Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-05T12:45:58.735Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Populist Attitudes among Teenagers: How Negative Relationships with Socialization Agents Are Linked to Populist Attitudes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 April 2024

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

While the origins and consequences of populist attitudes in adults are being studied extensively, it is still unknown when populist attitudes might emerge in a person’s life. Drawing on the existing literature on political socialization, we focus on populist attitudes during adolescence and explore the contributing role of negative relationships with parents, peers, and teachers. We provide the first comprehensive analysis of populist attitudes among a representative sample of children aged 12 to 18 (mean: 14.66 years) using a unique dataset gathered through interviews conducted in schools in Austria, Germany, and Switzerland (n=3,123). Our findings reveal a strong association between perceived unjust teacher behavior and the level of populist attitudes in adolescents, while the connection of peers and parents with populist attitudes appears to be limited. Further analyses using panel data from the UK support these findings.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of American Political Science Association

The impact of populism on democracy has been debated heavily in recent years (Rovira Kaltwasser Reference Rovira Kaltwasser2012). While some scholars consider populism as a redemptive force (Canovan Reference Canovan1999) that may be able to correct some representational deficits of democracy (Kriesi Reference Kriesi2020; Manow Reference Manow2020), others pointed towards the incompatibility between the populist conception of a general will and elements of liberal democracies like pluralism, freedom of speech, and political compromise (Müller Reference Müller2016; Urbinati Reference Urbinati2019). On the individual level, populist citizens often support general democratic principles, but they criticize democratic practices and the working methods in everyday politics (Pappas Reference Pappas2019; Zaslove et al. Reference Zaslove, Geurkink, Jacobs and Akkerman2021). Thus, such “dissatisfied democrats” (Rovira Kaltwasser and Van Hauwaert Reference Rovira Kaltwasser and Van Hauwaert2020) could, for instance, more easily be approached by certain political entrepreneurs to support illiberal policy solutions (Wuttke, Schimpf, and Schoen Reference Wuttke, Schimpf and Schoen2023).

The most widespread ideational approach defines populism as a thin-centered ideology that is grounded in a Manichean perception of good versus evil, in which evil elites conspire against the pure and good people, and where the foundation of political decision-making should be based on the general will (volonté générale) of the people (Mudde Reference Mudde2004). “The elite” is often used as a vaguely specified empty signifier and can refer to political actors, economic leaders, journalists, or bureaucrats and the like (Jagers and Walgrave Reference Jagers and Walgrave2007).Footnote 1 On the individual level, populist attitudes constitute a non-compensatory concept that consists of multiple components, such as anti-elitism, people-centrism, and a Manichean worldview (Castanho Silva et al. Reference Castanho Silva, Andreadis, Anduiza, Blanuša, Morlet Corti, Delfino, Rico, Ruth-Lovell, Spruyt, Steenbergen, Littvay, Hawkins, Carlin, Littvay and Kaltwasser2018). Citizens are therefore considered as populist only if they endorse all three dimensions at the same time (Wuttke, Schimpf, and Schoen Reference Wuttke, Schimpf and Schoen2020).

Some scholars argue that populist attitudes can be conceived as dispositions that remain dormant until they become activated by external triggers like corruption or misrepresentation (Hawkins, Rovira Kaltwasser, and Andreadis Reference Hawkins, Carlin, Littvay and Kaltwasser2020)—hence, they might be relatively stable over time (Ardag et al. Reference Ardag, Silva, Thomeczek, Bandlow-Raffalski and Littvay2020). Other scholars showed however, that there may be variations in populist attitudes as response to external stimuli (Rhodes-Purdy, Navarre, and Utych Reference Rhodes-Purdy, Navarre and Utych2021; Rico, Guinjoan, and Anduiza Reference Rico, Guinjoan and Anduiza2020). Most recent evidence highlights though, that both may be true for different parts of the population (Schimpf, Wuttke, and Schoen Reference Schimpf, Wuttke and Schoen2023).

It is however unclear when and how populist attitudes are exactly formed. Based on the existing literature on populism, it is often argued that the development of populist attitudes results from an interaction of the individual with the political process. Thus, intentional policy failures of elites, for instance through a mismatch between responsiveness and responsibility of political actors, may cause citizens to form populist attitudes (Hawkins, Rovira Kaltwasser, and Andreadis Reference Hawkins, Carlin, Littvay and Kaltwasser2018, Reference Hawkins, Kaltwasser and Andreadis2020). Following this, populist attitudes should begin to form at the time adolescents start to interact with the political system, that is, around voting age.

In a different view, the primacy model of political socialization suggests that political attitudes are malleable until early adulthood and become increasingly stable afterwards (see Deth, Abendschön, and Vollmar Reference Deth, Abendschön and Vollmar2011; Dinas Reference Dinas2013; Stoker and Jennings Reference Stoker and Jennings2008). It is therefore not surprising that we find already large gaps in political involvement at very young age (Abendschön and Tausendpfund Reference Abendschön and Tausendpfund2017; Cesarini, Johannsson, and Oskarsson Reference Cesarini, Johannesson and Oskarsson2014; Deth, Abendschön, and Vollmar Reference Deth, Abendschön and Vollmar2011; Jennings, Stoker, and Bowers Reference Jennings, Stoker and Bowers2009; Prior Reference Prior2019). Thus, we should expect that populist attitudes can potentially develop already before children make first contact with the act of voting, that is, during their teenage years.

We argue that adolescence is a pivotal phase for receptivity to populist ideas. Adolescents go through tremendous cognitive and socio-cognitive growth throughout this period, allowing them to think about political problems in more abstract and complex ways. During this time, the role of environmental variables—notably the family, peers, and school—is crucial and these proximal environments are critical in forming youth political orientation because they are where young people first meet politics. But while some origins of populist attitudes like grievances or emotions are probably working in the same way for adults and adolescents, others are likely to differ. In particular, a low sense of political efficacy and a perceived lack of representation—that is, the perceived lack of will or the inability of political actors to respond to individual or social grievances, a perception of having little say in politics, and a sense of injustice—are often a central motivator for the development of populist attitudes (Castanho Silva and Wratil Reference Castanho Silva and Wratil2023; Geurkink et al. Reference Geurkink, Zaslove, Sluiter and Jacobs2020; Hawkins, Rovira Kaltwasser, and Andreadis Reference Hawkins, Kaltwasser and Andreadis2020; Spruyt, Keppens, and Van Droogenbroeck Reference Spruyt, Keppens and Van Droogenbroeck2016). However, adolescents often lack experience with the political system that is necessary to form the perception of a representation gap. Thus, we argue that socialization agents function as entities or authorities in accordance with which adolescents can generate populist attitudes. If adolescents perceive themselves as being treated unjustly by their teachers or if they have few possibilities to bring in their opinion in their circle of friends or at home, they can also be likely to develop populist attitudes.

In this study, we investigate whether and how socialization agents contribute to the formation of populist attitudes among adolescents. In particular, we ask whether negative relationships with parents, peers, and teachers are associated with having stronger populist attitudes. The study contributes to our understanding of the formation of populist attitudes as it is the first to explore its pervasiveness and correlates among a representative sample of adolescents (n=3,123, meanage=14.66 years) in multiple countries using a unique dataset gathered through interviews conducted in schools throughout Austria, Germany, and Switzerland. The results indicate that perceived unjust teacher behavior is strongly and positively associated with populist attitudes among children, whereas the role of peers is more limited and non-existent for parents. Furthermore, we find that the association between teacher perception and populist attitudes might increase with age. Additional evidence from UK panel data supports these findings.

Socialization Processes and the Formation of Populist Attitudes

In a developmental perspective, the teenage years are characterized by biological, emotional, and social changes that can challenge existing structures and identities (Hurrelmann and Quenzel Reference Hurrelmann and Quenzel2019; National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine 2019). Without having had any previous experience with the political system themselves, children and adolescents often already have attitudes toward social and political issues. According to Weisberg (Reference Weisberg1980), three different models of political socialization can be distinguished, depending on the age phase. First, the primacy model, according to which political values are imparted as early as infancy. Here, fundamental political orientations and values are learned in the family, which can structure later political action and thinking. Second, the intermediate-period model, which takes place during adolescence. As cognitive development progresses, political concepts are now comprehended and, for example, an understanding of political engagement is developed. Third, the recency model, which holds that even after adolescence, cumulative experiences with politics are made and political thinking and behavior can change. Following this intermediate model of political socialization, children are able to develop affective connections to socio-political objects early in life, for example in the form of party identification (Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Converse, Miller and Stokes1960), whereas cognitive aspects mature during later stages of socialization.

In the absence of the opportunity to draw on their own experience, political socialization is the primary process for developing political beliefs and behaviors via social interactions and collaboration with others, such as parents, peers, and adult role models (Flanagan Reference Flanagan2013). Thus, political socialization can be understood as the individual process of learning patterns corresponding to one’s own societal position as mediated through various societal agents (Hyman Reference Hyman1959).

