal] psychopath has a special position in terms of genetics. If we were to describe people who we do not know as blood relatives of schizophrenics as ‘schizoid’ psychopaths, we would say something about them that we cannot prove, namely their genetic relationship to schizophrenics. Such people, no matter how clearly they display a ‘schizoid’ nature, should not be assigned this genetically prejudicial name, but rather classify them purely descriptively typologically. (Luxenburger, Reference Luxenburger and Boeters1939) (translation by A Klee and KSK) p. 812-3

The origins of our current concept of Schizotypal Personality Disorder (SPD), as outlined in DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and ICD-10 (Organization, 1992), can be traced to many comments from psychiatrists in the 19th century who, like Prichard, noted an excess of unusual character traits in relatives of their ‘insane’ asylum patients followed by systematic observations in the first set of family studies employing dementia praecox probands conducted in Munich by a number of investigators, including Hoffmann, in the first decades of the 20th century.

The familial/genetic link to schizophrenia was also employed in the development of the DSM-III criteria for SPD, the forerunner of the current DSM-5 and ICD-criteria. These criteria were based on personality traits observed in the cases of ‘borderline schizophrenia’ in the Danish Adoption study of schizophrenia (Spitzer, Endicott, & Gibbon, Reference Spitzer, Endicott and Gibbon1979) that had been shown to aggregate strongly in the biological relatives of the schizophrenia adoptees (Kety, Rosenthal, Wender, & Schulsinger, Reference Kety, Rosenthal, Wender and Schulsinger1971). Thus, the SPD diagnosis was created to capture the clinical features of individuals with a strong genetic risk for schizophrenia (Kendler, Reference Kendler1985). Many studies have since shown that SPD does indeed substantially aggregate in the close relatives of individuals with schizophrenia (Asarnow et al., Reference Asarnow, Nuechterlein, Fogelson, Subotnik, Payne, Russell and Kendler2001; Baron, Gruen, Asnis, & Kane, Reference Baron, Gruen, Asnis and Kane1983; Kendler, Gruenberg, & Strauss, Reference Kendler, Gruenberg and Strauss1981; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, McGuire, Gruenberg, O'Hare, Spellman and Walsh1993).

However, as pointed out by the above 1939 quote from Luxenburger – best known for conducting the first-ever systematic twin study of SZ (Luxenburger, Reference Luxenburger1928) (Kendler and Klee, Reference Kendler and Klee2022a) — it is one thing to show that SPD is increased in relatives of schizophrenia probands and another to show that SPD, ascertained from a population-based sample, bears a substantial genetic risk for schizophrenia. There might, for example, be multiple developmental pathways to SPD in the general population many of which do not depend on an elevated genetic risk for schizophrenia. Support for this hypothesis comes from the weak and/or inconsistent evidence that individuals selected solely for scoring high on self-report schizotypy scales have elevated schizophrenia polygenic risk scores (Docherty et al., Reference Docherty, Shabalin, Adkins, Mann, Krueger, Bacanu and McIntosh2020; Hatzimanolis et al., Reference Hatzimanolis, Avramopoulos, Arking, Moes, Bhatnagar, Lencz and Smyrnis2018; Mas-Bermejo et al., Reference Mas-Bermejo, Papiol, Via, Rovira, Torrecilla, Kwapil and Rosa2023) and epidemiological evidence that schizotypal/psychotic-like symptoms may not be at all specific to a liability to schizophrenia but rather may lie on the continuum of neuroticism and precede or follow the onset of a range of nonpsychotic psychiatric disorders (McGorry, Hartmann, Spooner, & Nelson, Reference McGorry, Hartmann, Spooner and Nelson2018; McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Saha, Al-Hamzawi, Andrade, Benjet, Bromet and Kessler2016; Saha, Scott, Varghese, & McGrath, Reference Saha, Scott, Varghese and McGrath2011; Zoghbi et al., Reference Zoghbi, Bernanke, Gleichman, Masucci, Corcoran, Califano and Brucato2019).

This report takes a different and to our knowledge novel approach to the study of SPD – to examine it in a general population sample (which has been done at least three times previously (Hastrup, Jennum, Ibsen, Kjellberg, & Simonsen, Reference Hastrup, Jennum, Ibsen, Kjellberg and Simonsen2021; Hjorthøj, Albert, & Nordentoft, Reference Hjorthøj, Albert and Nordentoft2018; Köhler-Forsberg et al., Reference Köhler-Forsberg, Antonsen, Pedersen, Mortensen, McGrath and Mors2023)) with available genetic risk measures. We ask the following major questions:

  1. 1. For SPD, what is its prevalence, age at onset, patterns of comorbidity (examining specifically those disorders previously suggested to be associated with SPD: autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Barneveld et al., Reference Barneveld, Pieterse, de Sonneville, van Rijn, Lahuis, van Engeland and Swaab2011; Esterberg, Trotman, Brasfield, Compton, & Walker, Reference Esterberg, Trotman, Brasfield, Compton and Walker2008), ADHD (Bernardi et al., Reference Bernardi, Faraone, Cortese, Kerridge, Pallanti, Wang and Blanco2012; Fagel et al., Reference Fagel, Swaab, De Sonneville, Van Rijn, Pieterse, Scheepers and Van Engeland2013), OCD (Attademo & Bernardini, Reference Attademo and Bernardini2021), and major depression (MD) (Rosell, Futterman, McMaster, & Siever, Reference Rosell, Futterman, McMaster and Siever2014)), and levels of social functioning?

  2. 2. In cases of SPD, what is their average genetic risk of schizophrenia and the potentially comorbid disorders of ASD, ADHD, OCD, and MD, and how does this profile of genetic risk compare with that of a matched sample of patients with schizophrenia?

