Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T13:55:36.365Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Death of parent, sibling, spouse, and child in a Swedish national sample and risk of subsequent stress reaction, major depression, alcohol-use disorder, and drug-use disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 April 2023

Kenneth S. Kendler*
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Sara L. Lönn
Affiliation:
Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden
Jan Sundquist
Affiliation:
Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden Department of Family Medicine and Community Health, Department of Population Health Science and Policy, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA Center for Community-based Healthcare Research and Education (CoHRE), Department of Functional Pathology, School of Medicine, Shimane University, Matsue, Japan
Kristina Sundquist
Affiliation:
Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden Department of Family Medicine and Community Health, Department of Population Health Science and Policy, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA Center for Community-based Healthcare Research and Education (CoHRE), Department of Functional Pathology, School of Medicine, Shimane University, Matsue, Japan
*
Author for correspondence: Kenneth S. Kendler, E-mail: Kenneth.Kendler@vcuhealth.org
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

To determine, in a general population, how much rates of stress reactions (SR), major depression (MD), alcohol-use disorder (AUD) and drug-use disorder (DUD) increase after the death of close relatives.

Methods

SR, MD, AUD, and DUD registrations were assessed from national Swedish registries. From the population followed from 2000 to 2018, those exposed to death of a close relative in 2002–2016 were matched to unexposed controls and analyzed in males and females by a controlled pre-post design using a difference-in-difference method.

Results

Substantial, brief increases in risk for SR and more modest prolonged increases in MD were observed after death of relatives in both men and women greatest with children, followed by spouses, parents, and siblings. Relatively long-lasting modest increases in AUD but not DUD were also observed following death of relatives. The absolute increases for SR and MD were greater in females than males and for AUD greater in males than females. However, logistic regression analyses showed most effects did not differ significantly by sex. Consistently larger increases in disorder risk were seen with the death of younger v. older parents, siblings, and spouses and with accidental v. non-accidental death in children.

Conclusions

Applying a matched cohort design to Swedish population registries, death of close relatives was associated with, and likely caused, substantial increases in rates of SR, MD, and AUD, consistent with smaller prior clinical investigations. Through such registries, we can, in large representative samples, integrate the impact of exposures to selected environmental adversities into disorder risk pathways.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

A long tradition of research has examined the association between stressful life events (SLEs) and risk for episode onset or recurrence of psychiatric disorders, especially major depression (MD) (Brown & Harris, Reference Brown and Harris1978; Brown, Harris, & Hepworth, Reference Brown, Harris and Hepworth1995; Cohen, Murphy, & Prather, Reference Cohen, Murphy and Prather2019; Holmes & Rahe, Reference Holmes and Rahe1967; Kendler, Karkowski, & Prescott, Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1998; Kessler, Reference Kessler1997; Paykel et al., Reference Paykel, Myers, Dienelt, Klerman, Lindenthal and Pepper1969). A substantial proportion of this association appears to be causal (Kendler & Gardner, Reference Kendler and Gardner2010a; Kendler, Karkowski, & Prescott, Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999). Most, but not all, studies have also seen elevations in rates of alcohol use and alcohol-use disorder (AUD) (Boden, Fergusson, & Horwood, Reference Boden, Fergusson and Horwood2014; Gorman & Peters, Reference Gorman and Peters1990; Jennison, Reference Jennison1992; Keyes, Hatzenbuehler, & Hasin, Reference Keyes, Hatzenbuehler and Hasin2011; Lee, Young Wolff, Kendler, & Prescott, Reference Lee, Young Wolff, Kendler and Prescott2012; Perreira & Sloan, Reference Perreira and Sloan2001; Storr et al., Reference Storr, Reboussin, Green, Mojtabai, Susukida, Young and Crum2021) and drug use and drug-use disorder (DUD) (Hyman & Sinha, Reference Hyman and Sinha2009; Myers, McLaughlin, Wang, Blanco, & Stein, Reference Myers, McLaughlin, Wang, Blanco and Stein2014) after SLE exposure. Nearly all such studies have relied on questionnaire or interview-based retrospective assessments of SLEs with the more detailed interview-based measures recognized as producing more valid measures (Brown, Sklair, Harris, & Birley, Reference Brown, Sklair, Harris and Birley1973; Dohrenwend, Dohrenwend, Dodson, & Shrout, Reference Dohrenwend, Dohrenwend, Dodson and Shrout1984; Dohrenwend, Link, Kern, Shrout, & Markowitz, Reference Dohrenwend, Link, Kern, Shrout, Markowitz and Cooper1987; Dohrenwend, Raphael, Schwartz, & Skodol, Reference Dohrenwend, Raphael, Schwartz, Skodol, Goldberger and Breznitz1993; Paykel, Reference Paykel1983). However, such assessments are time consuming and thus typically available on only limited samples. Furthermore, problems of interpretation remain including accurate timing of events, memory biases that might favor event recall when followed by illness onset and reverse causation in which the disorder predisposes to selection into stressful environments.