The teenage years and young adulthood are further characterized as “impressionable years” meaning that adolescents’ (political) attitudes are particularly malleable through personal experiences or political events due to cognitive changes and development and the search for identity and community (Deth, Abendschön, and Vollmar Reference Deth, Abendschön and Vollmar2011; Dinas Reference Dinas2013; Ghitza, Gelmand, and Auerbach Reference Ghitza, Gelman and Auerbach2023; Stoker and Jennings Reference Stoker and Jennings2008). While adolescents often find themselves in a trade-off between the individual development of a persona and, for example, the need for attachment to their peers, a pluralist society does not provide instructions how to cope with such conflict. Thus, we expect that the groundwork for the development of populist attitudes—which are characterized, among others, by a clearly structured black-and-white worldview (Manicheanism)—can already be laid during adolescence and thus be influenced by political socialization processes. Since political attitudes become more stable over the life-course (Bacovsky and Fitzgerald Reference Bacovsky and Fitzgerald2023; Denny and Doyle Reference Denny and Doyle2009; Firebaugh and Chen Reference Firebaugh and Chen1995; Plutzer Reference Plutzer2002; Russo and Stattin Reference Russo and Stattin2017), it is particularly important to uncover the mechanisms that lead to the development of populist attitudes in early life.

Previous research has identified three main factors that contribute towards the development of populist attitudes. First, (subjective) status loss, a poor socio-economic position, and relative deprivation have long been linked to stronger support for populist parties (for example, Gidron and Hall Reference Gidron and Hall2017; Pettigrew Reference Pettigrew2017). Although these mechanisms have been mainly reported for adults (Spruyt, Keppens, and Van Droogenbroeck Reference Spruyt, Keppens and Van Droogenbroeck2016), we believe that they also hold for adolescents, as there is strong evidence for the link between parental socio-economic status and adolescents’ political attitudes (Akee et al. Reference Akee, Copeland, Holbein and Simeonova2020; Holbein Reference Holbein2017). Second, emotions like anxiety and anger are seen as not only shaping political attitudes in general (Marcus Reference Marcus2000, Reference Marcus2022; Marcus et al. Reference Marcus, Valentino, Vasilopoulos and Foucault2019) but also specifically shaping the support for populist parties (Rico, Guinjoan, and Anduiza Reference Rico, Guinjoan and Anduiza2017; Salmela and von Scheve Reference Salmela and von Scheve2017). Finally, a low sense of political efficacy and a perceived lack of representation are often a central motivator for the development of populist attitudes (Castanho Silva and Wratil Reference Castanho Silva and Wratil2023; Geurkink et al. Reference Geurkink, Zaslove, Sluiter and Jacobs2020; Hawkins, Rovira Kaltwasser, and Andreadis Reference Hawkins, Carlin, Littvay and Kaltwasser2020; Spruyt, Keppens, and Van Droogenbroeck Reference Spruyt, Keppens and Van Droogenbroeck2016). In most cases this pertains to a lack of will or the inability of political actors to respond to individual or social grievances, a perception of having little say in politics, and a sense of injustice (Betz Reference Betz, Hawkins, Carlin, Littvay and Kaltwasser2019). Populist tendencies can thus be put into action through poor democratic governance and purposeful policy failure by political elites. Previous research identified the major forces behind such a trend in the form of widespread systemic corruption in developing countries or growing conflicts between political elites’ accountability and responsiveness in developed countries, as they grow more ideologically distant from their constituents (Hawkins et al. Reference Hawkins, Carlin, Littvay and Kaltwasser2018, Hawkins, Rovira Kaltwasser, and Andreadis Reference Hawkins, Kaltwasser and Andreadis2020). This frequently results in the emergence of political players who start to draw attention to the flaws in democracy as it exists now, but whose power is ultimately curbed by democratic actors through political discussion and decision-making. Populist rhetoric ultimately draws on these worries and offers clear friend-and-foe images, making it easier for vulnerable individuals to perceive themselves as part of the supposedly “good” group of the pure people and thus regain some feelings of control over their situation (for example, Fritsche et al. Reference Fritsche, Jonas, Ablasser, Beyer, Kuban, Manger and Schultz2013; Greenaway et al. Reference Greenaway, Haslam, Cruwys, Branscombe, Ysseldyk and Heldreth2015; Hogg and Gøtzsche Reference Hogg, Gøtzsche, Forgas, Crano and Fiedler2021; Widmann Reference Widmann2021; Wirz Reference Wirz2018).

However, while we believe that the influence of deprivation and emotions is quite similar among adolescents and adults, we assume that a perceived lack of political efficacy and representation develops differently. Although the connection between responsibility and responsiveness can be made for adults who regularly engage with the political process, this does not apply for adolescents who are at the verge of developing a political consciousness. In particular, adolescents often lack the political knowledge and experience necessary to form the perception of a representation gap that develops after years of experience with the political system (see further Easton and Dennis Reference Easton and Dennis1969; Huber, Jankowski, and Wegscheider Reference Huber, Jankowski and Wegscheider2023). Thus, we believe that socialization agents like parents, peers, and teachers play an important role (Koskimaa and Rapeli Reference Koskimaa and Rapeli2015; Quintelier Reference Quintelier2015). If adolescents perceive themselves as being treated unjustly by their teachers or if they have few possibilities to bring in their opinion in their circle of friends or at home, they can be likely to develop populist attitudes, too. Previous research has shown, for instance, that the assessment of the fairness of teacher behavior, particularly in terms of relational and procedural justice, has an influence on the legitimacy of teachers’ authority as well as the assessment of institutional authorities outside of school (Gouveia-Pereira et al. Reference Gouveia-Pereira, Vala, Palmonari and Rubini2003). Others found that even preschool-aged kids are already attentive to procedural justice in addition to distributive justice (Grocke, Rossano, and Tomasello Reference Grocke, Rossano and Tomasello2015). Hence, we focus on the influence of socialization agents on populist attitudes.

Our study is the first to assess the pervasiveness of populist attitudes among adolescents along with specific child-related correlates. To our knowledge, previous studies either surveyed young adults who are at the end or way beyond the impressionable years or they study other facets such as right-wing radicalism or extremism that were post-hoc deemed as aspects of populism. Thus, youth populist thinking, so far, is considered inconsistent and varies by context and time, especially in the context of voting behavior (Noack and Eckstein Reference Noack and Eckstein2023). For instance, right-wing populist parties are gaining more and more votes from young people, as can be seen in the 2022 elections in both France and Italy. Relatedly, Westheimer (Reference Westheimer2019) demonstrated that in the United States that about one-quarter of young adults consider democracy a (very) poor form of government and prefer a “strong leader.” In Germany, the Shell study showed that almost one-quarter of 12- to 25-year-olds are open to populist ideas and 9% are even open to national populists views based on a measure of populist attitudes that wrongly mixes anti-EU sentiments with xenophobia and Islamophobia (Shell Deutschland Holding GmbH Reference GmbH2019).

In summary, while populism is not exclusive to youth, research suggests that adolescence is a critical period for the susceptibility to populist ideas. During this time, adolescents experience significant cognitive and socio-cognitive development, allowing them to think about political issues in more abstract and nuanced ways. While the role of adolescents and the stability of political views in adulthood should not be disregarded, the influence of environmental factors—particularly the family, peers and school—cannot be overlooked. These proximal contexts play a crucial role in shaping youth’s political positioning and are where young individuals first encounter politics (Hatemi and Ojeda Reference Hatemi and Ojeda2021; Jennings, Stoker, and Bowers Reference Jennings, Stoker and Bowers2009).

The Role of Parents in the Political Socialization Process

Existing research emphasizes parents as one of the main political socialization agents. Children spend most of their time with their parents, who have a high impact on their daily lives. In this context, studies focus, for instance, on the influence of parents on the formation of party identification (Kroh and Selb Reference Kroh and Selb2009), political ideology (Van Ditmars Reference Van Ditmars2023; Weiss Reference Weiss2023), right-wing extremist attitudes (Oepke Reference Oepke2005), and political participation (Verba, Schlozman, and Burns Reference Verba, Schlozman, Burns and Zuckerman2005). Parents can influence the development of their children’s political orientations both through the way they treat their children and through their socioeconomic status (Jungkunz and Marx Reference Jungkunz and Marx2024; Neundorf and Smets Reference Neundorf and Smets2017; Prior Reference Prior2019). Existing studies show that parents with higher socioeconomic status are more likely to have children with higher education (Neundorf and Smets Reference Neundorf and Smets2017). The children’s level of education, in turn, influences their political interest and knowledge. In addition, parental socioeconomic status contributes to the emergence of class-specific political orientations (Jennings, Stoker, and Bowers Reference Jennings, Stoker and Bowers2009).

Intergenerational transmission by parents can be more overt or more subtle. Social learning theory (Bandura Reference Bandura1977) argues that parents act as role models and thus foster parent-child coherence in political attitudes and values (Gniewosz, Noack, and Buhl Reference Gniewosz, Noack and Buhl2009). Existing research also shows that successful transmission in this way relies on the child’s correct perception of parental values and attitudes (Knafo and Schwartz Reference Knafo and Schwartz2004). Parents exert a substantial influence on their children’s political awareness and engagement. Research suggests that highly politicized parents have the potential to foster positive civic orientations, encouraging their children’s active participation in politics (Beck and Jennings Reference Beck and Jennings1982). Successful transmission of political values occurs more frequently when families maintain a politicized environment, as consistent political signals are provided by parents (Jennings et al. Reference Jennings, Stoker and Bowers2009). Furthermore, parents as role models lead to the imitation and adoption of political behaviors and attitudes (Dryer Reference Dryer1998).