  3. 3. What proportion of cases of SPD progress to a diagnosis of schizophrenia and to what degree can demographic, clinical, and levels of genetic risk predict this progression? More specifically can we replicate prior evidence that among cases of SPD, drug use disorder predicts the development of schizophrenia?(Hjorthøj et al., Reference Hjorthøj, Albert and Nordentoft2018)

Methods

We collected information on individuals from Swedish population-based registers with national coverage linking each person's unique personal identification number which, to preserve confidentiality, was replaced with a serial number by Statistics Sweden (online Supplementary appendix table 1 and figure 1). We secured ethical approval for this study from the Regional Ethical Review Board in Lund and no participant consent was required (No. 2008/409 and later amendments). Our database consisted of all individuals born in Sweden between 1940 and 2000, to Swedish born parents. In the database, we included registrations for Schizotypal Personality Disorder (SPD) and schizophrenia (SZ). SPD was only available in ICD 10 (1997 to 2018), while for SZ we could also use ICD8 and ICD9 (registrations going back to 1973). See online Supplementary appendix table 2 for definitions of these variables.

For our first question (demographic features of SPD cases), we considered individuals with SPD without a prior registration of SZ. To compare these individuals with the general population we matched the cases of SPD with five individuals without a registration for SPD with the same year of birth, same sex, and same municipality of residence at the time of the cases' SPD registration. For SPD cases and their controls, we included lifetime registrations for MD, OCD, ADHD, and ASD in the database. Furthermore, we included information on social welfare, unemployment status, marital status, educational attainment, and grades from lower secondary school (at age 16). We used conditional logistic regression to calculate Odds Ratios (OR) by comparing SPD cases and their controls.

For the second question (genetic risk profiles), we created a database including the SPD cases and individuals registered for SZ during the same period (1997 to 2018). In the database, we included individual family genetic risk scores (FGRS) for SZ, MD, ASD, OCD, and ADHD, respectively. The FGRS is an estimate of an individual's genetic liability that reflects the relative lack or excess of disease in their pedigree relative to population expectations. The FGRSs are calculated from morbidity risks in first- through fifth-degree relatives, controlling for cohabitation effects, and thus arise from phenotypes in extended pedigrees (see online Supplementary Appendix table 3), not from molecular genetic data. We calculated the least squares means (95% CIs) for all FGRSs for the SPD and SZ cases while controlling for year of birth and sex. To evaluate potential genetic heterogeneity among SPD cases, we performed a Latent class analysis using the 4 FGRSs (OCD, ASD, ADHD, and SZ) as observed variables. We categorized the FGRSs into 4 classes based on k-means clustering before entering them into the LCA (see online Supplementary appendix table 4). The number of latent classes was determined by comparing model fit statistics between nested models. Improvement in model fit is indicated by smaller values of, Akaike's Information Criterion (AIC) (Akaike, Reference Akaike1987), Bayesian information criterion (BIC) (Vrieze, Reference Vrieze2012), and entropy values close to 1.0.

For the third major question (conversion to SZ among SPD cases), we selected SPD cases born between 1960 and 2000 because earlier cohorts had a truncated follow-up given late ages at first registration due to the availability of ICD-10 only in 1997. We used a Cox regression model to investigate time to SZ where we followed individuals to date of SZ, date of death, date of emigration, or end of follow-up (2018-12-31) whichever came first. We performed a series of univariable models with the demographic features, the FGRS profiles, educational variables, the comorbidity variables, and the site of diagnosis (e.g. inpatient, primary care) of SPD as covariates. All statistical analyses were performed using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, 2012).

Results

Demographic features

We found in our cohort 2952 individuals diagnosed with SPD, 660 (22.3%) of whom had a prior diagnosis of SZ and therefore were not further considered. Using the remaining cases (n = 2292; 47.5% male), SPD had a population prevalence of 0.044%. The site of registration for SPD, using a hierarchy, was inpatient n = 531 (23.2%), specialist (including psychiatrists) n = 784 (34.2%), and primary care n = 977 (42.6%). Estimation of an accurate age at first diagnosis was problematic given that diagnoses were only available from 1997–2018. This is likely best captured by our 1980–89 cohort where the mean (s.d.) equaled 25.8 (4.4) years.

We examined the rates of four psychiatric disorders suspected of being comorbid with SPD in our SPD cohort compared to matched controls (Table 1). All were substantially more common in our SPD with the ORs being strongest for ASD, followed by OCD, ADHD, and finally MD. Compared to controls, individuals with SPD were significantly more likely to receive social welfare, to be single, to be divorced if married, to have low academic performance at age 16, and to have low educational attainment. They also had moderately elevated rates of unemployment.

Table 1. Comparison with the general population (SPD individuals matched to five controls (same year of birth, same sex, and same municipality (at time of SPD registration)

*Low grades and low education are defined as more than 1 SD below the mean score.

Genetic risk profile for SPD

We present in Fig. 1, the mean (± 95% CIs) FGRS for our cases of SPD for schizophrenia, ASD, OCD, ADHD, and MD. The rates were substantially and significantly elevated for schizophrenia followed by ASD and MD. Elevations were more modest for ADHD and especially OCD. We also examined the mean (±95% CIs) levels of the FRGSSZ as a function of the site of SPD diagnosis. No significant difference was observed across the three groups: in-patient 0.41 (0.24–0.59); specialist 0.44 (0.29–0.59) and primary care 0.43 (0.30–0.56).