In this report, we evaluate a different methodological approach to SLE research that has only rarely been employed (Kessing, Agerbo, & Mortensen, Reference Kessing, Agerbo and Mortensen2004; Li, Tsai, Chen, Liang, & Chen, Reference Li, Tsai, Chen, Liang and Chen2022): the use of national registries. We examine changes in the rates of registration, in national medical and criminal registries, of four psychiatric diagnoses [stress reactions (SR), MD, AUD, and DUD] before and after the death of a parent, sibling, spouse, or child. As an SLE, death in a close relative has the methodological advantages of being temporally discrete, non-recurrent, assessed with high accuracy, and reflecting a severe stressor (Brugha, Bebbington, Tennant, & Hurry, Reference Brugha, Bebbington, Tennant and Hurry1985; Holmes & Rahe, Reference Holmes and Rahe1967; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1998). Furthermore, prior work based on measures of grief intensity provide us with predictions that can validate our approach – that psychiatric reactions to death are more severe when the death (i) occurs unexpectedly (Ball, Reference Ball1977; Keyes et al., Reference Keyes, Pratt, Galea, McLaughlin, Koenen and Shear2014; Shear, Reference Shear2015), (ii) involves children compared to other close relatives (Leahy, Reference Leahy1993; Middleton, Raphael, Burnett, & Martinek, Reference Middleton, Raphael, Burnett and Martinek1998; Sanders, Reference Sanders1980; Shear, Reference Shear2015), and (iii) occurs in adult relatives prematurely, that is in young or middle age compared to late adult life (Ball, Reference Ball1977; Segal & Bouchard, Reference Segal and Bouchard1993). To maximize our ability to infer causal effects from our observational data, we utilize a controlled pre-post design analyzed using a difference-in-difference method (Lechner, Reference Lechner2010; Ohlsson & Kendler, Reference Ohlsson and Kendler2020).

We examine rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in exposed cases v. unexposed controls for 2 years before and after the death of a parent, sibling, spouse, or child. We evaluate the degree to which the pattern differs across disorders, sexes, and category of deceased relative. We then examine the validity of our method by comparing differences in rates of disorder in a matched cohort design as a function of the age of the deceased parent, sibling, or spouse and, for children, whether the death was accidental or not.

Methods

We linked nationwide Swedish registers via the unique 10-digit identification number assigned at birth or immigration to all Swedish residents. The identification number was replaced by a serial number to ensure confidentiality. For the sources used to create our dataset, see online Appendix Table 1.

We identified males and females who were exposed to the death of a close relative between 2002 and 2016. If exposed to death of a parent, sibling, or spouse, we constrained the population to individuals exposed after age 18. For death of child, we required that the child was under age 18. We only examined cases with the first death in each relative class. Using Swedish hospital, specialistic and primary care medical registers, which contain ICD codes for each hospital discharge and out-patient medical contact, we defined SR using ICD-10 code F43 and MD using ICD-10 codes: F32 and F33 and ICD-9 codes: 296B, 298A, and 300E. For the definitions of AUD and DUD, see online Appendix Table 2.

Statistical methods

In our matched cohort design, we compare the relative increase in risk for the exposed v. an unexposed group (Sjölander & Greenland, Reference Sjölander and Greenland2013). For our matching variables, see online Appendix Table 3. We next identified affected individuals in 3-month periods, before and after the relative death, comparing the exposed to the unexposed utilizing logistic regression in which the log odds depending linearly on time. For the exposed group, the time-period 2 to 1 year before the death is defined as baseline. During this time period, our model assumes a constant difference between the exposed and unexposed. One year before the event we estimate the increased risk, compared to the expected, in 3 month periods by including dummy variables for each time period. Our parameter estimates the increase in odds compared to what would have been expected if the exposed group would have had the same difference in risk compared to the unexposed group during the baseline period. In addition, the model, which is presented in online Appendix Table 4, allows for separate associations in males and females. We present the increase in risk for the exposed v. unexposed individuals as odds ratio (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs), together with a graphical illustration of the probabilities for the unexposed and exposed. The expected probabilities in the unexposed are represented by a dashed line. Because of the large number of tests, we only indicate the 1% and 0.1% significances in the figures. The significance of the sex differences or age of the death of the relative is not presented but instead indicated by filled in triangles in the figures representing men. Our main results are presented in Figs 1–3 which are also given with 95% CIs in online Appendix Tables 5 and 6.

Fig. 1. (a) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in male and female exposed cases and unexposed controls 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a parent of an exposed case. The x-axis represents the 24 months before (−) and the 24 months after (+) parental death. The zero point from which a dotted vertical black line emerges, represents the date of death in the parent of the exposed case. The y-axis represents the probability of registration for that disorder in the three months period. The solid red/black line and circles represent the estimated and observed rates, respectively, of disorder in the exposed individuals. Correspondingly, the brown/gray line and triangles represent the estimated and observed rates of the disorder in the unexposed individuals. The dotted red/black line is the extrapolated rates of illness in the exposed cases expected from the results of the exposed and unexposed individuals for the first 12 months of observation. Asterisks above the solid red/black line with circles indicate the presence of significant differences in the ORs in exposed v. unexposed individuals: *p < 0.01 and **p < 0.001. Significant differences in the exposed-unexposed disorder ORs between males and females (p < 0.01) are represented by filled in red/black circles in the results for males. See online Appendix Table 5 for numerical estimates of all the exposed-unexposded ORs with 95% CIs and differences between those ORs in males v. female. (b) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in male and female exposed and unexposed individuals 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a sibling of an index case. For other details, see legend to (a). (c) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in male and female exposed and unexposed 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a spouse of an exposed individual. For other details, see legend to (a). (d) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in male and female exposed and unexposed 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a child of an exposed case. For other details, see legend to (a).

