Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T02:27:15.592Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Twinks, jocks, and bears—oh my! The stereotype content model extended to gay men and weight at work

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 March 2024

Joseph Alexander Carpini*
Affiliation:
Business School, Management & Organisations Department, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
Aleksandra Luksyte
Affiliation:
Business School, Management & Organisations Department, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
*
Corresponding author: Joseph Alexander Carpini; Email: Joseph.Carpini@uwa.edu.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Commentaries
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology

Adopting an intersectional lens, we extend the Best Practices for Weight-Based Research in Organizational Studies (Lemmon et al., Reference Lemmon, Jensen and Kuljanin2024) to research on gay men using the stereotype content model (Clausell & Fiske, Reference Clausell and Fiske2005). Gay men account for 4% of working adults internationally and, in Western countries, their workforce participation rates (64%) are higher than those of heterosexual men (50%; Carpini, Cox, et al., Reference Carpini, Cox, Gavin and Stone2023). Although some have argued sexual orientation is concealable, research suggests others can accurately identify the sexual orientation of a person in just 50 milliseconds (Rule & Ambady, Reference Rule and Ambady2008). In addition to the stereotypes associated with sexual orientation, gay men may be stigmatized and marginalized based on their weight because of increased scrutiny to their appearance (Austen et al., Reference Austen, Greenaway and Griffiths2020). This possibility is supported by research showing that gay men feel more judged based on their weight than their heterosexual counterparts (Fogarty & Walker, Reference Fogarty and Walker2022). As such, gay men may experience a “double whammy,” wherein they are stereotyped and discriminated against based on both their sexual orientation and weight. In this commentary, we will discuss how the intersectionality of sexual orientation and weight will make theoretical contributions with implications for gay men’s interpersonal experiences at work. In doing so, we integrate the stereotype content model (warmth and competence) with gay stereotypes based on an expanded understanding of weight that differentiates body compositions across two dimensions—thinness and muscularity—to elaborate theory about four gay archetypes (twinks, jocks, bears, and muscle bears).

Body ideals and gay subgroups

Industrial-organizational (I-O) psychologists have studied gay men as a homogenous group. An analysis of indexed articles published in seven leading I-O psychology journals (see Appendix for search details) suggests none of the 38 identified articles (of 25,297, or 0.002% total publications) considered weight heterogeneity within the gay community in their theorizing or analyses. Furthermore, in contrast to Lemmon et al.’s focal article that used weight and “fatness” synonymously, we adopt a nuanced perspective on weight (Baker & Florack, Reference Baker and Florack2021; Lemmon et al.) that differentiates two predominant body ideals among gay men: thinness and muscular ideals (Doyle & Engeln, Reference Doyle and Engeln2014). The thinness ideal reflects a desire for slenderness or a low body fat percentage and is most akin to the primary subject of discussion in Lemmon et al.’s (i.e., “fatness”) focal article. Yet, the muscular ideal corresponds to a desire to appear muscular with a higher muscle mass index. Thinness and muscularity are particularly pertinent to the gay community due to the 1980s AIDS epidemic. A physiological symptom of AIDS is weight loss and muscle wasting, which made thinness and muscularity salient physical characteristics for gay men. During the AIDS epidemic, low body fat percentages were normalized and greater muscle mass was desirable as indicators of perceived health (Austen et al., Reference Austen, Bonell and Griffiths2022; Fogarty & Walker, Reference Fogarty and Walker2022). Thinness and muscularity ideals are related to the body mass index, reflecting a weight to height proportion; yet the ideals are distinct because they emphasize striving toward a physical appearance of “either a slim and youthful body ideal or a heavier, hyper-masculine body ideal” (Doyle & Engeln, Reference Doyle and Engeln2014, p. 280).