However, such a one-step approach ignores how the family environment and the characteristics of the parent or child influence the transmission process. Following current research, transmission occurs in two steps instead. Children must first perceive their parents’ political attitudes and then decide whether to adopt or reject the perceived orientation for their own position (Hatemi and Ojeda Reference Hatemi and Ojeda2021). According to this view, transmission is a function of both the parent and the child, since neither the correct perception nor the adoption of parental orientations alone reflects actual transmission. At the same time, this view also implies that transmission can bypass the child’s perception and thus occur unintentionally, implicitly, or indirectly, and is overall a reciprocal process as opposed to a one-way transmission (Hatemi and Ojeda Reference Hatemi and Ojeda2021).

Existing research on the conditioning factors of this transmission process shows that the transmission of opinions, attitudes, or values from parents to their children can be more subtle than assumed in social learning theory (Baumrind Reference Baumrind, Brooks-Gunn, Lerner and Petersen1991). Central to this is the style of interaction between parent and child (Oepke Reference Oepke2005). Mediation plays an important role, and it is shown that a successful transmission between parent and child is promoted by authoritative education (balance between discipline and nurturance), while authoritarian education is detrimental to it (Weiss Reference Weiss2023). The authoritarian parenting style is characterized by a lack of warmth and high levels of demands and control, where parents have high expectations but are unresponsive to their children’s needs (Kılıçkaya, Uçar, and Nazhgül Reference Kılıçkaya, Uçar and Nazlıgül2023). Interactions with authority figures in the family, especially parents, thus play an important role. As a result, intergenerational transmission appears to be more than simply adopting parental views.Footnote 2 These attitudes are also shaped by the style of face-to-face interactions (Edwards Reference Edwards2004; Torney-Purta, Richardson, and Barber Reference Torney-Purta, Richardson and Barber2004). Strict and authoritarian parenting styles, characterized by strict rules and controls, have been shown to promote, for example, political alienation (Gniewosz, Noack, and Buhl Reference Gniewosz, Noack and Buhl2009) and right-wing extremist attitudes (Oepke Reference Oepke2005) in adolescence. We thus assume that experiences with strict authorities in terms of negative and non-reciprocal relationships with parents undermine the development of a positive bond between the individual and the broader social world, that is, the institutions of society and the representatives of the political system. This impression of a low position in the power structure may subsequently lead to the development of populist sentiments, mainly in the sense of an anti-elitist attitude. Thus, we form the following hypothesis:

H1: A negative, non-reciprocal parent-child relationship is associated with higher levels of populist attitudes.

The Role of Peers in the Political Socialization Process

Peers play a significant role in the political socialization of adolescents and young adults, as they engage in discussions on socio-political issues, share popular culture, and develop a set of values, whether they are common or opposing (Neundorf and Smets Reference Neundorf and Smets2017). The constant interaction and presence of peers in the lives of young people provide a platform for the formation of opinions and the development of political skills (Verba, Schlozman, and Brady Reference Verba, Schlozman and Brady1995). Through these discussions, adolescents are exposed to diverse perspectives, which contribute to their understanding of democratic principles and economic concepts such as the exchange of goods, services, and information (Quintelier Reference Quintelier2015).

It is worth considering the diverse settings in which peer interactions occur. Peer interactions take place within various contexts, including friendship networks, classmates, and other institutional settings such as associations or clubs. Each of these settings may exert distinct influences on political socialization. For instance, peer discussions within friendship networks, characterized by close and personal connections, may foster an environment conducive to open and honest political exploration. On the other hand, interactions within institutional settings, like clubs or associations, may introduce specific group dynamics and norms that shape political attitudes in a different manner (Quintelier Reference Quintelier2015).

Meta-analytic evidence further shows that peer (and parental) support is associated with stronger critical reflection and external political efficacy among adolescents (Heberle, Rapa, and Farago Reference Heberle, Rapa and Farago2020). Positive peer experiences amplify this effect, fostering a more comprehensive and nuanced understanding of socio-political landscapes. On the contrary, negative peer relationships could thus result in a lower likelihood to engage in or pay attention to complex discussions and a higher receptiveness for a simplified portrayal of socio-political issues (that is, Manicheanism).

Strong ties with classmates not only foster emotional and psychological well-being but also provide a conducive environment for political exploration. Adolescents, within this secure environment, are empowered to delve into and develop their political beliefs, using these relationships as a blueprint for future interactions both within and beyond the school premises (Noack and Eckstein Reference Noack and Eckstein2023). Furthermore, the presence of peers inherently introduces a set of social norms. These norms play a pivotal role in determining the behavior and attitudes of each individual within the group (Cochran and Brassard Reference Cochran and Brassard1979). By understanding and reflecting upon their interactions with peers, individuals can gain valuable insights into broader societal dynamics and discern their position within this larger social structure. However, not all peer interactions are affirming. Negative treatment from peers can be internalized by individuals, leading them to perceive themselves as holding a lower position in the social hierarchy. Such perceptions might cultivate feelings of being marginalized or subordinate to others, potentially leading to resentment or skepticism towards perceived opinion leaders or authority figures. With this in mind, we assume that:

H2: A negative relationship with peers is associated with higher levels of populist attitudes.

The Role of Teachers in the Political Socialization Process

Young people spend a significant part of the day at school, where they gain experience over many years (Abdelzadeh, Zetterberg, and Ekman Reference Abdelzadeh, Zetterberg and Ekman2015). In addition to the task of teaching knowledge and skills, schools also enable interpersonal experiences. From research on street-level bureaucracy we know that state institutions serve as places where the population comes into direct contact with lived political structures and policies and that the experiences made there have an impact on political attitudes (Ariely Reference Ariely2013; Bruch and Soss Reference Bruch and Soss2018; Lipsky Reference Lipsky1980; Shore and Tosun Reference Shore and Tosun2019). Individuals’ treatment by authorities in these institutions affects their recognition of their own standing and value in society, subsequently influencing their long-term expectations of procedural justice by the state (Weiss and Parth Reference Weiss and Parth2023). Existing studies show that these interpersonal experiences in the sense of relational justice positively impact both liberal democratic orientation and trust in formal institutions (Resh and Sabbagh Reference Resh and Sabbagh2014; Pretsch and Ehrhardt-Madapathi Reference Pretsch and Ehrhardt-Madapathi2018).

Experiencing fair treatment in school thus promotes liberal democratic attitudes among students, while unfair treatment can lead to the delegitimization of teachers and the school as a whole (Chory-Assad Reference Chory-Assad2002; Chory-Assad and Paulsel Reference Chory-Assad and Paulsel2004; Parth et al. Reference Parth, Weiss, Firat and Eberhardt2020; Torney-Purta, Wilkenfeld, and Barber Reference Torney-Purta, Wilkenfeld and Barber2008). Through everyday experiences at school, young people learn a “hidden curriculum” of positions and power (Bruch and Soss Reference Bruch and Soss2018). Based on social learning theory, schools can thus be perceived as a kind of miniature society within which students learn sociopolitical processes on a small scale (Bandura Reference Bandura1977; Kiess Reference Kiess2022; Noack and Eckstein Reference Noack and Eckstein2023; Wray-Lake Reference Wray-Lake2019). Teachers thereby represent the primary agent of reward and punishment (Cherng Reference Cherng2017; Resh and Sabbagh Reference Resh and Sabbagh2014). Positive engagement with teachers, who serve as models of authority, can cultivate a sense of empowerment, civic duty, and respect for societal norm. Such constructive interactions potentially lay the foundation for adolescents to develop non-populist attitudes, fostering a sense of trust in established institutions and promoting nuanced, collaborative dialogue over simplistic, divisive rhetoric. In contrast, teachers who abuse their power can harm students’ social development, particularly their attitudes towards reciprocity and society’s institutions (Pretsch and Ehrhardt-Madapathi Reference Pretsch and Ehrhardt-Madapathi2018). Previous research has shown that negative school experiences have detrimental effects. Bruch and Soss (Reference Bruch and Soss2018) discovered that negative encounters with school authorities decrease political engagement and trust among young people. This highlights the influence of school experiences on shaping perceptions of democratic society (Resh and Sabbagh Reference Resh and Sabbagh2014).

Schools serve as tangible representations of the state, providing students with insights into how public institutions operate and how they can anticipate treatment from authorities. These formative experiences during childhood and early adolescence shape individuals’ perceptions and can have long-lasting effects into adulthood (Bruch and Soss Reference Bruch and Soss2018). Therefore, equal treatment and interpersonal interactions in school are crucial for the development of civic identity (Resh and Sabbagh Reference Resh and Sabbagh2014). From research on adults, we know that populist attitudes arise from the interaction of the individual with the state (Hawkins et al. Reference Hawkins, Carlin, Littvay and Kaltwasser2018; Hawkins, Rovira Kaltwasser, and Andreadis Reference Hawkins, Kaltwasser and Andreadis2020). At this point, teachers represent the executive state and thus make the state tangible for young people, whereby, the behavior of individual teachers results in significant factors for political socialization (Koskimaa and Rapeli Reference Koskimaa and Rapeli2015; Quintelier Reference Quintelier2015). Unfair treatment by teachers, respectively by the state, could thus foster anti-elitist attitudes. Therefore, we assume that if children perceive unfair treatment by their teachers, they may be more inclined to develop populist attitudes:

H3: A negative teacher-child relationship is associated with higher levels of populist attitudes.