Fig. 2. (a) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in exposed and unexposed individuals 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a parent of an index case at younger than age 65 v. at age 65 or older. The x-axis represents the 24 months before (−) and the 24 months after (+) parental death. The zero point from which a dotted vertical black line emerges, represents the date of death. The y-axis represents the probability of registration for that disorder in the prior 3 months. The solid red/black line with circles represents the rates of disorder in the exposed individuals and the brown/gray line with triangles in those that are unexposed. The dotted red/black line is the extrapolated rates of illness in the exposed individuals expected from the results of the exposed and unexposed for the first 12 months of observation. Asterisks above the solid red/black line with circles indicate the presence of significant differences in the ORs in exposed v. unexposed: *p < 0.01 and **p < 0.001. Significant differences in the exposed-unexposed ORs between individuals when the parent died <65 v. ≥65 (p < 0.01) are represented by filled in red/black circles in the results for those dying at ≥65. See online Appendix Table 6 for numerical estimates of all the exposed-unexposed ORs with 95% CIs and differences between those ORs in exposed individuals who lost relatives at a younger v. older age. (b) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in exposed and unexposed 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a sibling of an exposed individual at younger than age 65 v. at age 65 or older. Significant differences in the exposed-unexposed ORs between individuals when the sibling died <65 v. ≥65 (p < 0.01) are represented by filled in red/black circles in the results for those dying at ≥65. For other details, see legend to (a). (c) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in exposed and exposed individuals 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a spouse of an exposed individual at younger than age 65 v. at age 65 or older. Significant differences in the exposed-unexposed ORs between exposed individuals when the sibling died <65 v. ≥65 (p < 0.01) are represented by filled in red/black circles in the results for those dying at ≥65. For other details, see legend to (a).

Fig. 3. Rates of registration for SR and MD in exposed and unexposed individuals 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a child from accidental and non-accidental causes. The x-axis represents the 24 months before (−) and the 24 months after (+) parental death. The zero point from which a dotted vertical black line emerges, represents the date of death. The y-axis represents the probability of registration for that disorder in the prior 3 months. The solid red/black line with circles represents the rates of disorder in the exposed individual and the brown/gray line with triangles the unexposed. The dotted red/black line is the extrapolated rates of illness in the exposed individuals expected from the results of the exposed and unexposed for the first 12 months of observation. Asterisks above the solid red/black line with circles indicate the presence of significant differences in the ORs in exposed v. unexposed: *p < 0.01 and **p < 0.001. Significant differences in the exposed-unexposed ORs between exposed individuals when the child died from accidental v. non-accidental causes (p < 0.01) are represented by filled in red/black circles in the results for those dying of non-accidental causes. See online Appendix Table 6 for numerical estimates of all the exposed-unexposed ORs with 95% CIs and differences between those ORs in individuals who lost a child from an accidental v. non-accidental death.

Thereafter, we investigated if the age of the relative, or cause of death of the child could have an impact on the increase in risk. We divided the parents, siblings, and spouses into two groups: younger or older than 65 and the age of retirement in Sweden. Children were divided into those who died accidently, defined by ICD-10 as S or T diagnosis, and those who did not.

Results

The number of exposed cases in our sample who had a parent, sibling, spouse, or child who had died during our observation period varied widely, and was highest for parents, followed by spouses and siblings with much smaller numbers for children, which made it impossible to usefully compare rates of AUD and DUD in their parents (Table 1). Table 1 also provides the raw rates of SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in the two years before and after the death in our exposed and matched unexposed controls. In all figures, the significance of the differences, as assessed by ORs, are marked by asterisks above each result for cases while differences between the sexes or the age of the deceased are noted by solid triangles. Each circle represents the observed rate of illness over the preceding 3 months so that the circle at +3 months reflects rates for the first 3 months after the death of a relative. For further details, see figure legends.

Table 1. Descriptive results for death of close relative in matched case and control samples

Main findings

Parents

Figure 1a shows the risk for diagnoses of SR, MD, AUD, and DUD 2 years before and after a parental death, separately in males and female. In both men and women, rates of SR peak at +3 months. We see a much smaller absolute rise of risk in males v. females but because of differences in base rates, the ORs at +3 months were similar: 4.70 (4.58–4.82) in females and 4.52 (4.34–4.71) in males.

The increased risk for SR then declines rapidly in both sexes but remains significant for over 1 year. Both sexes show small but significant increases in SR rates in the year preceding the death. The only significant sex difference was an OR higher in females in the 3 months prior to parental death.

More modest increases in rates of MD are seen in both sexes beginning in the year prior to the death with a much slower decline than seen with SR. While the absolute rise is greater in females than males, no significant differences were seen in the observed ORs which maximized at +3 months in both sexes: 1.30 (1.26–1.33) in females and 1.30 (1.23–1.36) in males.

Rates of AUD had no sustained increase in risk in females after parental death while we see a modest significant increase in rates in men over most of the 2-year follow-up period with a maximal OR of 1.10 (1.06–1.15) at +9 months. For DUD, we see no sustained increase associated with parental death in either sex but a small but significant increase at +6 and +9 months.