Figure 1. Mean (and 95% CIs) for the familial genetic risk score, depicted on the Y-axis, for schizophrenia (SZ), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), ADHD, and major depression (MD) and in cases of schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) and schizophrenia in the Swedish population born 1940–2000 by ICD-10 (for exact values of these results see online Supplementary appendix table 5). * FRGS scores for SPD and schizophrenia differ significantly.

A comparison of the genetic risk profile for SPD and schizophrenia

We then calculated the FGRS for the same set of four disorders in cases of schizophrenia ascertained over the same registration period (n = 12 466) and compared them to those found for SPD (Fig. 1). The genetic risk for schizophrenia was substantially and significantly higher in the schizophrenic v. the SPD cases while the reverse pattern was seen for the genetic risk for ASD, ADHD, and MD. The genetic risk for OCD did not differ in the two proband groups.

Evaluation of possible clinician bias in SPD diagnosis

Because the FGRS is based on recorded diagnoses in the extended family and not molecular genetic data, it is possible that Swedish clinicians were more likely to diagnose SPD in individuals with a close relative with schizophrenia, thereby biasing upward their apparent genetic risk. To evaluate this hypothesis, we recalculated the FGRSSZ in our samples for SPD and, as a comparison, for schizophrenia after stripping out information on all first-degree relatives. The mean (±95% CIs) FGRS in our SPD cases declined very modestly from 0.42 (0.32–0.52) to 0.39 (0.29–0.48)], with a similar small decline seen in our schizophrenia cases, from 0.58 (0.54–0.62) to 0.54 (0.50–0.58).

Genetic heterogeneity in SPD

Given that our SPD cohort has elevated genetic risk for four relatively uncommon disorders (SZ, ASD, OCD, and ADHD), we sought to evaluate the hypothesis that our sample is genetically heterogeneous and can be cleanly divided into more homogeneous groups each with a substantial elevation of genetic risk for only one of these disorders. We therefore performed a latent class analysis (LCA) of these four FGRSs each divided into four levels based on K-means clustering. By all three of our fit indices (AIC, BIC, and entropy), the three class solution fitted best (online Supplementary Appendix table 6). For simplicity of interpretation, we focus on the combination of the medium high and high groups as seen in Table 2, our three classes were easily named as those at (i) generally low genetic risk (67% of the sample), (ii) intermediate genetic risk (26% of the sample), and (iii) high genetic risk (7% of the sample). The low-risk group contained only a small proportion of individuals (all <4.0%) with substantially elevated genetic risks for our four disorders. In the intermediate-risk group, 5–21% of the sample had substantially elevated genetic risk to one of the four disorders, with those at high genetic risk for ADHD and ASD being the most common. In those at high genetic risk, 17–3% of the subjects were at particularly high genetic risk for one of our four disorders, with high risk being most common for ASD and SZ.

Table 2. Results from three-class solution of the latent class analysis of cases of SPD

Progression to schizophrenia over time

For these analyses, we examined all SPD cases born in 1960 and onwards (n = 1664). By the end of our follow-up period, a mean (s.d.) of 8.7 (5.3) years, 243 individuals (14.6%) had received a first diagnosis of SZ. Table 3 presents a univariable Cox regression analysis predicting the conversion of SPD to SZ in our sample. Progression was significantly predicted by all three demographic variables: earlier year of birth, younger age at first SPD diagnosis, and, particularly strongly, male sex. Of the five genetic risk factors examined, now including drug use disorder, only risk for schizophrenia significantly predicted progression of SPD to schizophrenia. We illustrate the impact of this effect in Fig. 2, with survival curves for three groups of the SPD cases at low, moderate, and high FGRSSZ. Twelve years after the first SPD diagnosis, the risk for schizophrenia (±95%CI) in the three groups were, respectively, 18.5% (95%CI: 16.0–21.4), 21.6% (14.2–32.1), and 36.9% (24.5–53.1). Poor academic performance at age 16 significantly protected against the development of schizophrenia. Compared to a diagnosis of SPD received in primary care, the rate of schizophrenia was substantially higher in those diagnosed in specialty and in-patient care. Comorbidity with MD, ADHD, and ASD but not OCD significantly reduced the probability of the progression to schizophrenia.

Table 3. Prediction of conversion of Schizotypal personality disorder to Schizophrenia

1 Upper CI limit = 1.001.

Figure 2. Survival curves for progression to a diagnosis of schizophrenia In cases of SPD with low, intermediate, and high genetic risk for Schizophrenia as calculated by a Cox model. The X-axis represents years since first diagnosis and the Y axis the Proportion who received a first diagnosis of schizophrenia.

Discussion

Using the Swedish medical and genealogical registries, we examined the genetic epidemiology of SPD as diagnosed by ICD-10 criteria. Of the many findings reported here, eight are noteworthy and are discussed in turn. First, SPD, as diagnosed in a clinical setting by a physician, was a quite rare disorder, diagnosed in around 0.04% of the Swedish population with a nearly equal prevalence in males and females. These estimates are much lower than the prevalence estimates of 1.6–3.9% obtained at personal interviews in four general population surveys in the US (Sansone & Sansone, Reference Sansone and Sansone2011) and 0.6% found in one such survey in Norway (Torgersen, Kringlen, & Cramer, Reference Torgersen, Kringlen and Cramer2001).

Second, consistent with prior reports (Attademo & Bernardini, Reference Attademo and Bernardini2021; Barneveld et al., Reference Barneveld, Pieterse, de Sonneville, van Rijn, Lahuis, van Engeland and Swaab2011; Esterberg et al., Reference Esterberg, Trotman, Brasfield, Compton and Walker2008; Rosell et al., Reference Rosell, Futterman, McMaster and Siever2014), we found that cases of SPD had, as a group, high levels of psychiatric comorbidity that were particularly prominent for ASD, OCD, ADHD, and MD.