Research Design

We conducted a survey of adolescents aged 12 to 18 (mean: 14.66 years, SD: 1.25) in the Lake Constance area, spanning schools in Austria (n=1,523), Germany (n=356), and Switzerland (n=1,244).Footnote 3 All surveys were taken in class electronically and submitted anonymously by each student online through the LimeSurvey platform.Footnote 4 The fieldwork period ranged from fall 2019 to early March 2020 in Eastern Switzerland, from March to June 2020 in Western Austria (Vorarlberg), and from September to December 2020 in Southern Germany (Baden-Württemberg). Whereas the Austrian and Swiss samples were part of larger nation-wide studies with representative sampling strategies (Quenzel and Böheim-Galehr Reference Quenzel and Böheim-Galehr2021; Beck and Ha Reference Beck and Ha2018), the German sample was conducted independently and could not be stratified based on representative quotas due to the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic. Overall, the sample is 52% female and 32% have a migration background.Footnote 5

Our main dependent variable, populist attitudes, was measured through a six-item version of the Castanho Silva et al. (Reference Castanho Silva, Andreadis, Anduiza, Blanuša, Morlet Corti, Delfino, Rico, Ruth-Lovell, Spruyt, Steenbergen, Littvay, Hawkins, Carlin, Littvay and Kaltwasser2018) battery on five-point Likert scales (refer to online appendix A for question wording and summary statistics). The question battery includes two items each on anti-elitism, people-centrism, and the perception of a Manichean worldview (table 1), which showed good internal coherence, cross-national validity, and external validity across countries (Castanho Silva et al. Reference Silva, Bruno, Helbling and Littvay2020). Since populist attitudes are considered a non-compensatory concept (Wuttke, Schimpf, and Schoen Reference Wuttke, Schimpf and Schoen2020) whereby individuals need to hold high values on all three dimensions (i.e., anti-elitism and people-centrism and Manichean worldview) at the same time to be regarded as populist, an additive index is inadequate for operationalization. Instead, we first calculated a mean index for each dimension which ranged from zero (do not agree at all) to four (fully agree) and then multiplied all three dimensions with each other. This way, respondents who score high on populist attitudes have high values on all three dimensions (see also Jungkunz, Fahey, and Hino Reference Jungkunz, Fahey and Hino2021). For robustness checks, we also provide fit statistics from confirmatory factor analyses in the online appendix (table A.2). The results indicate a good to very good fit in all countries. Furthermore, we reran all analyses using alternative aggregation methods (mean index and minimum value across dimensions) in online appendix A. The results are basically similar to the ones presented in the main text.

Table 1 Question wording for populist attitudes

As for independent variables, we use the relationship with different socialization agents: parents, peers, and teachers. For parental support we use one item asking “All in all, how much of a say do you have at home?” with response options on a five-point scale from very little to very much. Since this item is somewhat less precise in capturing the concrete experiences at home, we use two items as additional robustness checks that asked about whether “My parents don’t care how I do in school” and “My parents don’t have time to care about my school” with response options on four-point scales from fully agree to fully disagree.Footnote 6 Unfortunately, these two items were only asked in Austria and Germany which is why we present the results in the online appendices. For peer relationship, we use four items asking about how children are doing in class and their experiences with classmates: “I am treated badly by my classmates,” “I am alone in the breaks,” “When I make mistakes, I am made fun of by others,” and “My classmates stand by me when it matters.” We combined the responses from four-point Likert scales (fully agree to fully disagree) through a mean index (α = 0.653). For teachers, we use three items asking for how many of their teachers the following sentences were true: “I feel I am treated fairly,” “I am graded fairly,” and “Other students are treated better than I.” The responses were recorded on four-point scales from (almost) none to (almost) all. We combined the three items through a mean index (α = 0.628).

Finally, we added further control variables to our models. Migration background was constructed based on either the children or their parents being born in a country outside Austria, Germany, Liechtenstein, and Switzerland. To account for different aspiration levels of schools across countries, we categorized the education level of children’s schools into those that grant access to university and those that do not. Similarly, we measure parental education, as a dummy which is one if at least one of both parents has a university degree. For material deprivation we use the Family Affluence Scale (FAS III) from the Health Behaviour in School-Aged Children (HBSC) study of the World Health Organization (WHO), which is a sum score of family wealth based on six items asking about the number of cars, computers, and bathrooms a family owns, the child having their own room, the family possessing a dishwasher, and the number of vacations abroad during the past year (Inchley et al. Reference Inchley, Currie, Budisavljevic, Torsheim, Jåstad, Cosma, Kelly and Arnarsson2020). The total score ranges from zero (low wealth) to ten (high wealth). We reversed the total score so that higher scores indicate higher deprivation. We further control for sex, age, and country-level differences (using country dummies). Finally, since data collection was impacted by the COVID-19 pandemic, we add a dummy if the interview took place after the first lockdown in the respective country.

Analytically, we perform a series of multilevel regression models to adjust for the nested data structure (students nested in classes). In addition, we can investigate the association of class level averages with individual level attitudes. Indeed, we may assume that little support between classmates or highly negative teacher perception in general is linked to populist attitudes. To account for these possibilities, we calculated class averages of teacher evaluation and peer evaluation and added them to the models. All models are estimated as random slope models, that is, we allow the intercepts of the dependent variable (populist attitudes) and the effects of the independent variables of interest on populist attitudes to vary between classes. To ease interpretation, we centered age around the mean and rescaled all other continuous variables to a scale from zero to ten in all models.

Finally, we emphasize our findings by using panel data from the British Household Panel Study (BHPS) and the UK Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS) (University of Essex, Institute for Social and Economic Research 2023). We explain the research design, question wording, and operationalization in greater detail in online appendix C and we report a summary of the findings in the section on robustness checks. The data allow us to track young people’s attitudes during adolescence up into adulthood. To do so, we link children’s answers from the youth questionnaire (age 13 to 15) with answers from when children move into the adult questionnaire (at age 16). This setup also allows us to include parental information by linking children to parents. However, since our study is the first to measure populist attitudes among adolescents, we use external political efficacy as dependent variable—which has been shown to be associated with populist attitudes (Bene and Boda Reference Bene and Boda2023; Geurkink et al. Reference Geurkink, Zaslove, Sluiter and Jacobs2020; Spruyt, Keppers, and Van Droogenbroeck Reference Spruyt, Keppens and Van Droogenbroeck2016) and populist party support (Krause and Wagner Reference Krause and Wagner2021). Similarly, we have to rely on more general satisfaction with family and friends instead of specific measures of relationships.

Results

To give an impression about the prevalence of populist attitudes among adolescents, figure 1 displays the overall distribution of populist attitudes and the mean values by age in the pooled sample. In general, the degree of populist attitudes among adolescents is rather low; three-quarters of that population hold values below two on a scale from one to five (Panel A). Furthermore, there are no significant age differences, as the mean level of populist attitudes fluctuates around the value of two between age 12 and 18 (Panel B). The are further no differences in distributions between countries (refer to figure A.1 in the online appendix). Finally, the mean values and distributions of populist attitudes are also quite similar compared to adult samples in Austria and Germany as we show in online appendix B.Footnote 7 Whereas adolescents have a mean level of populist attitudes of 1.80 (SD=0.86, refer to table A.1), adults hold a mean level of 1.70 (SD=0.70, refer to table B.1).Footnote 8

Figure 1 Distribution of populist attitudes

Note: Panel A displays the distribution of populist attitudes. Panel B shows mean values of populist attitudes by age with 95%-confidence intervals. Populist attitudes range from one to five. Both panels are based on the pooled sample.

Table 2 presents the main findings of our study.Footnote 9 As we can see in the first model, possibilities of codetermination, that is, having a say at home, show no correlation with populist attitudes (b=-0.025, 95%-CI [-0.072; 0.021]). This also holds when we use a more detailed index about the parent-child relationship that is only available in Austria and Germany (refer to table A.3, b=0.051, 95%-CI [-0.005; 0.107]). Hence, the parent–child relationship seems to be less well connected to populist attitudes. In Model 2, we then find that a bad peer relationship, e.g. being treated badly by classmates or feeling alone at school, is positively associated with populist attitudes (b=0.075, 95%-CI [0.019; 0.132]). Similarly but even stronger, we find in Model 3 that a negative teacher-child relationship is also positively associated with populist attitudes (b=0.121, 95%-CI [0.076; 0.166]). Perceived unfair treatment in class and in school is thus substantially connected to populist attitudes among adolescents.

Table 2 Multilevel regression models for populist attitudes

Notes: Unstandardized estimates from linear multilevel regression models with standard errors in parentheses. Models include random-slopes for evaluations of relationships with parents, peers, and teachers (if possible). Age has been centered. “Education high” refers to being in a school that grants university entrance. CH and DE are country dummies for Switzerland and Germany. Higher values on relationship variables indicate a bad relationship. All continuous variables range from zero to ten.

