Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T10:16:39.833Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Fear learning and extinction predicts anxiety in daily life: a study of Pavlovian conditioning and ecological momentary assessment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 September 2022

Kathryn L. Modecki*
Affiliation:
Menzies Health Institute Queensland; Centre for Mental Health, School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University, Mt Gravatt, Queensland, Australia
Katherine M. Ryan
Affiliation:
School of Applied Psychology Griffith University Mt Gravatt, Queensland, Australia
Allison M. Waters
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health; School of Applied Psychology Griffith University Mt Gravatt, Queensland, Australia
*
Author for correspondence: Kathryn L. Modecki, E-mail: k.modecki@griffith.edu.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The association between anxious mood and aberrant fear learning mechanisms has not been fully elucidated. Studying how fear conditioning and extinction constructs relate to anxiety symptoms and reactivity to stressful and benign moments in everyday life provides a powerful addition to experimental paradigms.

Method

Fifty-one young adults completed laboratory-based differential conditioning and extinction tasks with (CS + ) and without (CS-) an aversive unconditional stimulus (US). Electrodermal skin conductance responses were measured during each phase, followed by ecological momentary assessment (EMA) tapping anxiety and stressors six times daily for seven days (2, 142 moments).

Results

Conditioned electrodermal reactivity to the CS + and overgeneralisation to the CS- were associated with greater change in anxiety (measured via EMA), across non-stressful situations, remaining the same across stressful situations. Likewise, during extinction when the CS + is now safe, more electrodermal reactivity to the CS + was associated with more anxiety change across non-stressful situations and remained the same across stressful situations. Also, during extinction when threat is absent, more electrodermal reactivity at the late stage of the CS- was associated with less momentary anxiety change in response to stressful situations; more electrodermal activity at the late stage of the CS + was associated with more anxiety change across non-stressful situations and remained the same across stressful situations.

Conclusions

Sampling ‘in vivo’ emotion and stress experiences, study findings revealed links between conditioned electrodermal reactivity and overgeneralisation to safe stimuli and heightened anxious reactivity during non-stressful (i.e. safe) moments in daily life, coupled with less change in response to actual stressors.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

With an estimated lifetime prevalence of nearly a third of individuals worldwide, anxiety disorders represent a major health and economic burden (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005; Marciniak, Lage, Landbloom, Dunayevich, & Bowman, Reference Marciniak, Lage, Landbloom, Dunayevich and Bowman2004). Fear conditioning and extinction models show significant promise in explaining how anxiety develops within laboratory-based Pavlovian conditioning tasks (Lonsdorf et al., Reference Lonsdorf, Menz, Andreatta, Fullana, Golkar, Haaker and Merz2017) and can be treated via extinction principles underlying exposure therapy in CBT (Heinig et al., Reference Heinig, Pittig, Richter, Hummel, Alt, Dickhöver and Wittchen2017).

Pavlovian fear learning models of anxiety are widely supported (Duits et al., Reference Duits, Cath, Lissek, Hox, Hamm, Engelhard and Baas2015) and indicate that anxiety develops through the association of a conditioned stimulus (CS + ) and an aversive, previously benign unconditioned stimulus (US), so that the CS + , in itself, is capable of eliciting anxiety whereas a stimulus that is never paired with the US will not (CS-). Importantly, during an extinction phase, the CS + is repeatedly presented without the unconditioned stimulus so that eventually, the heighted reactivity to the CS + begins to weaken and the learned fear is extinguished. More anxious individuals exhibit elevated electrodermal (measured via skin conductance) responses to the CS + and CS- during fear conditioning and exhibit perturbations in reducing responses to the CS + when it is no longer paired with threat during extinction (i.e. overgeneralisation) (Duits et al., Reference Duits, Cath, Lissek, Hox, Hamm, Engelhard and Baas2015; Dymond, Dunsmoor, Vervliet, Roche, & Hermans, Reference Dymond, Dunsmoor, Vervliet, Roche and Hermans2015).