Third, consistent with a prior Danish study (Hastrup et al., Reference Hastrup, Jennum, Ibsen, Kjellberg and Simonsen2021), we found that SPD patients were relatively impaired, with poor marital, occupational, and educational functioning, and an over 8-fold elevation in receipt of social welfare.

Fourth, as diagnosed by Swedish clinicians, individuals with SPD are at a substantially elevated genetic risk for schizophrenia. This provides robust support, from an epidemiological sample, for the repeated observations in adoption and family studies for the close familial/genetic link between SPD and schizophrenia (Asarnow et al., Reference Asarnow, Nuechterlein, Fogelson, Subotnik, Payne, Russell and Kendler2001; Baron et al., Reference Baron, Gruen, Asnis and Kane1983; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Gruenberg and Strauss1981; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, McGuire, Gruenberg, O'Hare, Spellman and Walsh1993). The possibility that our schizophrenia genetic risk estimate for the SPD cases was upwardly biased by clinicians preferentially assigning an SPD diagnosis to cases with schizophrenic first-degree relatives was not supported by our analyses. Our robust evidence for a genetic link between SPD and schizophrenia stands in contrast to the weak indications of elevated schizophrenia polygenic risk scores (PRS) in population samples scoring high on self-report schizotypy scales (Docherty et al., Reference Docherty, Shabalin, Adkins, Mann, Krueger, Bacanu and McIntosh2020; Hatzimanolis et al., Reference Hatzimanolis, Avramopoulos, Arking, Moes, Bhatnagar, Lencz and Smyrnis2018; Mas-Bermejo et al., Reference Mas-Bermejo, Papiol, Via, Rovira, Torrecilla, Kwapil and Rosa2023). These findings might arise because relatives of schizophrenics are much better discriminated against from relatives of controls by the signs of schizotypy found at personal interviews, such as odd communication, poor rapport, guardedness, and oddness than by schizotypal symptoms assessed by questionnaire, such as magical thinking, ideas of reference and illusions (Kendler, McGuire, Gruenberg, and Walsh, Reference Kendler, McGuire, Gruenberg and Walsh1995; Kendler, Thacker, and Walsh, Reference Kendler, Thacker and Walsh1996). This is, in turn, consistent with evidence for the lack of specificity of the polygenic risk from a large genetic association study of psychotic experiences in the UK Biobank (Legge et al., Reference Legge, Jones, Kendall, Pardiñas, Menzies, Bracher-Smith and Hotopf2019). This study found support for a shared genetic liability between psychotic experiences and schizophrenia, major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, ASD, and ADHD suggesting that SZ has a more specific genetic relationship with SPD than with the phenotype of psychotic experiences. Of note, the level of genetic risk in our SPD cases did not differ for those diagnosed in primary, specialist, or in-patient sites, suggesting that, at least from a genetic perspective, the validity of SPD was independent of the medical site of diagnosis.

Fifth, the second highest genetic risk score observed in our SPD sample was for ASD. We are unaware of prior reports of this association but it is consistent with the frequent overlap observed for schizotypal and autistic symptoms and signs (Barneveld et al., Reference Barneveld, Pieterse, de Sonneville, van Rijn, Lahuis, van Engeland and Swaab2011). Of particular interest, FGRSASD was significantly higher in SPD cases than in schizophrenia, suggesting an etiologic relationship between the two syndromes. One way to assess the genetic relationship between SPD and ASD is to note that the level of FGRSASD in the SPD cases was 47% of that observed in ASD cases in this cohort from which we removed the SPD cases. This is considerably lower than the parallel figure of 72% for FGRSSZ in cases of SPD v. schizophrenia. Our results suggest an important genetic relationship between SPD and ASD but not nearly as close as that seen for SPD and schizophrenia.

Sixth, the cases of SPD also had significant elevations of genetic risk for OCD, ADHD, and MD, which are consistent with prior reports of excess comorbidity of SPD with these disorders (Attademo & Bernardini, Reference Attademo and Bernardini2021; Bernardi et al., Reference Bernardi, Faraone, Cortese, Kerridge, Pallanti, Wang and Blanco2012; Fagel et al., Reference Fagel, Swaab, De Sonneville, Van Rijn, Pieterse, Scheepers and Van Engeland2013; Rosell et al., Reference Rosell, Futterman, McMaster and Siever2014). Compared to cases of schizophrenia, genetic risks were significantly elevated for ADHD and MD but not OCD.

Seventh, we evaluated, via LCA, whether our sample could be characterized by high levels of genetic heterogeneity such that most of our SPD cases had high genetic risk for only one of our four rarer disorders: SZ, ASD, OCD, and ADHD. Our best fit 3 class solution – defined as those at low, moderate, and high genetic risk -- was inconsistent with this hypothesis as the three classes differed quantitatively across all four genetic risks.