* p < 0.05,

** p < 0.01,

*** p < 0.001.

In Model 4, we then include the relationship perceptions with all three socialization agents at the same time. As we can see, the association of peer evaluation with populist attitudes becomes weaker (b=0.057, 95%-CI [0.001; 0.112]), whereas the association of teacher evaluation remains largely the same (b=0.114, 95%-CI [0.070; 0.159]). The coefficient for parental relationship remains insignificant.

Furthermore, we tested in Model 5 whether the average level of unfair teacher treatment is associated with adolescents’ populist attitudes, too. This does not seem to be the case, though (b=0.037, 95%-CI [-0.092; 0.166]). Since we could argue that teacher perceptions work differently when they are shared with others, we also interacted the individual teacher evaluation with the class-level average evaluation of teachers. As we show in table A.4 in the online appendix, such a connection is not significant. There is also no significant interaction term for bad peer-relationships on a class level (table A.4). However, we observe that the association of peer relationship perception with populist attitudes becomes non-significant in those models. Thus, we tentatively conclude that teacher relationships seem to hold an important connection with populist attitudes.

Finally, we find that girls and adolescents from schools with a higher aspiration level have on average a somewhat lower level of populist attitudes, whereas adolescents with migration background and higher family affluence are associated with stronger populist attitudes. There are no differences across age, parental education, countries, and whether the survey was carried out before or after the lockdown.

Robustness Checks

We performed a series of robustness checks in the online appendices to corroborate our findings. First, we reran all main models using different operationalizations for populist attitudes (tables A.5 and A.6). Regardless of operationalization, our results are robust and we basically find the same patterns as presented in the text. Furthermore, we tested whether the relationships behave differently in the three countries (tables A.7 through A.9). Although we find no major differences between Austria, Germany, and Switzerland, we have to acknowledge that the association of negative teacher relationship with populist attitudes is only significant at the 10% level in Switzerland (p=0.097). However, given the sample size (n=639), we still believe that this is in line with our main findings. The coefficients of peer relationship perceptions are, however, not significant in any of the country-specific models.

Since political socialization could potentially work differently for adolescents with migration background (for example, more negative teacher relationship or less responsive parental education), we also reran our main models excluding all adolescents with migration background. The findings show, however, that our results from the main models hold even if we exclude migrants from the models (table A.11). Furthermore, including parental political interest (as reported by the child) does not change our results in a meaningful way (table A.12). We also tested whether the three subdimensions of populist attitudes are connected differently to (negative) relationship experiences with socialization agents (table A.13). The results show that negative relationships with teachers (and to a somewhat lower degree with peers) are strongly correlated with anti-elitism and Manicheanism. In turn, negative relationships with parents are not related to any of the three dimensions.

Finally, we checked upon the heterogeneity of associations of socialization agent perceptions with populist attitudes by age groups through additional interaction models (table A.10). While we find no significant interaction term in general, the results are suggestive that the association of teacher evaluation with populist attitudes might increase as adolescents get older (figure 2). Since the majority of our sample falls between age 14 to 16 however, imprecision increases at the lower and upper end of the age range. Future research could, therefore, investigate this relationship further. We also find no differences in associations by age for peer and parental evaluation. While both models show a negative trend across age groups, that is, a lower association strength, it is not significant (refer to figures A.4 and A.5).

Figure 2 Marginal effect of teacher evaluation on populist attitudes by age

Note: Marginal effect of teacher evaluation on populist attitudes by age with 95%-confidence intervals. Higher values on teacher relationship indicate a bad relationship.

Taken together, we believe that our results are robust across various specifications.Footnote 10 While we treated the potential heterogeneity across age in exploratory fashion, it is in line with previous research on the socialization of children and early adolescents, which finds that the role of teachers and schools increases over time, whereas the influence of parents diminishes (see also Bacovsky and Fitzgerald Reference Bacovsky and Fitzgerald2023; Bruch and Soss Reference Bruch and Soss2018).

One potential downside of our study is its cross-sectional nature and the risk of unobserved confounders (see also Cinelli, Forney, and Pearl Reference Cinelli, Forney and Pearl2022). To underscore our findings, we ran additional analyses using panel data from the BHPS and UKHLS (refer to online appendix C).Footnote 11 Table 3 shows the association of relationships with socialization agents in adolescence with political efficacy in adulthood—a potential mediator in the causal chain to populist attitudes. The results mainly confirm the findings from our own study, showing a significant negative association of a bad teacher relationship in adolescence with the level of external political efficacy in early adulthood (b=-0.077, 95%-CI [-0.151; -0.003]) and vice versa for good relationship (b=0.101, 95%-CI [0.029; 0.174]). In turn, we find no significant associations for satisfaction with family or friends. Models including additional control variables do not change the results substantially (table C.3 in online appendix C). Finally, we investigated the connection of negative relationships with socialization agents on political involvement more generally. Using fixed-effects models, we tested whether changes in relationship perceptions are associated with changes in political interest. The results from fixed-effects linear probability models in table C.4 in online appendix C show that a one point higher perception that the “the teachers are always getting at me” (on a scale from zero to ten) is associated with holding 1.3 percentage points lower level of political interest (and 2.3 percentage points higher level for positive teacher perception). In turn, there are no significant results for satisfaction with family or friends.

Table 3 Regression models of external political efficacy in early adulthood

Note: Unstandardized estimates from linear regression models with standard errors in parentheses. Data from BHPS and UKHLS. Shown are associations of mean values of predictors measured in adolescence (age 13 to 15) with the mean value of external political efficacy in early adulthood (age 18 to 21). All models control additionally for labor force status (in early adulthood), self-esteem (in adolescence), the highest level of education ever achieved, parental education, sex, and migration background. All continuous variables range from zero to ten. Full models are reported in table C.2 in online appendix C.

* p < 0.05,

** p < 0.01,

*** p < 0.001.

Discussion and Conclusion

In this study, we highlighted the prevalence of populist attitudes among adolescents using unique representative surveys in three countries and we investigated the role of socialization agents for the development of populist attitudes. Our results demonstrate a consistent association between negative teacher relationships and higher levels of populist attitudes. Thus, children who feel they are treated unfairly by their teachers are more likely to hold higher levels of populist attitudes already at young age. Potentially, this association is also likely to increase with age but needs further research. In turn, a negative peer-relationship is less strongly associated with populist attitudes and there is consistently no correlation between parental relationship perception and populist attitudes. These findings hold for a variety of specifications and the theorized mechanisms and the long-term connection of teacher relationship perceptions are supported through additional findings from panel data in the UK.

One reason—maybe the main reason—for this might be the nature of the relationship with teachers as opposed to the one children have with their parents and peers. Teachers are a (state) authority outside the household in which children grow up. At the same time, children only interact with teachers in a given setting in school, that is, teachers “play” one specific role, which makes it easier for adolescents to draw conclusions from good or bad relationships. For parents and peers this is somewhat different, as they might fulfill different roles at different times. For instance, parents serve as authority figures, but they are also an emotional safe haven in times of trouble from the outside world. Thus, it becomes difficult for children to relate the negative experience with this kind of authority figure to potential anti-elitist sentiments from populists. Preliminary evidence can be drawn from correlations between relationship perceptions and the three different subdimensions of populist attitudes, but further research is needed.

Our study is the first to measure populist attitudes among a representative sample of adolescents in multiple countries. The findings present important implications for the development of populist attitudes and their consequences for democracy. Most importantly, populist attitudes are not only formed by the time adolescents start to interact with the political system, that is, around voting age. Rather, our results are more in line with the “impressionable years” hypothesis (Dinas Reference Dinas2013; Stoker and Jennings Reference Stoker and Jennings2008), suggesting that populist attitudes might develop already before adolescents engage with the political process, that is, during the teenage years. In a developmental perspective, the struggle between individual self-development and the need for group attachment can leave a void for adolescents that can attract them to populist rhetoric that portrays the world in structured black-and-white terms. This further highlights the importance of fostering political trust from early onwards, for example, through citizenship education in school but also extracurricular activities and positive learning environments that stimulate a participatory culture in class (see Ott, Meusburger, and Quenzel Reference Ott, Meusburger and Quenzel2023).

However, our study has some limitations. First, the field time of our study in Austria and Germany coincided with the COVID-19 pandemic. While we controlled for post-lockdown interviews in our models, there may be other processes at hand that cannot be captured by such a dummy variable. Second, although the Austrian and Swiss samples are representative for their respective regions, the German sample is not. Third, our analyses are mostly based on subjective perceptions of relationships with socialization agents. While we believe that the internalization of experiences are more relevant for populist attitudes, further research could investigate the role of objective indicators. Fourth, our main study is based on cross-sectional data, which means that we cannot make causal claims. Although the addition of the UK panel data allows us to further study the long-term connection between relationship experiences in adolescence with political efficacy in adulthood, future research could investigate the (enduring) causal impact on populist attitudes more directly when new data becomes available.

While we believe that we make a valuable contribution to the field of populism and political socialization research, more work is also needed to address the specific timing of the development of populist attitudes. This would, however, require a multidisciplinary effort to integrate populist attitudes scales into existing panel data sets in the fields of sociology or educational sciences. Doing so would then allow us to investigate, for instance, whether schools can work against other conditions like deprivation and negative emotions that contribute towards the development of populist attitudes. Experimental work could further increase our understanding about mitigating populist attitudes already at the onset of political socialization. In addition, our results revealed further gaps to which in-depth research should be devoted. In the wake of the debate surrounding the increasing malaise of boys, the findings that girls seem to be less populist and also that higher educated children seem to hold lower levels of populist attitudes, the role of gender presents itself as a interesting venue for further research.