However, the association between aberrant fear learning mechanisms and anxious mood has not been fully elucidated. Theoretical tenets underlying aberrant fear learning is that more anxious individuals (a) more readily acquire elevated conditioned fear responses, (b) generalise these associations to safe stimuli, and (c) have difficulty inhibiting conditioned responses with new information indicating that the CS + is safe during extinction (Scheveneels, Boddez, & Hermans, Reference Scheveneels, Boddez and Hermans2021). Moreover, a major premise is that these individuals are more likely to generalise these heightened associative processes to everyday stressors and benign events which do not necessitate a ‘reactive’ response (Craske, Treanor, Conway, Zbozinek, & Vervliet, Reference Craske, Treanor, Conway, Zbozinek and Vervliet2014).

To date, however, no studies have examined the association of Pavlovian fear conditioning, overgeneralisation to safe stimuli, and impaired extinction with anxiety symptoms in daily life. Given extant data, other related studies may be relevant which have demonstrated that elevated defensive responding, in the form of larger startle eye blink responses to safe stimuli during fear-potentiated startle eye blink experiments, characterise individuals with anxiety disorders (e.g. Grillon, Ameli, Goddard, Woods, and Davis, Reference Grillon, Ameli, Goddard, Woods and Davis1994; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Nazarian, Mineka, Zinbarg, Griffith, Naliboff and Craske2014) and predict anxiety disorder onset four years later (Craske et al. Reference Craske, Wolitzky-Taylor, Mineka, Zinbarg, Waters, Vrshek-Schallhorn and Ornitz2012). Several additional studies provide preliminary support for reduced extinction of a conditioned emotional response as a vulnerability factor for posttraumatic stress (Lommen, van de Schoot, & Engelhard, Reference Lommen, van de Schoot and Engelhard2014; Orr et al., Reference Orr, Lasko, Macklin, Pineles, Chang and Pitman2012). However, no studies have examined the translation of fear learning processes to ‘in vivo’ experiences of anxiety in everyday moments and stressors across daily life. Such research is critical for characterising whether laboratory-based measures of fear learning impairments predict everyday fluctuations in anxiety symptoms and maladjustment to stressful and benign (i.e. safe) moments in day-to-day life (Scheveneels, Boddez, Vervliet, & Hermans, Reference Scheveneels, Boddez, Vervliet and Hermans2016).

In this study, we aimed to characterise the relations between fear acquisition and extinction constructs based on electrodermal (skin conductance) measures indexed within Pavlovian fear conditioning and extinction tasks and everyday anxious reactivity to stressful and non-stressful (i.e. safe) events measured via ecological momentary assessment (EMA). EMA leverages individuals' reporting of their experiences (including stressors, positive, and benign events) and their current emotions in the moment, across various time points and settings (Myin-Germeys et al., Reference Myin-Germeys, Kasanova, Vaessen, Vachon, Kirtley, Viechtbauer and Reininghaus2018, Reference Myin-Germeys, Oorschot, Collip, Lataster, Delespaul and Van Os2009). Because each individual is their own control, EMA facilitates modelling of emotional experiences, including individuals' emotion reactivity (i.e. change in emotion) in relation to events (Hamaker, Reference Hamaker, Mehl and Conner2012; Telford, McCarthy-Jones, Corcoran, & Rowse, Reference Telford, McCarthy-Jones, Corcoran and Rowse2012). Thus, EMA offers excellent ecological validity for measuring stress reactivity across daily life (Modecki, Duvenage, Uink, Barber, & Donovan, Reference Modecki, Duvenage, Uink, Barber and Donovan2022; Modecki, Goldberg, Ehrenreich, Russell, & Bellmore, Reference Modecki, Goldberg, Ehrenreich, Russell and Bellmore2019).