Siblings

As seen in Fig. 1b, the prevalence for our four disorders after death of a sibling in males and females closely resembles that seen in parents with (i) a sharp rise of risk for SR with ORs of 5.16 (4.89–5.45) in females and 5.35 (4.89–5.84) in males that declines quickly, (ii) a slower more sustained rise in risk for MD with ORs maximizing at 1.30 (1.23–1.36) in females and 1.28 (1.20–1.37) in males, (iii) a sustained modest significant increased risk for AUD in males [peak OR 1.24 (1.17–1.32)] but not females, and (iv) no durable increase in either sex for DUD. As with death of parent, the absolute level of increase for SR and MD was substantially greater in females, the ORs of the two sexes differed significantly only at one time point (−3 months for SR).

Spouse

The increase in risk for SR and MD after death of spouse is substantially greater than that observed after the death of a parent or sibling. A sharp rise of SR starts at −3 months, maximizing at +3 months. While the absolute rates at both time points are quantitatively greater in females than males, the ORs are significantly higher in males than females. At +3 months, these equal to, respectively [24.19 (21.03–27.82)] and [18.34 (15.98–21.04)]. Rates of SR then decline rapidly.

The rise of risk for MD also begins well before the spousal death, is slower in its rise and decline and of more modest magnitude than seen with SR. The ORs are significantly greater in males than females for 2 years – from −3 to +21 months. For example, at +3 months, the ORs are respectively, 3.08 (2.86–3.33) and 2.31 (2.13–2.52).

Both males and females demonstrate a significantly increased risk for AUD after death of their spouses lasting throughout our observation period. The absolute value of the increase in AUD risk is consistently greater in men than women and the ORs are significant greater in men over nearly the entire follow-up period. For both sexes, the risk is highest at +3 months and equals 1.79 (1.57–2.03) in females and 2.45 (2.18–2.74) in males. Both sexes demonstrate a short-lived modest significantly increased risk for DUD after spousal death.

Child

The increases in risk for SR and MD after death of a child were substantially higher than those observed with deaths of parents, siblings, or spouses. For SR, we see a sharp and very large increase in mothers maximizing at +3 months with an OR of 31.3 (26.17–37.43). The absolute increase in males was similar in shape but smaller while the ORs at +3 months were non-significantly larger: 42.26 (32.37–55.16).

For MD, we see, in females, a more slowly rising peak maximizing at +6 months with an OR of 5.27 (4.35–6.38) and a subsequent fall off in risk over the follow-up period. For males, the risk rise was smaller in magnitude and maximized at +3 months with an OR of 4.51 (3.49–5.82). The ORs did not differ significantly across females and males for either SR or MD.

Validation

The increased risks for both SR and MD were substantially and significantly reduced in individuals after the death of a parent, sibling, or spouse who was 65 or older v. under age 65 (Fig. 2a–2c).

Rates of AUD were appreciably lower after the death of an older v. younger spouse, but these resulting ORs were not significantly different at our 1% threshold. The rates of both SR and MD were lower for parents who lost their child from non-accidental v. accidental deaths, but only the differences in SR were statistically significant.

Discussion

We sought, in these matched cohort pre-post analyses, to examine the association between death of close relatives and rates of registration for four psychiatric diagnoses in the Swedish general adult population. Furthermore, we sought to determine differences in the magnitude of the changes in rates across (i) disorders, (ii) class of relative, and (iii) the sex of the exposed case. We review these in turn.

We observed systematic differences in the rates of our four disorders immediately prior to and after the death of a relative that were stable across the kind of deceased relative and the sex of the exposed individual. SR had the most immediate and largest rise in rates and, typically, the most rapid decline. MD showed a slower and smaller increase in risk that tended to be longer-lasting. When present, the increase of AUD was modest to moderate in magnitude but persistent. Finally, we saw little evidence of a sustained significant increase in DUD after the death of a close relative.

In most of our analyses, the rise in disorder risk preceded the death of the relative, particularly in spouses. This rise likely resulted from several causes including severe illnesses, injuries, and care-giving burdens that were stressful for the exposed case as well as anticipatory loss. As predicted by this hypothesis, no elevation in the rates of SD and MD were seen prior to accidental death of children.

We also found large differences in the magnitude of the increase in disorder after the death of our four classes of relatives. The peak ORs for SR in women after the death of a parent, sibling, spouse, and child, equaled, respectively, 4.70, 5.16, 16.71, and 31.30. The maximal OR for MD after the death of these four classes of relatives in males equaled 1.27, 1.28, 2.97, and 4.51, respectively. Our results, particularly with respect to rates of SR and MD, are consistent with the prior literature on grief intensity, suggesting that on average, in adulthood, the affective bonds and resulting intensity of bereavement on loss, are strongest with children, followed by spouses, and then parents and siblings (Leahy, Reference Leahy1993; Middleton et al., Reference Middleton, Raphael, Burnett and Martinek1998; Sanders, Reference Sanders1980; Shear, Reference Shear2015).

For males, the maximum OR for AUD was considerably higher after the death of a spouse (2.45) than after the death of a parent or sibling (1.10, 1.24), with a similar pattern of ORs seen in females: 1.79, 1.12, and 1.16. The much greater risk in AUD risk after spousal death may arise because, in addition to the emotional stress of the loss, the death of a spouse also means the disappearance of the well-established protective effect of the presence of spouses against rates of risky drinking and alcohol abuse (Leonard & Rothbard, Reference Leonard and Rothbard1999; Umberson, Reference Umberson1992). Indeed, in Sweden that state of marriage is substantially protective against risk for AUD (Kendler, Lonn, Salvatore, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Lonn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2016a) while divorce substantially increases AUD risk (Kendler, Lonn, Salvatore, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Lonn, Salvatore, Sundquist and Sundquist2017).