Finally, more research is also needed about children’s and adolescents’ understanding of the concept of “populism.” While it has been shown that even children in the first year of primary school can hold structured political orientations (Deth, Abendschön, and Vollmar Reference Deth, Abendschön and Vollmar2011), we still know too little about what adolescents conceive of concepts like “the elite” or “the people”—something that applies to adults, too. In sum, our study provided first insights into a new and hopefully ongoing area of populism research.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful for valuable comments and suggestions from Jan Eichhorn, Aida Just, Arndt Leininger, Paul Marx, Sabrina Mayer, Martín Portos Garcia, Sigrid Roßteutscher, Carsten Wegscheider, Nadja Wehl, Norbert Zmyj, and the four anonymous reviewers. We would also like to thank Gudrun Quenzel and Michael Beck for (co-)leading the design and implementation of the youth survey in the Lake Constance area. Finally, we thank Julia Ha, Elisa Lehnerer, Katharina Meusburger, Martina Ott, Alessandro Renna, and Martina Schläpfer for their combined effort in organizing and conducting the youth survey. This research was supported by a grant from the Internationale Bodensee-Hochschule (project number 756/18, PI: Gudrun Quenzel). Sebastian Jungkunz further acknowledges funding from the projects “Growing into Politics under Material Hardship” (project number: 505158386) and “Political and religious extremism: Measuring and explaining explicit and implicit attitudes” (project number: 438614532) of the German Research Foundation (DFG).

Supplementary Materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/S1537592724000434.

Footnotes

A list of permanent links to Supplemental Materials provided by the authors precedes the References section.

*

Data replication sets are available in Harvard Dataverse at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/ABGCKD

1 In a similar way, this applies to the notion of “the people,” which can be used quite flexibly to unite different groups.

2 Further studies also show the reciprocal nature of parent-child socialization indicating that parental attitudes may be shaped through (interaction with) their children (Fitzgerald Reference Fitzgerald2011; McDevitt and Chaffee Reference McDevitt and Chaffee2002).

3 In Switzerland, populist attitudes were asked randomly only among about two-thirds of the sample and the number of respondents in the regression models is thus reduced. We describe the study in greater detail in online appendix A (see also Jungkunz Reference Jungkunz2024).

4 Although we cannot fully rule out social desirability effects, we assume that the anonymity of the situation does not lead to increased bias in responses.

5 For more information about the study and sampling strategies, see Quenzel, Beck, and Jungkunz (Reference Quenzel, Beck and Jungkunz2023).

6 While it can be argued that adolescents might under- or overstate their say at home or parental care about their performance, we assume (in general) that the internalization of such in the form of subjective perceptions are more likely to affect populist attitudes.

7 The studies were conducted as part of an earlier research project (see also Helbling and Jungkunz Reference Helbling and Jungkunz2020; Jungkunz Reference Jungkunz2021).

8 This also applies to other operationalizations of populist attitudes (refer to figures B.2 and B.3). However, we find that the average values of populist attitudes are slightly lower in the youth sample compared to the adult sample in those cases (refer to tables A.1 and B.1).

9 The intraclass-correlation coefficient (ICC) in the null model is 0.085.

10 This is also confirmed by additional sensitivity analyses based on E-Values (table A.14). E-Values describe the minimal degree of correlation that an unmeasured confounder would need to have with both the predictor and populist attitudes, subject to the measured covariates, to completely explain away the association between predictor and populist attitudes (Mathur et al. Reference Mathur, Ding, Riddell and VanderWeele2018; VanderWeele and Ding Reference VanderWeele and Ding2017). Since the E-Value of negative teacher relationship is by far the highest of all predictors, it is considered the least sensitive coefficient in the model.