We hypothesised that if heightened fear conditioning and overgeneralisation to safe stimuli heightens reactivity in daily life (Duits et al., Reference Duits, Cath, Lissek, Hox, Hamm, Engelhard and Baas2015; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Nazarian, Mineka, Zinbarg, Griffith, Naliboff and Craske2014), then larger electrodermal responses to the CS + and the safe stimulus (CS-) during conditioning will predict elevated anxiety in relation to safe situations (i.e. no stress situations) comparable to the degree of anxiety in relation to threat situations (i.e. stressors) in daily life. Predictions regarding extinction are less clear in the absence of prior studies. However, if slowed extinction and overgeneralisation relate to difficulty inhibiting anxious reactivity in daily life (Lommen et al., Reference Lommen, van de Schoot and Engelhard2014), then elevated electrodermal responses to the CS + and CS- during extinction will predict less change in anxiety following a stressor (that is, anxiety level after a stressor, controlling for anxiety at the prior moment) by virtue of elevated anxiety reactivity across non-stressful moments.

Methods

Participants

Original study participants were 62 young adults (71% female; 29% male) age 17–28 years (Mage = 19.11, s.d. = 2.77) recruited via a University research pool. Participants were awarded course credit for phase one and a gift card for phase two to the value of the amount of their SMS responses to the EMA protocol (up to $45). Participants were drawn from studies running in the lab (see Stimulus Materials). Two participants from the lab-based sessions declined to participate in the EMA phase (97% uptake); nine participants who completed the EMA phase were not in a control study arm (e.g. were exposed to an additional task prior to fear conditioning) and so were subsequently excluded. Thus, the final sample consisted of 51 young adults (Mage = 18.96, s.d. = 2.70, 17–28 years; 70% female).

Measures

Anxiety symptoms

The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults (STAI; Spielberger, Jacobs, Russell, and Crane, Reference Spielberger, Jacobs, Russell and Crane1983) was used to assess anxiety symptomology. We used the 20-item trait sub-scale designed to differentiate the more general and longstanding quality of ‘trait anxiety,’ from short-term, fluctuations in state anxiety, which are better captured within our ESM design. Higher scores indicating higher symptomology/anxiety, M = 44.23, s.d. = 10.88, range = 23–67, α = 0.91. Six participants were not given the STAI to complete; they were still included in the models in accordance with best practice (Enders, Reference Enders2022).

Phase one: Conditioning and extinction task

Stimulus materials

Two different stimuli were used across four individual studies which contributed to the current project, each of which was a fear conditioning and extinction task. Studies (#1 and #2) used geometric shapes of a pastel cream triangle and pastel pink trapezoid as the CSs. The US was a 3s recording of a three-pronged garden fork scraped over slate (see Neumann & Waters, Reference Neumann and Waters2006; Waters, Theresiana, Neumann, & Craske, Reference Waters, Theresiana, Neumann and Craske2017). Two studies (#3 and #4), used photographs of dogs as the CSs. The US was a 3s aversive sound of a dog growling and a woman screaming reaching 100 dB. The dichotomous study stimulus covariate was included in all models and was non-significant. See online Supplementary Section for details related to individual studies and study stimuli and additional sensitivity checks associated with each.

Skin conductance responses (SCR)

SCR were recorded using two self-adhesive isotonic electrodes (Biopac systems EL507) attached to the thenar and hypothenar eminences of the non-dominant hand. Data were analysed using AcqKnowledge software Version 5.0. See online Supplementary section for details and descriptives.

Phase two: Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA)

Momentary emotion

Participants were asked ‘Please indicate how _____ you feel right now?’ at each sampling moment. Participants rated how happy, sad, angry, and anxious they were feeling (1 = Not at all, 5 = Very much) (Schneiders et al., Reference Schneiders, Nicolson, Berkhof, Feron, Van Os and Devries2006; Uink, Modecki, & Barber, Reference Uink, Modecki and Barber2017). For the current study, we focused on anxious emotions in relation to stress responses, M = 1.89, s.d. = 1.19.