The interpretation of our findings on sex differences in the rates of disorders after death of close relatives is a function of the scale of measurement for which there is no right or wrong answer (Kendler & Gardner, Reference Kendler and Gardner2010b; Rothman, Greenland, & Lash, Reference Rothman, Greenland and Lash2008). Assuming an additive model, we see, after death in a close relative, substantially greater increases in rates of SR and MD in women v. men and AUD in men v. women. However, if we assume a multiplicative scale of measurement, as assessed by ORs, we see relatively few sex differences, the most striking of which is the greater rise in rates of MD and AUD in males v. females after the death of their spouse. These results may be due to the greater mental health marriage benefit obtained by men v. women (Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, Reference Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton2001; Umberson, Reference Umberson1992).

We next validated our method by showing, consistent with prior evidence that the psychological reaction to death in adult relatives is stronger if the death occurs prematurely (24, 29), that rates of SR and MD were significantly higher when parents, siblings and spouses died under v. over age 65. In agreement with prior studies, we also showed higher rates of SR and MD in parents when their children died by accidental v. non-accidental causes (Ball, Reference Ball1977; Keyes et al., Reference Keyes, Pratt, Galea, McLaughlin, Koenen and Shear2014; Shear, Reference Shear2015).

Finally, we combined a pre-post and matched cohort design analyzed by a difference-in-difference method to maximize our power for causal inference (Ohlsson & Kendler, Reference Ohlsson and Kendler2020). While we cannot rule out a role for hidden confounders, the consistency of our findings and the power of our statistical approach suggest that the observed associations between increased rates of SR, MD, and AUD and the death of close relatives are largely causal in nature.

Our results have one straight forward clinical implication. Following the death of a close relative, survivors are at high risk for a range of psychiatric outcomes. Brief interventions by health professionals may help to insure these conditions are not sustained and impairing in the future.

Limitations

Six potentially important limitations of this work should be considered. First, its value depends on the quality of diagnoses in Swedish medical registries, which has been rather widely studied and supported (Ludvigsson et al., Reference Ludvigsson, Andersson, Ekbom, Feychting, Kim, Reuterwall and Olausson2011). The validity of MD diagnoses is supported by its prevalence, sex ratio, sibling, and twin correlations and associations with well-documented psychosocial risk factors (Kendler, Ohlsson, Lichtenstein, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Lichtenstein, Sundquist and Sundquist2018; Sundquist, Ohlsson, Sundquist, & Kendler, Reference Sundquist, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Kendler2017). We have also shown that the genetic profile of cases of MD is similar for those ascertained in primary care, specialist, and in-patient settings (Kendler, Ohlsson, Bacanu, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Bacanu, Sundquist and Sundquist2022). The validity of our definitions of AUD and DUD are reinforced by the high rates of concordance for ascertainment across registries (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Sundquist, Ohlsson, Palmer, Maes, Winkleby and Sundquist2012, Reference Kendler, Ji, Edwards, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015), and the similarity of genetic epidemiological findings for AUD and DUD in Sweden compared to those in other samples (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Sundquist, Ohlsson, Palmer, Maes, Winkleby and Sundquist2012, Reference Kendler, Ji, Edwards, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015, Reference Kendler, PirouziFard, Lonn, Edwards, Maes, Lichtenstein and Sundquist2016b; Kendler, Maes, Sundquist, Ohlsson, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Maes, Sundquist, Ohlsson and Sundquist2013). We know of no work evaluating the validity of SR diagnoses in Sweden. We explored the effects of death of relatives on the SR ICD subtypes (see online Appendix Table 7) and found significant elevations in rates in exposed v. unexposed individual across all subtypes, being highest for F430 (acute stress reaction) and F439 (Reaction to severe stress, unspecified) and lowest in F438 (Other reactions to severe stress).

Second, we could only study cases of SR and MD coming to clinical attention (while cases of AUD and DUD were also detected through criminal registries). While Sweden has a nationalized medical system with minimal barriers to care, it is likely that a proportion of individuals with SR or depressive syndromes did not seek medical care. The ORs we found for death of relatives and MD, which ranged from 1.27 for siblings of males to 5.27 for children of females, are generally lower than those obtained from retrospective interview-based studies (e.g. Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1998). This discrepancy could have resulted from a downward bias in our estimates due to missing untreated cases and/or upward biases from selective recall and/or mood-biased recall (Bower, Reference Bower1981). However, an upward bias on our ORs might arise if death of a relative increased physician attendance of the exposed case, thereby providing chances for a diagnosis of SR, MD, AUD, or DUD.

Third, while our main analyses did not distinguish recurrent from incident cases, we present such results in online Appendix Figs 1–4. The main patterns of our findings are seen in both subgroups, although, the estimated effects are generally larger in incident v. recurrent cases. This is likely due to part to the much lower incidence rates for our disorders in the unexposed controls.

Fourth, while death is typically a temporally discrete event, the process of dying can differ widely across individuals and present to their relative's variable stressors and care-giving burdens. Evidence for this is seen in the frequent elevation of rates of our disorders in exposed cases preceding the death of their relative.