Although the majority of research considers the two ideals separately (Fogarty & Walker, Reference Fogarty and Walker2022), we contend research on gay men can be advanced by simultaneously examining the two ideals in a 2 × 2 matrix (thinness vs muscularity: higher/lower). Consistent with this notion, some research examining various gay subcultural identities has pointed to four archetypes—twinks, jocks, bears, and muscle bears—corresponding to the various combination of thinness and muscularity ideals (Doyle & Engeln, Reference Doyle and Engeln2014). “Twinks” are characterized by being high on thinness and low on muscularity. “Jocks” are defined by their high level of both thinness and muscularity (Fogarty & Walker, Reference Fogarty and Walker2022). There are two types of “bears” that become relevant to the distinction of body composition as a function of physical ideals (McGrady, Reference McGrady2016). A “bear” is low both on thinness and muscularity; whereas a “muscle bear” is low on thinness and high on muscularity (Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Bourne and Lyons2022).

Integrating gay body ideals and the stereotype content model

Building on and extending our consideration of gay body ideals and their corresponding subcultural groups, we now turn to the implications of subgroup membership on perceptions of competence and warmth (Fiske et al., Reference Fiske, Cuddy, Glick and Xu2002). Competence reflects the extent to which individuals are perceived to be strong, independent, and assertive, qualities generally considered more masculine than feminine. Conversely, warmth captures interpersonal qualities such as kindness, supportiveness, and cooperativeness, and are perceived to be more feminine than masculine (Luksyte et al., Reference Luksyte, Unsworth and Avery2018). Consistent with research on in- and outgroup bias, gay men are excluded from the optimal high–high (e.g., high competence–high warmth) classification (Clausell & Fiske, Reference Clausell and Fiske2005). Such bias is important because perceptions of competence and warmth account for up to 80% of the variance in perceivers’ impressions of others (Carpini, Cox, et al., Reference Carpini, Cox, Gavin and Stone2023), contributing to both valuable work (e.g., favorable performance appraisal) and interpersonal outcomes (e.g., social inclusion; Carpini et al., Reference Carpini, Luksyte, Parker and Collins2023). Specifically, perceived lack of competence is related to direct discrimination such as unequal access to promotion, pay, and hiring; whereas perceived lack of warmth is more strongly related to interpersonal discrimination such as incivility, microaggressions, and hostility (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Walker, Hebl and King2013).

We argue integrating the dimensions of competence and warmth with the gay body ideals of thinness and muscularity will provide insight into differential experiences of gay men at work. With respect to thinness, previous research suggests overweight individuals are often penalized with lower ratings of competence and warmth due to attributions of laziness and lack of control over one’s dietary preferences (Lemmon et al.). Conversely, underweight men also receive lower ratings of competence but benefit from higher ratings of warmth due to this body composition being positively associated with perceived agreeableness and energy (Baker & Florack, Reference Baker and Florack2021). Research also suggests muscularity may have important implications for perceptions of competence and warmth. Specifically, high muscularity is positively related to indicators of competence such as being tough and disciplined while attracting lower ratings of warmth (Baker & Florack, Reference Baker and Florack2021).

Adopting an intersectional lens that accounts for both body ideals and sexual orientation simultaneously, we posit bears will experience the most negative evaluations with both competence and warmth being rated as lower, even compared to heterosexual men of the same body composition. We expect this effect as it will represent a double jeopardy of being both perceived as overweight and an outgroup member. As such, bears may experience both direct and interpersonal discrimination such as being denied promotion and experiencing heightened incivility from others. Twinks, as a function of their very high thinness and low muscularity, are likely to be perceived as moderately warm and lower on competence. We expect twinks to be evaluated lower on competence than equally thin heterosexual men yet higher on warmth because their body type is more feminine, corresponding to their sexual orientation (Clausell & Fiske, Reference Clausell and Fiske2005). Accordingly, twinks may experience more direct discrimination and less interpersonal mistreatment.

We expect muscle bears to be perceived as being moderately competent yet lower on warmth. Such expectations are likely to stem from the compensatory effect of mixed signals from being gay while also being high on muscularity and low on thinness. All of these mixed messages may cancel out negative evaluations associated with being overweight (Lemmon et al.; McGrady, Reference McGrady2016). As such, muscle bears may not be discriminated in terms of promotion opportunities, yet, they may become targets of interpersonal mistreatment. Finally, jocks are likely to be perceived as moderately high on both warmth and competence. We posit jocks will have the most positive evaluations of the gay archetypes because they will benefit from their muscularity to reinforce masculinity and competence, whereas their thinness and gay identity will lend itself to higher evaluations of warmth. We expect jocks to be evaluated less positively than “hunks,” the heterosexual equivalent, because jocks maintain their outgroup status as a sexual minority (Fogarty & Walker, Reference Fogarty and Walker2022). Hence, out of all other types, jocks are less likely to experience either direct or interpersonal discrimination.