Momentary stress

Participants were asked ‘Since the last set of questions, has anything negative or stressful/bad happened to you?’ at each sampling moment (Schneiders et al., Reference Schneiders, Nicolson, Berkhof, Feron, Van Os and Devries2006; Uink, Modecki, Barber, & Correia, Reference Uink, Modecki, Barber and Correia2018). The question format ensured that participants reported on recent stressors in the last several hours. Participants reported stressful events at approximately 12% of all sampling moments. Responses were dummy coded (0 = no bad event since last messaged, 1 = bad event since last messaged) (blinded).

Momentary uplifts

Participants also reported on momentary uplifts in a similar fashion ‘Since the last set of questions, has anything good or positive happened to you?’ to control for these experiences in all models (Uink, Modecki, Barber, & Correia, Reference Uink, Modecki, Barber and Correia2018). Participants reported uplifts at approximately 18% of all sampling moments. The online Supplementary Section incudes full details for all EMA questions.

Procedure

Approval was obtained from the University Ethics Committee to undertake the fear conditioning tasks (# 2019/167; 2019/165; 2018/891) and to conduct a week-long EMA follow-up (#2018/499). For phase 1, participants completed the fear conditioning and extinction task in one of four studies and following the lab session, were recruited for phase two. Participants provided their phone number and returned the signed consent form via email to participate in phase two. Subsequently, participants were called/texted to assign a start date.

Phase 1: fear conditioning, extinction and retest

All studies involved the same number of trials across the fear conditioning and extinction studies, though the stimuli and US differed. Participants completed the STAI after the experiment.Footnote Footnote 1 During the acquisition phase, participants were presented with 24 trials, consisting of 12 CS + trials with 100% reinforcement (CS + paired with the US) and 12 CS- trials (presented without the US). The first two trials were a CS + followed by a CS- trial (or CS- followed by CS + ). Subsequent trials were presented in random order across participants with no more than two trials of either CS presented consecutively. A fixation cross was presented on the screen from CS offset to next CS onset. The intertrial interval (ITI) between each trial varied from between 16 to 30s. During the extinction phase, participants were presented with 24 trials consisting of 12 CS + (without the US) and 12 CS- trials in random order with no more than two trials of either CS presented consecutively.

Phase 2: EMA

Participants completed phase 2 within 4 weeks after completing phase 1. Data were collected via brief surveys embedded within text messages to participants' mobile phone. Study participants were assessed at random time-points, within pre-specified time-blocks, six times daily for seven days. Text messages were sent each morning (8:30–9:30), lunch (11:30–12:30), afternoon (2:30–3:30), evening (5:30–6:30), night-time (8:30–9:30pm), and bedtime (9:30–10:30pm). Surveys were closed within an hour of being sent.

Response definitions and statistical analyses

Between-person measures

The primary between-person constructs of interest were the SCR indices. The magnitude of SCRs elicited during the presentation of each CS was scored within two latency windows; first interval responses (FIR) commencing within 1–5s after CS onset, reflecting the initial orienting response to the CS, (Öhman, Reference Öhman1983; Öhman & Bohlin, Reference Öhman and Bohlin1973; Prokasy, Reference Prokasy1977), and late interval responses (LIR) occurring within 6–10s, providing a means of comparing responses to the US on CS + trials and US absence on CS- trials (Prokasy, Reference Prokasy1977; Prokasy & Kumpfer, Reference Prokasy, Kumpfer, Prokasy and Raskin1973). Separate average FIRs and LIRs were computed across trials for CS + and CS- during conditioning and extinction phases. See online Supplementary Section.

Between-person covariates

Analyses also controlled for STAI-trait. Given large variance in the STAI-T relative to other constructs, we completed a simple transformation (divided by two) to enhance model convergence. We further controlled for other relevant constructs at the person-level, including participant gender (0 = Female, 1 = Male), study stimuli (dogs = 0; shapes = 1), and EMA start day (Monday = 1 to Sunday = 7).