Fifth, while our assessment of the death of relatives is objective and accurately dated, our registry data do not permit us to assess a number of dimensions of the loss that require respondent report, such as long-term contextual threat developed by Brown which has been shown to robustly predict rates of subsequent depressive episodes (Brown & Harris, Reference Brown and Harris1989; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1998).

Finally, in examining potential psychiatric syndromes after the death of loved ones, concern is appropriate regarding discrimination between a psychiatric illness and normal grief. In using diagnoses coded by thousands of Swedish physicians, we have no control over this diagnostic distinction. This concern is greatest for SR which are described in ICD-10

…as maladaptive responses to severe or continued stress, in that they interfere with successful coping mechanisms and therefore lead to problems of social functioning. (Organization, 1992)

Conclusions

Applying a matched cohort design to the Swedish population registries, death of close relatives was associated with, and likely contributed causally to, substantial increases in rates of SR, MD, and AUD, consistent with smaller prior clinical investigations. While our field has been able to ‘scale-up’ ascertainment of psychiatric disorders using national registries and large-scale biobanks, it has proven more difficult to obtain, in large representative population samples, high-quality measures of environmental adversities, especially those, like SLEs, occurring in adulthood in temporal proximity to disorder onset or recurrence. We tried to address this limitation by showing that we can accurately measure, in a large representative population, one class of severe SLEs – death of close relatives – which precede and likely cause substantial increases in rates of SR, MD, and AUD. Importantly, this approach eliminates the problem of biased reporting and selective recall which limits the interpretation of most prior SLE studies utilizing self-report. While individually often severe, however, death of relatives reflects, in most human populations, only a moderate proportion of the total adversities experienced in a lifetime. The next challenge is to extend this work to other objectively defined SLEs so that together they can be used, along with familial/genetic and other social/environmental risk factors, to develop, in high-quality registry samples, more realistic and comprehensive etiologic models for important psychiatric and substance-use disorders. In summary, our results suggest that it is feasible, in large representative samples, to integrate into risk disorder pathways that include genetic, social, and early developmental adversities, the major impact of exposures to selected environmental adversities of adulthood.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723000570.

Financial support

This project was supported by grants R01AA023534 and R01DA030005 from the National Institutes of Health and from the Swedish Research Council (2020-01175).

Conflict of interest

None of the authors have any conflicts of interest to declare.