Finally, our follow-up data showed that 15% of our SPD cases developed schizophrenia, a rate moderately lower than the 20.9% reported over three treatment contacts in a Danish sample of SPD cases (Köhler-Forsberg et al., Reference Köhler-Forsberg, Antonsen, Pedersen, Mortensen, McGrath and Mors2023) and considerably lower than the 33% seen over 20 years also in a Danish sample (Hjorthøj et al., Reference Hjorthøj, Albert and Nordentoft2018) of SPD cases. The literature also contains one ‘negative’ study as Zogbi et al., found that among those with an Attenuated Psychosis Syndrome, a diagnosis of SPD did not predict the development of schizophrenia (Zoghbi et al., Reference Zoghbi, Bernanke, Gleichman, Masucci, Corcoran, Califano and Brucato2019). Importantly, among the genetic predictors only the FRGSSZ predicted conversion to SZ and, as seen in Fig. 2, the effect is relatively large. Consistent with epidemiological data showing a male excess in incident cases of schizophrenia (McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Saha, Welham, El, MacCauley and Chant2004), we found males with SPD were over 50% more likely than females to progress to schizophrenia. A large effect was also found for the site of diagnosis. Since the site did not influence genetic risk in our SPD cases, it is likely that this effect was a result of the greater comorbidity in the SPD cases diagnosed in hospital and specialist v. primary care settings. Interestingly, in SPD cases, comorbidity with MD, ASD, and ADHD reduced the risk for the development of schizophrenia. Given prior evidence that a significant proportion of cases with drug-induced psychosis go on to develop schizophrenia especially if they have a high genetic risk for SZ (Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2019), we included drug use disorder (DUD) in cases of SPD as a possible predictor of the development of SZ. However, we did not find, as reported by Hjorthøj et al., that the SPD to schizophrenia conversion was significantly predicted by drug use disorder (Hjorthøj et al., Reference Hjorthøj, Albert and Nordentoft2018). However, their reported HR (1.34) is within the confidence intervals of our finding (HR = 1.19 (0.92; 1.53), so we cannot claim a definitive non-replication.

Limitations

These results should be considered in the context of four methodological limitations. First, on the basis of the limited number of prior population prevalence estimates based on personal interviews (Sansone & Sansone, Reference Sansone and Sansone2011; Torgersen et al., Reference Torgersen, Kringlen and Cramer2001), it is likely that SPD is substantially under-diagnosed in the Swedish healthcare system. This is perhaps unsurprising as SPD is not likely to be a diagnosis emphasized in the training of many physicians. We have a limited ability to determine the degree to which the diagnosed cases of SPD in Sweden are representative of all population cases. The fact that the conversion rate to schizophrenia is lower in our sample than seen in Danish registry cases of SPD suggests that our sample is not heavily biased toward severe cases and/or those with very schizophrenia-like presentations. Supportive of this conclusion is the high percentage of our SPD cases diagnosed by primary care physicians. We also show that our robust evidence of genetic risk for SZ in our SPD cases does not substantially result from a clinician bias to diagnose SPD in first-degree relatives of SZ probands. These findings provide some evidence to suggest that our SPD cases are not highly biased toward those with clinically severe schizophrenia-like presentations.

Second, our findings are also dependent on the quality of the non-SPD diagnoses from the Swedish registers which have generally been considered of high quality (Ludvigsson et al., Reference Ludvigsson, Andersson, Ekbom, Feychting, Kim, Reuterwall and Olausson2011). The validity of SZ and OCD has been explicitly examined and supported (Ekholm et al., Reference Ekholm, Ekholm, Adolfsson, Vares, Osby, Sedvall and Jonsson2005; Lichtenstein et al., Reference Lichtenstein, Bjork, Hultman, Scolnick, Sklar and Sullivan2006; Rück et al., Reference Rück, Larsson, Lind, Perez-Vigil, Isomura, Sariaslan and Mataix-Cols2015). The validity of MD diagnoses is supported by its prevalence, sex ratio, familial correlations, and associations with psychosocial risk factors (Kendler, Ohlsson, Lichtenstein, Sundquist, and Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Lichtenstein, Sundquist and Sundquist2018; Sundquist, Ohlsson, Sundquist, and Kendler, Reference Sundquist, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Kendler2017). However, we know of no studies assessing the validity of ADHD or ASD diagnoses in the Swedish medical registries although they have both been used in prior studies (e.g. (Daniels et al., Reference Daniels, Forssen, Hultman, Cnattingius, Savitz, Feychting and Sparen2008; Giacobini et al., Reference Giacobini, Ahnemark, Medin, Freilich, Andersson, Ma and Ginsberg2023; Keil et al., Reference Keil, Daniels, Forssen, Hultman, Cnattingius, Söderberg and Sparen2010; Zhang-James et al., Reference Zhang-James, Chen, Kuja-Halkola, Lichtenstein, Larsson and Faraone2020)).

Third, the FGRS, a family phenotype-based measure to assess quantitative genetic risk distinct from PRS, has been now widely published (Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2021a; Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2021b; Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2023a; Kendler, Ohlsson, Bacanu et al., Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Bacanu, Sundquist, Edwards and Sundquist2023b; Kendler, Ohlsson, Mościcki et al., Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Mościcki, Sundquist, Edwards and Sundquist2023c; Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2023d; Kendler, Rosmalen, Ohlsson, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Rosmalen, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2023e) with evidence that it is not highly sensitive to assumptions involved in its calculation, that the correction for cohabitation effects performs appropriately, and that the method agrees well with other similar statistical approaches (Hujoel, Gazal, Loh, Patterson, & Price, Reference Hujoel, Gazal, Loh, Patterson and Price2020; Krebs et al., Reference Krebs, Hellberg, Ohlsson, Steinbach, Pedersen, Werge and Kendler2023). Furthermore, recent empirical analyses and simulations demonstrate that the observed modest correlations between FRGS-like statistics and PRS from the iPsych study for psychiatric disorders are consistent with the hypothesis that current phenotype-based extended family measures, like those used in this study, and molecular-based polygene scores are both fallible measures of the same underlying large set of small effect genetic risk alleles (Krebs et al., Reference Krebs, Hellberg, Ohlsson, Steinbach, Pedersen, Werge and Kendler2023).