Starting the gay-weight research agenda

Our analysis provides opportunities for future research on gay men, a sizeable working population, yet whose experiences at work are still understudied (Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Bourne and Lyons2022). Using an intersectional lens, we provide a nuanced yet powerful perspective on the experiences of gay men at work by incorporating thinness and muscularity ideals with the stereotype content model. With this said, our analysis is far from complete and future research can take it further. First, our extension only considered gay men from a Western perspective and as such future researcher could examine non-Western gay subgroups. For example, bears of an Asian decent may be described as panda bears, which denotes both their weight/body composition (bear) and broad ethnic group (Asian), reflecting an additional dimension of diversity. Second, there are subcultural appearance stereotypes among gay men that intersect not only with body composition but also with age. For example, twinks are most frequently associated with being slender and younger (Fogarty & Walker, Reference Fogarty and Walker2022); whereas polar bears are stereotypically older (McGrady, Reference McGrady2016). Third, research has highlighted the perceived fit between occupations and employee gay identity (Rule et al., Reference Rule, Bjornsdottir, Tskhay and Ambady2016). This research suggests some jobs are considered “gayer” (e.g., flight attendant, hairdresser) than others (e.g., engineer); and thus, gay men may experience more positive outcomes in occupations that are perceived to “fit” their sexual orientation (e.g., nursing, teaching humanities subjects) as opposed to occupations that are perceived to be a misfit (e.g., teaching STEM subjects, construction). Extending this line of research, how might twinks, jocks, bears, and muscle bears fare within different occupations that may or may not conform with occupational stereotypes? For example, will twinks experience less formal and interpersonal discrimination in jobs that are consistent with their body composition such as retail, as opposed to manual labor jobs that may be perceived as a misfit? Will bears experience the least formal and interpersonal discrimination in more masculine jobs like automotive repair and manual labor? Last, scholars should consider the experience of gay men across time as they may transition from one subcultural grouping to another. For example, how might the experience of twinks change as a function of increased muscle mass, thus occupying a new subcultural identity as a jock? How might the interpersonal experiences of a bear change as a function of a change in thinness (i.e., weight loss; Lemmon et al., Reference Lemmon, Jensen and Kuljanin2024)?

Spilling the tea—beyond the gays

Our extension of weight research at work to gay men could also function as the basis for research on other identities associated with different sexual orientations. First, gay men represent only one category of individuals within the broader LGBTQAI+ umbrella. For example, scholars could apply our theorizing to lesbian subgroups such as those identifying as “butch” (stereotypically portrayed as high (low) in agentic (feminine) terms such as strong, opinionated, abrasive) or “fem” (described in prototypical feminine adjectives including sensitive, nurturing, accommodating). Butch lesbians are stereotypically portrayed similarly to bears because they tend to be low on both thinness and muscularity. Despite these similarities, research suggests theoretical insights are not readily generalizable across gays and lesbians offering opportunities for nuanced theoretical development (Carpini, Cox, et al., Reference Carpini, Cox, Gavin and Stone2023). Second, scholars can extend our consideration of gay body composition subgroups beyond the rainbow community to heterosexuals. For example, thinness and muscularity are also prominent features of heterosexual men as evident in terms like “hunk” and “dad bod” (representing a combination of low muscle mass and moderate body fat percentage). Is it possible heterosexual men with dad bods may be perceived as most competent and warm (Carpini et al., Reference Carpini, Luksyte, Parker and Collins2023)? What are the interpersonal work outcomes for heterosexual men with dad bods and how might they differ from other body compositions?