11 For a similar procedure, see Jungkunz and Marx (Reference Jungkunz and Marx2024).

References

Abdelzadeh, Ali, Zetterberg, Pär, and Ekman, Joakim. 2015. “Procedural Fairness and Political Trust among Young People: Evidence from a Panel Study on Swedish High School Students.” Acta Politica 50:5378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abendschön, Simone, and Tausendpfund, Markus. 2017. “Political Knowledge of Children and the Role of Sociostructural Factors.” American Behavioral Scientist 61(2): 204–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akee, Randall, Copeland, William, Holbein, John B., and Simeonova, Emilia. 2020. “Human Capital and Voting Behavior across Generations: Evidence from an Income Intervention.” American Political Science Review 114(2): 609–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ardag, M. Murat, Silva, Bruno Castanho, Thomeczek, Jan Philipp, Bandlow-Raffalski, Steffen F., and Littvay, Levente. 2020. “Populist Attitudes and Political Engagement: Ugly, Bad, and Sometimes Good?Representation 56(3): 307–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariely, Gal. 2013. “Public Administration and Citizen Satisfaction with Democracy: Cross-National Evidence.” International Review of Administrative Sciences 79(4): 747–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bacovsky, Pavel, and Fitzgerald, Jennifer. 2023. “Raising a Politically Engaged Generation: When Parental Influence Matters Most.” Youth & Society 55(1): 4460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, Albert. 1977. “Self-Efficacy: Toward a Unifying Theory of Behavioral Change.” Psychological Review 84(2): 191215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumrind, Diana. 1991. “Parenting Styles and Adolescent Development.” In Encyclopedia of Adolescence, ed. Brooks-Gunn, Jeanne, Lerner, Richard, and Petersen, Anne. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Beck, Michael, and Ha, Julia. 2018. Lebenswelten Ostschweizer Jugendlicher. St. Gallen: Pädagogische Hochschule St.Gallen.Google Scholar
Beck, Paul Allen, and Jennings, Kent. 1982. “Pathways to Participation.” American Political Science Review 76(1): 94108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bene, Márton, and Boda, Zsolt. 2023. “A Safety Net against Populism? An Investigation of the Interaction Effect of Political Efficacy and Democratic Capacities on Populist Attitudes.” Political Research Exchange 5(1): 2220385. https://doi.org/10.1080/2474736X.2023.2220385CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Betz, Hans-Georg. 2019. “Populist Mobilization across Time and Space.” In The Ideational Approach to Populism. Concept, Theory, and Analysis, ed. Hawkins, Kirk A., Carlin, Ryan E., Littvay, Levente, and Kaltwasser, Cristóbal Rovira, 181201. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bruch, Sarah, and Soss, Joe. 2018. “Schooling as a Formative Political Eexperience: Authority Relations and the Education of Citizens.” International Review of Administrative Sciences 16(1): 3657.Google Scholar
Campbell, Angus, Converse, Philip E., Miller, Warren E., and Stokes, Donald E.. 1960. The American Voter. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Canovan, Margaret. 1999. “Trust the People! Populism and the Two Faces of Democracy.” Political Studies 47(1): 216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castanho Silva, Bruno, Andreadis, Ioannis, Anduiza, Eva, Blanuša, Nebojša, Morlet Corti, Yazmin, Delfino, Gisela, Rico, Guillem, Ruth-Lovell, Saskia P., Spruyt, Bram, Steenbergen, Marco, and Littvay, Levente. 2018. Public Opinion Surveys: A New Scale. In The Ideational Approach to Populism. Concept, Theory, and Analysis, ed. Hawkins, Kirk A., Carlin, Ryan E., Littvay, Levente, and Kaltwasser, Cristóbal Rovira, 150171. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva, Castanho, Bruno, Sebastian Jungkunz, Helbling, Marc, and Littvay, Levente. 2020. “An Empirical Comparison of Seven Populist Attitudes Scales. Political Research Quarterly 73(2): 409–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castanho Silva, Bruno, and Wratil, Christopher. 2023. “Do Parties’ Representation Failures Affect Populist Attitudes? Evidence from a Multinational Survey Experiment.” Political Science Research and Methods 11(2): 347–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cesarini, David, Johannesson, Magnus, and Oskarsson, Sven. 2014. “Pre-Birth Factors, Post-Birth Factors, and Voting: Evidence from Swedish Adoption Data.” American Political Science Review 108(1): 7187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cherng, Hua-Yu Sebastian. 2017. “If They Think I Can: Teacher Bias and Youth of Color Expectations and Achievement.” Social Science Research 66:170–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chory-Assad, Rebecca. 2002. “Classroom Justice: Perceptions of Fairness as a Predictor of Student Motivation, Learning, and Aggression.” Communication Quarterly 50(1): 5877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chory-Assad, Rebecca, and Paulsel, Michelle. 2004. “Classroom Justice: Student Aggression and Resistance as Reactions to Perceived Unfairness.” Communication Education 53(3): 253–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cinelli, Carlos, Forney, Andrew, and Pearl, Judea. 2022. “A Crash Course in Good and Bad Controls.” Sociological Methods & Research. https://doi.org/10.1177/00491241221099552CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cochran, Moncrieff, and Brassard, Jane. 1979. “Child Development and Personal Social Networks.” Child Development 50(3): 601–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denny, Kevin, and Doyle, Orla. 2009. “Does Voting History Matter? Analysing Persistence in Turnout.” American Journal of Political Science 53(1): 1735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deth, Jan W. von, Abendschön, Simone, and Vollmar, Meike. 2011. “Children and Politics: An Empirical Reassessment of Early Political Socialization.” Political Psychology 32(1): 147–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinas, Elias. 2013. “Opening ‘Openness to Change’: Political Events and the Increased Sensitivity of Young Adults.” Political Research Quarterly 66(4): 868–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dryer, Helen. 1998. “Parental Role Models, Gender and Educational Choice.” British Journal of Sociology 49(3): 375–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Easton, David, and Dennis, Jack. 1969. Children in the Political System. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Edwards, Michael. 2004. Civil Society. Malden, MA: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Firebaugh, Glenn, and Chen, Kevin. 1995. “Vote Turnout of Nineteenth Amendment Women: The Enduring Effect of Disenfranchisement.” American Journal of Sociology 100(4): 972–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzgerald, Jennifer. 2011. “Family Dynamics and Swiss Parties on the Rise: Exploring Party Support in a Changing Electoral Context.” Journal of Politics 73(3): 783–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flanagan, Constance A. 2013. Teenage Citizens: The Political Theories of the Young. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fritsche, Immo, Jonas, Eva, Ablasser, Catharina, Beyer, Magdalena, Kuban, Johannes, Manger, Anna-Marie, and Schultz, Marlene. 2013. “The Power of We: Evidence for Group-Based Control.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 49(1): 1932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geurkink, Bram, Zaslove, Andrej, Sluiter, Roderick, and Jacobs, Kristoff. 2020. “Populist Attitudes, Political Trust, and External Political Efficacy: Old Wine in New Bottles?Political Studies 68(1): 247–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghitza, Yair, Gelman, Andrew, and Auerbach, Jonathan. 2023. “The Great Society, Reagan’s Revolution, and Generations of Presidential Voting.” American Journal of Political Science 67(3): 520–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gidron, Noam, and Hall, Peter A.. 2017. “The Politics of Social Status: Economic and Cultural Roots of the Populist Right.” British Journal of Sociology 68(S1): S57S84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gniewosz, Burkhard, Noack, Peter, and Buhl, Monika. 2009. “Political Alienation in Adolescence: Associations with Parental Role Rodels, Parenting Styles, and Classroom Climate.” International Journal of Behavioral Development 33(4): 337–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gouveia-Pereira, Maria, Vala, Jorge, Palmonari, Augusto, and Rubini, Monica. 2003. “School Experience, Relational Justice and Legitimation of Institutional.” European Journal of Psychology of Education 18(3): 309–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenaway, Katherine H., Haslam, S. Alexander, Cruwys, Tegan, Branscombe, Nyla R., Ysseldyk, Renate, and Heldreth, Courtney. 2015. “From ‘We’ to “Me’: Group Identification Enhances Perceived Personal Control with Consequences for Health and Well-Being.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 109(1): 5374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grocke, Patricia, Rossano, Federico, and Tomasello, Michael. 2015. “Procedural Justice in Children: Preschoolers Accept Unequal Resource Distributions If the Procedure Provides Equal Opportunities.” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 140:197210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatemi, Peter K., and Ojeda, Christopher. 2021. “The Role of Child Perception and Motivation in Political Socialization.” British Journal of Political Science 51(3): 1097–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, Kirk A., Carlin, Ryan, Littvay, Levente, and Kaltwasser, Cristóbal Rovira. 2018. The Ideational Approach to Populism. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, Kirk A., Kaltwasser, Cristóbal Rovira, and Andreadis, Ioannis. 2020. “The Activation of Populist Attitudes.” Government and Opposition 55(2): 283307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heberle, Amy E., Rapa, Luke J., and Farago, Flora. 2020. “Critical Consciousness in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review, Critical Assessment, and Recommendations for Future Research.” Psychological Bulletin 146(6): 525–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helbling, Marc, and Jungkunz, Sebastian. 2020. “Social Divides in the Age of Globalization.” West European Politics 43(6): 1187–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogg, Michael A., and Gøtzsche, Oluf. 2021. “Self-Uncertainty and Populism. Why We Endorse Populist Ideologies, Identify with Populist Groups, and Support Populist Leaders.” In The Psychology of Populism. The Tribal Challenge to Liberal Democracy, ed. Forgas, Joseph P., Crano, William D., and Fiedler, Klaus, 197218. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holbein, John B. 2017. “Childhood Skill Development and Adult Political Participation.” American Political Science Review 111(3): 572–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, Robert A., Jankowski, Michael, and Wegscheider, Carsten. 2023. “Explaining Populist Attitudes: The Impact of Policy Discontent and Representation.” Politische Vierteljahresschrift 64(1): 133–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurrelmann, Klaus, and Quenzel, Gudrun. 2019. Developmental Tasks in Adolescence. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hyman, Herbert. 1959. Political Socialization: A Study in the Psychology of Political Behavior. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Inchley, Jo, Currie, Dorothy, Budisavljevic, Sanja, Torsheim, Torbjørn, Jåstad, Atle, Cosma, Alina, Kelly, Colette, and Arnarsson, A. Már. 2020. Spotlight on Aadolescent Health and Well-Being. Findings from the 2017/2018 Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) Survey in Europe and Canada . International Report. Volume 1. Key Findings. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe.Google Scholar
Jagers, Jan, and Walgrave, Stefaan. 2007. “Populism as Political Communication Style: An Empirical Study of Political Parties’ Discourse in Belgium.” European Journal of Political Research 46(3): 319–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jennings, M. Kent, Stoker, Laura, and Bowers, Jake. 2009. “Politics across Generations: Family Transmission Reexamined.” Journal of Politics 71(3): 782–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jungkunz, Sebastian. 2021. “Political Polarization during the COVID-19 Pandemic.” Frontiers in Political Science 3:622512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jungkunz, Sebastian. 2024. “Replication Data for: Populist Attitudes among Teenagers: How Negative Relationships with Socialization Agents are Linked to Populist Attitudes.” Harvard Dataverse. https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/ABGCKDCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jungkunz, Sebastian, Fahey, Robert A., and Hino, Airo. 2021. “How Populist Attitudes Scales Fail to Capture Support for Populists in Power.” PLoS ONE 16(12): e0261658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jungkunz, Sebastian, and Marx, Paul. 2024. “Material Deprivation in Childhood and Unequal Political Socialization: The Relationship between Children’s Economic Hardship and Future Voting.” European Sociological Review 40(1): 7284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiess, Johannes. 2022. “Learning by Doing: The Impact of Experiencing Democracy in Education on Political Trust and Participation.” Politics 42(1): 7594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knafo, Ariel, and Schwartz, Shalom H.. 2004. “Identity Formation and Parent-Child Value Congruence in Adolescence.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 22(3): 439–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koskimaa, Vesa, and Rapeli, Lauri. 2015. “Political Socialization and Political Interest: The Role of School Reassessed.” Journal of Political Science Education 11(2): 141–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krause, Werner, and Wagner, Aiko. 2021. “Becoming Part of the Gang? Established and Nonestablished Populist Parties and the Role of External Efficacy.” Party Politics 27(1): 161–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kriesi, Hanspeter. 2020. “Is There a Crisis of Democracy in Europe?Politische Vierteljahresschrift 61(2): 237–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroh, Martin, and Selb, Peter. 2009. “Inheritance and the Dynamics of Party Identification.” Political Behavior 31(4): 559–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kılıçkaya, Selin, Uçar, Nehir, and Nazlıgül, Merve Denizci. 2023. “A Systematic Review of the Association between Parenting Styles and Narcissism in Young Adults: From Baumrind’s Perspective.” Psychological Reports 126(2): 620–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipsky, Michael. 1980. Street-Level Bureaucracy: Dilemmas of the Individual in Public Services. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Manow, Philipp. 2020. (Ent-)Demokratisierung der Demokratie. Berlin: Suhrkamp.Google Scholar