Above, we noted the frequent symptom overlap between schizotypal and autistic symptoms and signs (Cook, Zhang, & Constantino, Reference Cook, Zhang and Constantino2020; Esterberg et al., Reference Esterberg, Trotman, Brasfield, Compton and Walker2008), but did not explicitly address the question of whether the high rates of FGRSASD in our SPD potentially reflects misdiagnosis, that is that some cases of SPD may really have ASD. To address this question, we excluded 13.8% of the SPD cases with comorbid ASD and calculated the mean FGRSASD for the remaining SPD cases. We found that it decreased only modestly and non-significantly from the value found for the full sample (0.304, 0.247–0.361) to 0.247 (0.188–0.306). This suggests that the elevated FGRS for ASD in our SPD cases is not likely a result of misdiagnosis or solely due to levels of SPD/ASD comorbidity.

Conclusion

As diagnosed in the Swedish primary-care, specialist, and in-patient registries, SPD is a rare disorder. Affected individuals have high levels of comorbidity with ASD, OCD, ADHD, and MD and frequently demonstrate impaired educational attainment and subsequent psychosocial functioning. They have substantial elevations in their genetic risk for schizophrenia, independent of the specific registry in which they were diagnosed, but are also at increased genetic risk for ASD, MD, ADHD, and, to a lesser extent, OCD. Compared to cases of schizophrenia, they have significantly lower genetic risk of schizophrenia, but higher risks for ASD, ADHD, and MD. We could reject the hypothesis that our cases of SPD were genetically heterogeneous with most cases having an elevated genetic risk for only one of the four relatively rare disorders: SZ, ASD, ADHD, and OCD. Approximately 15% progress to schizophrenia over an 8-year follow-up. Predisposing factors include male sex, genetic risk for schizophrenia, and SPD diagnoses from an in-patient setting. Protective factors include comorbidity with ASD, MD, and ASD. Our results not only support the designation of SPD as a schizophrenia spectrum disorder but also suggest a potentially important etiologic link to ASD, and perhaps to ADHD, OCD, and MD.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724000230.

Acknowledgements

None.

Funding statement

The funding source had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication. Location of where work was done at Lund University, Virginia Commonwealth University. This project was supported by grants from the Swedish Research Council (2020-01175).

Competing interests

None.