Appendix—search strategy

Search terms: “gay*” OR “homosexual*” OR “sexual minorit*” in titles, abstracts, heading words, key words. Journals included in our search: Academy of Management Journal, Journal of Applied Psychology, Personnel Psychology, Journal of Management, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Journal of Organizational Behavior, and Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. Journals selected based on quality ratings (see Carpini et al., Reference Carpini, Parker and Griffin2017). We omitted five entries due to not being related to gay men (e.g., only discussed lesbians). We performed search on 23/08/2023 using PsycInfo database.

Data availability and deposit statement

All data used is publicly available using the instructions provided in the Appendix.

Funding statement

We gratefully acknowledge the UWA Business School Future Fund Research Grant.

Disclosure statement

None.

References

Austen, E., Bonell, S., & Griffiths, S. (2022). Fat is feminine: A qualitative study of how weight stigma is constructed among sexual minority men who use Grindr. Body Image, 42, 160172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.06.005.Google Scholar
Austen, E., Greenaway, K. H., & Griffiths, S. (2020). Differences in weight stigma between gay, bisexual, and heterosexual men. Body Image, 35, 3040. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.08.002.Google Scholar
Baker, A., & Florack, A. (2021). Uncovering men’s stereotype content (warmth and competence) associated with a representative range of male body size categories. Body Image, 37, 148161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2021.02.007.Google Scholar
Carpini, J. A., Cox, A., Gavin, M., & Stone, R. (2023). Managing diversity. In R. Stone, A. Cox, M. Gavin, & J.A. Carpini (Eds.), Human resource management (11thed.) Wiley.Google Scholar
Carpini, J. A., Luksyte, A., Parker, S. K., & Collins, C. G. (2023). Can a familiar gender stereotype create a not-so-familiar benefit for women? Evidence of gendered differences in ascribed stereotypes and effects of team member adaptivity on performance evaluations. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 44(4), 590605. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2702.Google Scholar
Carpini, J. A., Parker, S. K., & Griffin, M. A. (2017). A look back and a leap forward: A review and synthesis of the individual work performance literature. Academy of Management Annals, 11(2), 825885. https://doi.org/10.5465/annals.2015.0151.Google Scholar
Clausell, E., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). When do subgroup parts add up to the stereotypic whole? Mixed stereotype content for gay male subgroups explains overall ratings. Social Cognition, 23(2), 161181. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.23.2.161.65626.Google Scholar
Doyle, D. M., & Engeln, R. (2014). Body size moderates the association between gay community identification and body image disturbance. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 1(3), 279284. https://doi.org/10.1037/sgd0000049.Google Scholar
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878902. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878.Google Scholar
Fogarty, S. M., & Walker, D. C. (2022). Twinks, jocks, and bears, oh my! Differing subcultural appearance identifications among gay men and their associated eating disorder psychopathology. Body Image, 42, 126135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.05.010.Google Scholar
Franklin, J. D., Bourne, A., & Lyons, A. (2022). Characteristics and functions of subcultural identities in the lives of gay, bisexual, and queer-identifying men in Australia. Psychology and Sexuality, 13 (3), 459473. https://doi.org/10.1080/19419899.2020.1856172.Google Scholar
Lemmon, G., Jensen, J. M., & Kuljanin, G. (2024). Best practices for weight at work research. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 17(1), 85105.Google Scholar
Luksyte, A., Unsworth, K., & Avery, D. (2018). Innovative work behavior and sex-based stereotypes: Examining sex differences in perceptions and evaluations of innovative work behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 39(3), 292305. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2219.Google Scholar
McGrady, P. B. (2016). Grow the beard, wear the costume”: Resisting weight and sexual orientation stigmas in the bear subculture. Journal of Homosexuality, 63(12), 16981725 Google Scholar
Morgan, W. B., Walker, S. S., Hebl, M. R., & King, E. B. (2013). A field experiment: Reducing interpersonal discrimination toward pregnant job applicants. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(5), 799809. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034040.Google Scholar
Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2008). Brief exposures: Male sexual orientation is accurately perceived at 50 ms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 11001105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2007.12.001.Google Scholar
Rule, N. O., Bjornsdottir, R. T., Tskhay, K. O., & Ambady, N. (2016). Subtle perceptions of male sexual orientation influence occupational opportunities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101(12), 16871704. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000148.Google Scholar