References

Ball, J. F. (1977). Widow's grief: The impact of age and mode of death. OMEGA-Journal of Death and Dying, 7(4), 307333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boden, J. M., Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2014). Associations between exposure to stressful life events and alcohol use disorder in a longitudinal birth cohort studied to age 30. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 142, 154160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129148. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7224324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. O. (1978). Social origins of depression: A study of psychiatric disorder in women. London: Tavistock.Google Scholar
Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. O. (1989). Life events and illness (G. W. Brown & T. O. Harris Eds.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Brown, G. W., Harris, T. O., & Hepworth, C. (1995). Loss, humiliation and entrapment among women developing depression: A patient and non-patient comparison. Psychological Medicine, 25(1), 721. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7792364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, G. W., Sklair, F., Harris, T. O., & Birley, J. L. (1973). Life-events and psychiatric disorders. 1. Some methodological issues. Psychological Medicine, 3(1), 7487. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/4692493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brugha, T., Bebbington, P., Tennant, C., & Hurry, J. (1985). The list of threatening experiences: A subset of 12 life event categories with considerable long-term contextual threat. Psychological Medicine, 15(1), 189194. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3991833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., Murphy, M. L., & Prather, A. A. (2019). Ten surprising facts about stressful life events and disease risk. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dohrenwend, B. P., Link, B. G., Kern, R., Shrout, P., & Markowitz, J. (1987). Measuring life events: The problem of variability within categories. In Cooper, B. (Ed.), Psychiatric epidemiology: Progress and prospects (pp. 103119). London: Croom Helm. (Reprinted from: Not in File).Google Scholar
Dohrenwend, B. P., Raphael, K., Schwartz, S., & Skodol, A. (1993). The structured event probe and narrative rating method (SEPRATE) for measuring stressful life events. In Goldberger, L. & Breznitz, S. (Eds.), Handbook of stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects (2nd ed., pp. 174199). New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Dohrenwend, B. S., Dohrenwend, B. P., Dodson, M., & Shrout, P. E. (1984). Symptoms, hassles, social supports, and life events: Problem of confounded measures. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93(2), 222230. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6725756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorman, D. M., & Peters, T. J. (1990). Types of life events and the onset of alcohol dependence. British Journal of Addiction, 85(1), 7179. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2310856.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social readjustment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11(2), 213218. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6059863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyman, S. M., & Sinha, R. (2009). Stress-related factors in cannabis use and misuse: Implications for prevention and treatment. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 36(4), 400413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jennison, K. M. (1992). The impact of stressful life events and social support on drinking among older adults: A general population survey. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 35(2), 99123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Gardner, C. O. (2010a). Dependent stressful life events and prior depressive episodes in the prediction of major depression: The problem of causal inference in psychiatric epidemiology. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(11), 11201127. doi:67/11/1120 [pii];10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Gardner, C. O. (2010b). Interpretation of interactions: Guide for the perplexed. British Journal of Psychiatry, 197(3), 170171. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.110.081331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Ji, J., Edwards, A. C., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2015). An extended Swedish national adoption study of alcohol use disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(3), 211218. doi:2088151 [pii];10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Karkowski, L. M., & Prescott, C. A. (1998). Stressful life events and major depression: Risk period, long-term contextual threat, and diagnostic specificity. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 186(11), 661669. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9824167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Karkowski, L. M., & Prescott, C. A. (1999). Causal relationship between stressful life events and the onset of major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(6), 837841. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10360120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Lonn, S. L., Salvatore, J., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2016a). Effect of marriage on risk for onset of alcohol use disorder: A longitudinal and co-relative analysis in a Swedish national sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(9), 911918. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15111373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Lonn, S. L., Salvatore, J., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2017). Divorce and the onset of alcohol use disorder: A Swedish population-based longitudinal cohort and co-relative study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(5), 451458. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16050589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Maes, H. H., Sundquist, K., Ohlsson, H., & Sundquist, J. (2013). Genetic and family and community environmental effects on drug abuse in adolescence: A Swedish national twin and sibling study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(2), 209217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Bacanu, S., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2022). Differences in genetic risk score profiles for drug use disorder, major depression, and ADHD as a function of sex, age at onset, recurrence, mode of ascertainment, and treatment. Psychological Medicine, 113. doi:10.1017/S0033291721005535.Google ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Lichtenstein, P., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2018). The genetic epidemiology of treated major depression in Sweden. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(11), 11371144. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17111251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., PirouziFard, M., Lonn, S., Edwards, A. C., Maes, H. H., Lichtenstein, P., … Sundquist, K. (2016b). A national Swedish twin-sibling study of alcohol use disorders. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 19(5), 430437. doi:S1832427416000621 [pii];10.1017/thg.2016.62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Sundquist, K., Ohlsson, H., Palmer, K., Maes, H., Winkleby, M. A., & Sundquist, J. (2012). Genetic and familial environmental influences on the risk for drug abuse: A national Swedish adoption study. Archive of General Psychiatry, 69(7), 690697. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessing, L. V., Agerbo, E., & Mortensen, P. B. (2004). Major stressful life events and other risk factors for first admission with mania. Bipolar Disorders, 6(2), 122129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C. (1997). The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 191214. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9046559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keyes, K. M., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., & Hasin, D. S. (2011). Stressful life experiences, alcohol consumption, and alcohol use disorders: The epidemiologic evidence for four main types of stressors. Psychopharmacology, 218(1), 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keyes, K. M., Pratt, C., Galea, S., McLaughlin, K. A., Koenen, K. C., & Shear, M. K. (2014). The burden of loss: Unexpected death of a loved one and psychiatric disorders across the life course in a national study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(8), 864871.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127(4), 472503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leahy, J. M. (1993). A comparison of depression in women bereaved of a spouse, child, or a parent. OMEGA-Journal of Death and Dying, 26(3), 207217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lechner, M. (2010). The estimation of causal effects by difference-in-difference methods. Foundations and Trends in Econometrics, 4(3), 165224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, L. O., Young Wolff, K. C., Kendler, K. S., & Prescott, C. A. (2012). The effects of age at drinking onset and stressful life events on alcohol use in adulthood: A replication and extension using a population-based twin sample. Alcohol Clinical Experimental Research, 36(4), 693704. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01630.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, K. E., & Rothbard, J. C. (1999). Alcohol and the marriage effect. Journal of Studies on Alcohol Supplement, 13, 139146. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10225498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, D.-J., Tsai, S.-J., Chen, T.-J., Liang, C.-S., & Chen, M.-H. (2022). Risks of major mental disorders after parental death in children, adolescents, and young adults and the role of premorbid mental comorbidities: A population-based cohort study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 57(12), 23932400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ludvigsson, J. F., Andersson, E., Ekbom, A., Feychting, M., Kim, J. L., Reuterwall, C., … Olausson, P. O. (2011). External review and validation of the Swedish national inpatient register. BMC Public Health, 11, 450. doi:1471-2458-11-450 [pii];10.1186/1471-2458-11-450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Middleton, W., Raphael, B., Burnett, P., & Martinek, N. (1998). A longitudinal study comparing bereavement phenomena in recently bereaved spouses, adult children and parents. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 32(2), 235241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, B., McLaughlin, K. A., Wang, S., Blanco, C., & Stein, D. J. (2014). Associations between childhood adversity, adult stressful life events, and past-year drug use disorders in the national epidemiological study of alcohol and related conditions (NESARC). Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28(4), 1117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohlsson, H., & Kendler, K. S. (2020). Applying causal inference methods in psychiatric epidemiology: A review. JAMA Psychiatry, 77(6), 637644. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.3758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Organization, W. H. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Paykel, E. S. (1983). Methodological aspects of life events research. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 27(5), 341352. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6668560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paykel, E. S., Myers, J. K., Dienelt, M. N., Klerman, G. L., Lindenthal, J. J., & Pepper, M. P. (1969). Life events and depression. A controlled study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 21(6), 753760. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/5389659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perreira, K. M., & Sloan, F. A. (2001). Life events and alcohol consumption among mature adults: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 62(4), 501508. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11513228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothman, K. J., Greenland, S., & Lash, T. L. (2008). Modern epidemiology (3rd ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
Sanders, C. M. (1980). A comparison of adult bereavement in the death of a spouse, child, and parent. OMEGA-Journal of Death and Dying, 10(4), 303322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segal, N. L., & Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (1993). Grief intensity following the loss of a twin and other relatives: Test of kinship genetic hypotheses. Human Biology, 65, 87105.Google ScholarPubMed
Shear, M. K. (2015). Complicated grief. New England Journal of Medicine, 372(2), 153160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sjölander, A., & Greenland, S. (2013). Ignoring the matching variables in cohort studies – When is it valid and why? Statistics in Medicine, 32(27), 46964708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storr, C. L., Reboussin, B. A., Green, K. M., Mojtabai, R., Susukida, R., Young, A. S., … Crum, R. M. (2021). Stressful life events and transitions in problematic alcohol use involvement among US adults. Substance Use & Misuse, 56(14), 21712180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sundquist, J., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, K., & Kendler, K. S. (2017). Common adult psychiatric disorders in Swedish primary care (where most mental health patients are treated). BMC Psychiatry, 17, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Umberson, D. (1992). Gender, marital status and the social control of health behavior. Social Science & Medicine, 34(8), 907917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. (a) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in male and female exposed cases and unexposed controls 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a parent of an exposed case. The x-axis represents the 24 months before (−) and the 24 months after (+) parental death. The zero point from which a dotted vertical black line emerges, represents the date of death in the parent of the exposed case. The y-axis represents the probability of registration for that disorder in the three months period. The solid red/black line and circles represent the estimated and observed rates, respectively, of disorder in the exposed individuals. Correspondingly, the brown/gray line and triangles represent the estimated and observed rates of the disorder in the unexposed individuals. The dotted red/black line is the extrapolated rates of illness in the exposed cases expected from the results of the exposed and unexposed individuals for the first 12 months of observation. Asterisks above the solid red/black line with circles indicate the presence of significant differences in the ORs in exposed v. unexposed individuals: *p < 0.01 and **p < 0.001. Significant differences in the exposed-unexposed disorder ORs between males and females (p < 0.01) are represented by filled in red/black circles in the results for males. See online Appendix Table 5 for numerical estimates of all the exposed-unexposded ORs with 95% CIs and differences between those ORs in males v. female. (b) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in male and female exposed and unexposed individuals 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a sibling of an index case. For other details, see legend to (a). (c) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in male and female exposed and unexposed 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a spouse of an exposed individual. For other details, see legend to (a). (d) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in male and female exposed and unexposed 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a child of an exposed case. For other details, see legend to (a).