References

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52(3), 317332. doi:10.1007/bf02294359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: Fifth edition, DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Asarnow, R. F., Nuechterlein, K. H., Fogelson, D., Subotnik, K. L., Payne, D. A., Russell, A. T., … Kendler, K. S. (2001). Schizophrenia and schizophrenia-spectrum personality disorders in the first-degree relatives of children with schizophrenia: The UCLA family study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(6), 581588. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11386988CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Attademo, L., & Bernardini, F. (2021). Schizotypal personality disorder in clinical obsessive–compulsive disorder samples: A brief overview. CNS Spectrums, 26(5), 468480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barneveld, P. S., Pieterse, J., de Sonneville, L., van Rijn, S., Lahuis, B., van Engeland, H., & Swaab, H. (2011). Overlap of autistic and schizotypal traits in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Research, 126(1–3), 231236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, M., Gruen, R., Asnis, L., & Kane, J. (1983). Familial relatedness of schizophrenia and schizotypal states. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140(11), 14371442. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6624988Google ScholarPubMed
Bernardi, S., Faraone, S. V., Cortese, S., Kerridge, B. T., Pallanti, S., Wang, S., & Blanco, C. (2012). The lifetime impact of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC). Psychological Medicine, 42(4), 875887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, M. L., Zhang, Y., & Constantino, J. N. (2020). On the continuity between autistic and schizoid personality disorder trait burden: A prospective study in adolescence. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 208(2), 94100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniels, J. L., Forssen, U., Hultman, C. M., Cnattingius, S., Savitz, D. A., Feychting, M., & Sparen, P. (2008). Parental psychiatric disorders associated with autism spectrum disorders in the offspring. Pediatrics, 121(5), e1357e1362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Docherty, A. R., Shabalin, A. A., Adkins, D. E., Mann, F., Krueger, R. F., Bacanu, S.-A., … McIntosh, A. M. (2020). Molecular genetic risk for psychosis is associated with psychosis risk symptoms in a population-based UK cohort: Findings from generation Scotland. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 46(5), 10451052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekholm, B., Ekholm, A., Adolfsson, R., Vares, M., Osby, U., Sedvall, G. C., & Jonsson, E. G. (2005). Evaluation of diagnostic procedures in Swedish patients with schizophrenia and related psychoses. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 59(1), 457464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esterberg, M. L., Trotman, H. D., Brasfield, J. L., Compton, M. T., & Walker, E. F. (2008). Childhood and current autistic features in adolescents with schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 104(1–3), 265273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fagel, S. S., Swaab, H., De Sonneville, L. M., Van Rijn, S., Pieterse, J. K., Scheepers, F., & Van Engeland, H. (2013). Development of schizotypal symptoms following psychiatric disorders in childhood or adolescence. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 22, 683692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giacobini, M., Ahnemark, E., Medin, E., Freilich, J., Andersson, M., Ma, Y., & Ginsberg, Y. (2023). Epidemiology, treatment patterns, comorbidities, and concomitant medication in patients with ADHD in Sweden: A registry-based study (2018–2021). Journal of Attention Disorders, 27(12), 13091321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hastrup, L. H., Jennum, P., Ibsen, R., Kjellberg, J., & Simonsen, E. (2021). Costs of schizotypal disorder: A matched-controlled nationwide register-based study of patients and spouses. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 144(1), 6071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatzimanolis, A., Avramopoulos, D., Arking, D. E., Moes, A., Bhatnagar, P., Lencz, T., … Smyrnis, N. (2018). Stress-dependent association between polygenic risk for schizophrenia and schizotypal traits in young army recruits. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(2), 338347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hjorthøj, C., Albert, N., & Nordentoft, M. (2018). Association of substance use disorders with conversion from schizotypal disorder to schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(7), 733739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffmann, H. (1921). Studien über Vererbung und Entstehung Geistiger Störungen: II. Die Nachkommenschaft bei endogenen Psychosen. In Studien über vererbung und entstehung geistiger störungen (pp. 4231). Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hujoel, M. L., Gazal, S., Loh, P.-R., Patterson, N., & Price, A. L. (2020). Liability threshold modeling of case–control status and family history of disease increases association power. Nature Genetics, 52(5), 541547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keil, A., Daniels, J. L., Forssen, U., Hultman, C., Cnattingius, S., Söderberg, K. C., … Sparen, P. (2010). Parental autoimmune diseases associated with autism spectrum disorders in offspring. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass.), 21(6), 805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S. (1985). Diagnostic approaches to schizotypal personality disorder: A historical perspective. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 11(4), 538553. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3909377CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Gruenberg, A. M., & Strauss, J. S. (1981). An independent analysis of the Copenhagen sample of the Danish adoption study of schizophrenia. II. The relationship between schizotypal personality disorder and schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38(9), 982984. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7283669CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., & Klee, A. (2022a). The beginnings of scientific psychiatric twin research: Luxenburger's 1928 “preliminary report on the psychiatric examination of a series of twins”. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 48(Suppl 1), S37S44. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbac110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., & Klee, A. (2022b). Hermann Hoffmann's 1921 monograph: “The offspring of endogenous psychoses: Genealogical-Characterological examinations”. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 48(Suppl 1), S20S27. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbac108CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., McGuire, M., Gruenberg, A. M., O'Hare, A., Spellman, M., & Walsh, D. (1993). The Roscommon Family Study. III. Schizophrenia-related personality disorders in relatives. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50(10), 781788. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8215802CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., McGuire, M., Gruenberg, A. M., & Walsh, D. (1995). Schizotypal symptoms and signs in the Roscommon Family Study. Their factor structure and familial relationship with psychotic and affective disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52(4), 296303. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7702446CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2023a). The impact of sex, age at onset, recurrence, mode of ascertainment and medical complications on the family genetic risk score profiles for alcohol use disorder. Psychological Medicine, 53(5), 17321740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Bacanu, S., Sundquist, J., Edwards, A. C., & Sundquist, K. (2023b). Differences in genetic risk score profiles for drug use disorder, major depression, and ADHD as a function of Sex, Age at onset, recurrence, mode of ascertainment, and treatment. Psychological Medicine, 53(8), 34483460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Lichtenstein, P., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2018). The genetic epidemiology of treated major depression in Sweden. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(11), 11371144. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17111251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Mościcki, E. K., Sundquist, J., Edwards, A. C., & Sundquist, K. (2023c). Genetic liability to suicide attempt, suicide death, and psychiatric and substance use disorders on the risk for suicide attempt and suicide death: A Swedish national study. Psychological Medicine, 53(4), 16391648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2019). Prediction of onset of substance-induced psychotic disorder and Its progression to schizophrenia in a Swedish national sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 176(9), 711719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2021a). Family genetic risk scores and the genetic architecture of major affective and psychotic disorders in a Swedish national sample. JAMA Psychiatry, 78, 735743. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2021.0336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2021b). The patterns of family genetic risk scores for eleven major psychiatric and substance use disorders in a Swedish national sample. Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 18. doi:10.1038/s41398-021-01454-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, J, & Sundquist, K. (2023d). The moderation of the genetic risk for alcohol and drug use disorders in a Swedish national sample by the genetic aptitude for educational attainment. Psychological Medicine, 53(7), 30773084.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Rosmalen, J. G. M., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2023e). A distinctive profile of family genetic risk scores in a Swedish national sample of cases of fibromyalgia, irritable bowel syndrome, and chronic fatigue syndrome compared to rheumatoid arthritis and major depression. Psychological Medicine, 53, 38793886. doi:10.1017/S0033291722000526CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Thacker, L., & Walsh, D. (1996). Self-report measures of schizotypy as indices of familial vulnerability to schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22(3), 511520. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8873301CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kety, S. S., Rosenthal, D., Wender, P. H., & Schulsinger, F. (1971). Mental illness in the biological and adoptive families of adopted schizophrenics. American Journal of Psychiatry, 128(3), 302306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köhler-Forsberg, O., Antonsen, S., Pedersen, C. B., Mortensen, P. B., McGrath, J. J., & Mors, O. (2023). Schizophrenia spectrum disorders in Denmark between 2000 and 2018: Incidence and early diagnostic transition. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 148(2), 190198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krebs, M.B., A. V., Hellberg, K.L.J., Ohlsson, H., Steinbach, J., Pedersen, E.M., iPSYCH Study Consortium, Werge, T., … Kendler, K.S. (2023). The relationship between genotype- and phenotype-based estimates of genetic liability to human psychiatric disorders, in practice and in theory. medRxiv, epub. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.06.19.23291606Google Scholar
Legge, S. E., Jones, H. J., Kendall, K. M., Pardiñas, A. F., Menzies, G., Bracher-Smith, M., … Hotopf, M. (2019). Association of genetic liability to psychotic experiences with neuropsychotic disorders and traits. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(12), 12561265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtenstein, P., Bjork, C., Hultman, C. M., Scolnick, E., Sklar, P., & Sullivan, P. F. (2006). Recurrence risks for schizophrenia in a Swedish national cohort. Psychological Medicine, 36(10), 14171425. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16863597CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ludvigsson, J. F., Andersson, E., Ekbom, A., Feychting, M., Kim, J. L., Reuterwall, C., … Olausson, P. O. (2011). External review and validation of the Swedish national inpatient register. BMC Public Health, 11, 450. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-450CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luxenburger, H. (1928). Vorlaufiger Bericht uder psychiatrische Serienuntersuchungen und Zwillingen. Zeitschrift für die gesamte neurologie und psychiatrie, 116(December 1928), 297326. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02892967CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luxenburger, H. (1939). Die Schizophrenie und ihr Erbkreis. In Boeters, H. (Ed.), Erbbiologie und Erbpathologie Nervöser und Psychischer Ƶustände und Funktionen (pp. 769872). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mas-Bermejo, P., Papiol, S., Via, M., Rovira, P., Torrecilla, P., Kwapil, T. R., … Rosa, A. (2023). Schizophrenia polygenic risk score in psychosis proneness. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 111.Google ScholarPubMed
McGorry, P. D., Hartmann, J. A., Spooner, R., & Nelson, B. (2018). Beyond the “at risk mental state” concept: Transitioning to transdiagnostic psychiatry. World Psychiatry, 17(2), 133142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrath, J., Saha, S., Welham, J., El, S. O., MacCauley, C., & Chant, D. (2004). A systematic review of the incidence of schizophrenia: The distribution of rates and the influence of sex, urbanicity, migrant status and methodology. BMC Medicine, 2, 13. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-2-13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrath, J. J., Saha, S., Al-Hamzawi, A., Andrade, L., Benjet, C., Bromet, E. J., … Kessler, R. C. (2016). The bidirectional associations between psychotic experiences and DSM-IV mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(10), 9971006. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15101293CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Organization, W. H. (1992). The IDC-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Prichard, J. C. (1837). A treatise on insanity and other disorders affecting the mind. Philadelphia: Haswell, Barrington, and Haswell.Google Scholar
Rosell, D. R., Futterman, S. E., McMaster, A., & Siever, L. J. (2014). Schizotypal personality disorder: A current review. Current Psychiatry Reports, 16, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rück, C., Larsson, K. J., Lind, K., Perez-Vigil, A., Isomura, K., Sariaslan, A., … Mataix-Cols, D. (2015). Validity and reliability of chronic tic disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder diagnoses in the Swedish National Patient Register. BMJ Open, 5(6), e007520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saha, S., Scott, J. G., Varghese, D., & McGrath, J. J. (2011). The association between general psychological distress and delusional-like experiences: A large population-based study. Schizophrenia Research, 127(1–3), 246251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sansone, R. A., & Sansone, L. A. (2011). Personality disorders: A nation-based perspective on prevalence. Innovations in Clinical Neuroscience, 8(4), 13.Google ScholarPubMed
SAS Institute, I. (2012). SAS/STAT® online documentation, version 9.4. Cary, N.C.: SAS Institute, Inc.Google Scholar
Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J., & Gibbon, M. (1979). Crossing the border into borderline personality and borderline schizophrenia. The development of criteria. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36(1), 1724. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/760694CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sundquist, J., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, K., & Kendler, K. S. (2017). Common adult psychiatric disorders in Swedish primary care (where most mental health patients are treated). BMC Psychiatry, 17(1), 235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torgersen, S., Kringlen, E., & Cramer, V. (2001). The prevalence of personality disorders in a community sample. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(6), 590596. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11386989CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vrieze, S. I. (2012). Model selection and psychological theory: A discussion of the differences between the Akaike information criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian information criterion (BIC). Psychological Methods, 17(2), 228243. doi:10.1037/a0027127CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang-James, Y., Chen, Q., Kuja-Halkola, R., Lichtenstein, P., Larsson, H., & Faraone, S. V. (2020). Machine-learning prediction of comorbid substance use disorders in ADHD youth using Swedish registry data. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(12), 13701379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zoghbi, A. W., Bernanke, J. A., Gleichman, J., Masucci, M. D., Corcoran, C. M., Califano, A., … Brucato, G. (2019). Schizotypal personality disorder in individuals with the Attenuated Psychosis Syndrome: Frequent co-occurrence without an increased risk for conversion to threshold psychosis. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 114, 8892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Comparison with the general population (SPD individuals matched to five controls (same year of birth, same sex, and same municipality (at time of SPD registration)

Figure 1

Figure 1. Mean (and 95% CIs) for the familial genetic risk score, depicted on the Y-axis, for schizophrenia (SZ), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), ADHD, and major depression (MD) and in cases of schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) and schizophrenia in the Swedish population born 1940–2000 by ICD-10 (for exact values of these results see online Supplementary appendix table 5). * FRGS scores for SPD and schizophrenia differ significantly.

Figure 2

Table 2. Results from three-class solution of the latent class analysis of cases of SPD

Figure 3

Table 3. Prediction of conversion of Schizotypal personality disorder to Schizophrenia

Figure 4

Figure 2. Survival curves for progression to a diagnosis of schizophrenia In cases of SPD with low, intermediate, and high genetic risk for Schizophrenia as calculated by a Cox model. The X-axis represents years since first diagnosis and the Y axis the Proportion who received a first diagnosis of schizophrenia.

Supplementary material: File

Kendler et al. supplementary material

Kendler et al. supplementary material
Download Kendler et al. supplementary material(File)
File 43.5 KB