Marcus, George E. 2000. “Emotions in Politics.” Annual Review of Political Science 3(1): 221–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, George E. 2022. “Hidden Affections: Presumptions that Continue to Misshape the Measurement of Emotion.” Advances in Politics and Economics 5(1): 7398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, George E., Valentino, Nicholas A., Vasilopoulos, Pavlos, and Foucault, Martial. 2019. “Applying the Theory of Affective Intelligence to Support for Authoritarian Policies and Parties.” Political Psychology 40(S1): 109–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathur, Maya B., Ding, Peng, Riddell, Corinne A., and VanderWeele, Tyler J.. 2018. “Web Site and R Package for Computing E-values.” Epidemiology 29(5): e45e47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDevitt, Michael, and Chaffee, Steven. 2002. “From Top-Down to Trickle-Up Influence: Revisiting Assumptions about the Family in Political Socialization.” Political Communication 19(3): 281301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mudde, Cas. 2004. “The Populist Zeitgeist.” Government and Opposition 39(3): 541–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, Jan.-Werner. 2016. What Is Populism? Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2019. The Promise of Adolescence: Realizing Opportunity for All Youth. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Neundorf, Anja, and Smets, Kaat. 2017. “Political Socialization and the Making of Citizens.” In Oxford Handbook Topics in Politics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199935307.013.98Google Scholar
Noack, Peter, and Eckstein, Katharina. 2023. “Populism in Youth: Do Experiences in School Matter?Child Development Perspectives 17(2): 9096.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oepke, Maren. 2005. Rechtsextremismus unter ost- und westdeutschen Jugendlichen: Einflüsse von gesellschaftlichem Wandel, Familie, Freunden und Schule. Opladen: Budrich.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ott, Martina B., Meusburger, Katharina M., and Quenzel, Gudrun. 2023. “Adolescents’ participation opportunities and student well-being in school.Frontiers in Education 8: 1111981. https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2023.1111981CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pappas, Takis S. 2019. Populism and Liberal Democracy: A Comparative and Theoretical Analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parth, Anne-Marie, Weiss, Julia, Firat, Rojda, and Eberhardt, Manuel. 2020. "How Dare You!”—The Influence of Fridays for Future on the Political Attitudes of Young Adults.” Frontiers in Political Science 2: 611139. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpos.2020.611139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, Thomas F. 2017. “Social Psychological Perspectives on Trump Supporters.” Journal of Social and Political Psychology 5(1): 107–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plutzer, Eric. 2002. “Becoming a Habitual Voter: Inertia, Resources, and Growth in Young Adulthood.” American Political Science Review 96(1): 4156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pretsch, Johanna, and Ehrhardt-Madapathi, Natalie. 2018. “Experiences of Justice in School and Attitudes towards Democracy: A Matter of Social Exchange?Social Psychology of Education 21:655–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prior, Markus. 2019. Hooked. How Politics Captures People’s Interest. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Quenzel, Gudrun, Beck, Michael, and Jungkunz, Sebastian. 2023. Bildung und Partizipation. Mitbestimmung von Schülerinnen und Schülern in Deutschland, Österreich und der Schweiz.Opladen: Barbara Budrich.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quenzel, Gudrun, and Böheim-Galehr, Gabriele. 2021. Lebenswelten 2020 – Werthaltungen junger Menschen in Vorarlberg. Innsbruck: Studien Verlag.Google Scholar
Quintelier, Ellen. 2015. “Engaging Adolescents in Politics: The Longitudinal Effect of Political Socialization Agents.” Youth & Society 47(1): 5169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Resh, Nura, and Sabbagh, Clara. 2014. “Sense of Justice in School and Civic Attitudes.” Social Psychology of Education 17:5172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rhodes-Purdy, Matthew, Navarre, Rachel, and Utych, Stephen M.. 2021. “Populist Psychology: Economics, Culture, and Emotions.” Journal of Politics 83(4): 1559–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rico, Guillem, Guinjoan, Marc, and Anduiza, Eva. 2017. “The Emotional Underpinnings of Populism: How Anger and Fear Affect Populist Attitudes.” Swiss Political Science Review 23(4): 444–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rico, Guillem, Guinjoan, Marc, and Anduiza, Eva. 2020Empowered and Enraged: Political Efficacy, Anger and Support for Populism in Europe.” European Journal of Political Research 59(4): 797816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rovira Kaltwasser, Cristóbal. 2012. “The Ambivalence of Populism: Threat and Corrective for Democracy.” Democratization 19(2): 184208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rovira Kaltwasser, Cristóbal, and Van Hauwaert, Steven M.. 2020. “The Populist Citizen: Empirical Evidence from Europe and Latin America.” European Political Science Review 12(1): 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russo, Silvia, and Stattin, Håkan. 2017. “Stability and Change in Youths’ Political Interest.” Social Indicators Research 132(2): 643–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmela, Mikko, and von Scheve, Christian. 2017. “Emotional Roots of Right-Wing Political Populism.” Social Science Information 56(4): 567–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schimpf, Christian H., Wuttke, Alexander, and Schoen, Harald. 2023. “Neither a Trait nor Wildly Fluctuating: On the Stability of Populist Attitudes and its Implications for Empirical Research.” British Journal of Political Science. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123423000492CrossRefGoogle Scholar
GmbH, Shell Deutschland Holding. 2019. Jugend 2019. Eine Generation meldet sich zu Wort. Frankfurt: Beltz.Google Scholar
Shore, Jennifer, and Tosun, Jale. 2019. “Personally Affected, Politically Disaffected? How Experiences with Public Employment Services Impact Young People’s Political Efficacy.” Social Policy and Administration 53(7): 958–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spruyt, Bram, Keppens, Gil, and Van Droogenbroeck, Filip. 2016. “Who Supports Populism and What Attracts People to It?Political Research Quarterly 69(2): 335–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoker, Laura, and Jennings, M. Kent. 2008. “Of Time and the Development of Partisan Polarization.” American Journal of Political Science 52(3): 619–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torney-Purta, Judith, Wilkenfeld, Britt, and Barber, Carolyn. 2008. “How Adolescents in 27 Countries Understand, Support, and Practice Human Rights.” Journal of Social Issues 64(4): 857–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torney-Purta, Judith, Richardson, Wendy, and Barber, Carolyn. 2004. Trust in Government-Related Institutions and Civic Engagement among Adolescents: Analysis of Five Countries from the IEA Civic Education Study. University of Maryland, Center for Information & Reseach on Civic Learning & Engagement (CIRCLE).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
University of Essex, Institute for Social and Economic Research. 2023. “Understanding Society: Waves 1-12, 2009-2021 and Harmonised BHPS: Waves 1–18, 1991–2009.” Version Number: 17th ed. UK Data Service. SN: 6614. http://doi.org/10.5255/UKDA-SN-6614-18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Urbinati, Nadia. 2019. Me the People: How Populism Transforms Democracy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Van Ditmars, Mathilde. 2023. “Political Socialization, Political Gender Gaps and the Intergenerational Transmission of Left-Right Ideology.” European Journal of Political Research 62(1): 324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VanderWeele, Tyler J., and Ding, Peng. 2017. “Sensitivity Analysis in Observational Research: Introducing the E-Value.” Annals of Internal Medicine 167(4): 268–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verba, Sidney, Schlozman, Kay, and Brady, Henry. 1995. Voice and Equality: Civic Voluntarism in American Politics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verba, Sidney, Schlozman, Kay, and Burns, Nancy. 2005. “Family Ties: Understanding the Intergenerational Transmission of Political Participation.” In The Social Logic of Politics. Personal Networks as Contexts for Political Behaviour, ed. Zuckerman, Alan, 95114. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Weisberg, Herbert. 1980. “A Multidimensional Conceptualization of Party Identification.” Journal of Political Science Education 2(1): 3360.Google Scholar
Weiss, Julia. 2023. “Intergenerational Transmission of Left-Right Ideology: A Question of Gender and Parenting Style?Frontiers in Political Science 5: 1080543. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpos.2023.1080543CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, Julia, and Parth, Anne-Marie. 2023. “The Democratic Lessons Learned—How Experiences of (un-)Equal Treatment in School Influence Satisfaction with Democracy in Later Life.” Zeitschrift für Politikwissenschaft 33: 2956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westheimer, Joel. 2019. “Civic Education and the Rise of Populist Nationalism. Peabody Journal of Education 94(1): 416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widmann, Tobias. 2021. “How Emotional Are Populists Really? Factors Explaining Emotional Appeals in the Communication of Political Parties.” Political Psychology 42(1): 163–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wirz, Dominique S. 2018. “Persuasion through Emotion? An Experimental Test of the Emotion-Eliciting Nature of Populist Communication.” International Journal of Communication 12:1114–38.Google Scholar
Wray-Lake, Laura. 2019. “How Do Young People Become Politically Engaged?Child Development Perspectives 13(2): 127–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wuttke, Alexander, Schimpf, Christian, and Schoen, Harald. 2020. “When the Whole Is Greater than the Sum of Its Parts: On the Conceptualization and Measurement of Populist Attitudes and Other Multidimensional Constructs.” American Political Science Review 114(2): 356–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wuttke, Alexander, Schimpf, Christian, and Schoen, Harald. 2023. “Populist Citizens in Four European Countries: Widespread Dissatisfaction Goes with Contradictory but Pro-democratic Regime Preferences.” Swiss Political Science Review 29(2): 246–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaslove, Andrej, Geurkink, Bram, Jacobs, Kristof, and Akkerman, Agnes. 2021. “Power to the People? Populism, Democracy, and Political Participation: A Citizen’s Perspective.” West European Politics 44(4): 727–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Question wording for populist attitudes

Figure 1

Figure 1 Distribution of populist attitudesNote: Panel A displays the distribution of populist attitudes. Panel B shows mean values of populist attitudes by age with 95%-confidence intervals. Populist attitudes range from one to five. Both panels are based on the pooled sample.

Figure 2

Table 2 Multilevel regression models for populist attitudes

Figure 3

Figure 2 Marginal effect of teacher evaluation on populist attitudes by ageNote: Marginal effect of teacher evaluation on populist attitudes by age with 95%-confidence intervals. Higher values on teacher relationship indicate a bad relationship.

Figure 4

Table 3 Regression models of external political efficacy in early adulthood

Supplementary material: File

Jungkunz and Weiss supplementary material

Jungkunz and Weiss supplementary material
Download Jungkunz and Weiss supplementary material(File)
File 479.7 KB
Supplementary material: Link

Jungkunz and Weiss Dataset

Link