Figure 1

Fig. 2. (a) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in exposed and unexposed individuals 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a parent of an index case at younger than age 65 v. at age 65 or older. The x-axis represents the 24 months before (−) and the 24 months after (+) parental death. The zero point from which a dotted vertical black line emerges, represents the date of death. The y-axis represents the probability of registration for that disorder in the prior 3 months. The solid red/black line with circles represents the rates of disorder in the exposed individuals and the brown/gray line with triangles in those that are unexposed. The dotted red/black line is the extrapolated rates of illness in the exposed individuals expected from the results of the exposed and unexposed for the first 12 months of observation. Asterisks above the solid red/black line with circles indicate the presence of significant differences in the ORs in exposed v. unexposed: *p < 0.01 and **p < 0.001. Significant differences in the exposed-unexposed ORs between individuals when the parent died <65 v. ≥65 (p < 0.01) are represented by filled in red/black circles in the results for those dying at ≥65. See online Appendix Table 6 for numerical estimates of all the exposed-unexposed ORs with 95% CIs and differences between those ORs in exposed individuals who lost relatives at a younger v. older age. (b) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in exposed and unexposed 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a sibling of an exposed individual at younger than age 65 v. at age 65 or older. Significant differences in the exposed-unexposed ORs between individuals when the sibling died <65 v. ≥65 (p < 0.01) are represented by filled in red/black circles in the results for those dying at ≥65. For other details, see legend to (a). (c) Rates of registration for SR, MD, AUD, and DUD in exposed and exposed individuals 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a spouse of an exposed individual at younger than age 65 v. at age 65 or older. Significant differences in the exposed-unexposed ORs between exposed individuals when the sibling died <65 v. ≥65 (p < 0.01) are represented by filled in red/black circles in the results for those dying at ≥65. For other details, see legend to (a).

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Rates of registration for SR and MD in exposed and unexposed individuals 2 years before and 2 years after the death of a child from accidental and non-accidental causes. The x-axis represents the 24 months before (−) and the 24 months after (+) parental death. The zero point from which a dotted vertical black line emerges, represents the date of death. The y-axis represents the probability of registration for that disorder in the prior 3 months. The solid red/black line with circles represents the rates of disorder in the exposed individual and the brown/gray line with triangles the unexposed. The dotted red/black line is the extrapolated rates of illness in the exposed individuals expected from the results of the exposed and unexposed for the first 12 months of observation. Asterisks above the solid red/black line with circles indicate the presence of significant differences in the ORs in exposed v. unexposed: *p < 0.01 and **p < 0.001. Significant differences in the exposed-unexposed ORs between exposed individuals when the child died from accidental v. non-accidental causes (p < 0.01) are represented by filled in red/black circles in the results for those dying of non-accidental causes. See online Appendix Table 6 for numerical estimates of all the exposed-unexposed ORs with 95% CIs and differences between those ORs in individuals who lost a child from an accidental v. non-accidental death.

Figure 3

Table 1. Descriptive results for death of close relative in matched case and control samples

Supplementary material: File

Kendler et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 705.5 KB