Within-person measures

The key within-person measures of interest were the EMA responses.

Momentary emotion

We tested for change in emotion by predicting anxious emotion at the current sampling moment (T = 0) while controlling for anxious emotion at the previous time point (T − 1). Thus, analyses characterise changes in anxiety, controlling for other momentary covariates.

Within-person covariates

Momentary uplifts and Time of day, a variable designating time of day for each momentary report (1 = morning to 6 = bedtime), were included in all models to control for potential confounds.

Analytical plan

We ran a series of hierarchical linear models (HLM) to account for repeated measures of stress and emotion nested within-person (e.g. Hoffman and Rovine, Reference Hoffman and Rovine2007). HLM accounts for non-independence in the variables measured at each sampling moment and allows for separate estimation of within-person and between-person variance in outcomes. Within-person constructs are modelled at level 1 and between-person constructs are modelled at level 2. Level 1 variables were group-mean centred (centred on each participant's average for the week) and level 2 variables were grand-mean centred (on the sample's average) (Enders & Tofighi, Reference Enders and Tofighi2007). With a total of 2, 142 possible responses (51 participants × 42 responses), 76% (N = 1637 data points) were available for analyses. We applied FIML estimation and all available data were employed to estimate missing data points (Enders, Reference Enders2022).

Separate models were estimated for FIRs and LIRs to CS + and CS- trials during conditioning and extinction. Details of model building procedures are provided in the online Supplementary Section, along with relevant model equations. Model fit indices, including AIC, BIC, as well as associated within-person and between-person residual variances, are reported within results tables. Building models methodically, we sequentially modelled null, level one, two-level, and cross-level interaction models, with this final model which included the cross-level interaction term (skin conductance moderating the regression of emotion on stress) of critical interest and bolded in study tables. To account for multiple analyses, we applied a Benjamini Hochberg procedure for adjusted significance tests to manage the False Discovery Rate within sets of analyses (FDR; Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995). The FDR was applied within each table, with traditional p values reported, and those meeting FDR-corrected significance levels marked with an asterisk. Further sensitivity tests were run for all models, in which Study was entered as a control, as opposed to Stimuli as reported here. The same pattern of results emerged, and there were no significant findings associated with different study participation (See online Supplementary Section).

Results

Acquisition

First interval SCRs to CS- (CSM – FIR)

Building the models from level 1 predictors (left side of Table 1, level one model) of stressor, anxiety at T-1, uplift, and within-day timing of survey, we then modelled the predictive variation of the effect of experiencing a stressor on subsequent anxiety change based on theorised person-level constructsFootnote 2 (two-level model column). Here, stimulus, study start day, trait anxiety, gender and the relevant skin conductance construct were entered into level 2. Next, we estimated a random coefficient model with the between-person skin conductance predictor (CSM-FIR) and gender. That is, two cross-level regression coefficients were simultaneously modelled (Cross-level interaction column, bolded). The regression coefficient for CSM-FIR was statistically significant (b = −0.10, p = 0.02) indicating that CSM-FIR moderated the anxiety on stress regression. Further, the regression coefficient was also significant for gender (b = −0.46, p = 0.02), indicating larger anxiety reactivity to stressors for females. Plots for all significant gender interactions are provided in online Supplementary Fig. S2.

Table 1. First interval skin conductance responses to CSM and CSP during acquisition and anxiety models

Note. Stressor = 0 = no bad event since last messaged, 1 = bad event since last messaged; Uplift = 0 = no uplift since last messaged, 1 = uplifting event since last messaged; Study stimuli = dogs = 0, shapes = 1; Start day = Monday = 1 to Sunday = 7; STAI-T = Trait anxiety score ÷ 2. Gender = 0 = female, 1 = male. Slope Coefficients represent the cross-level interactions (e.g. level-2 moderator of the level-1 anxiety on stressor regression). Two-level and cross-level model coefficients which met FDR-corrected significance level marked with an asterisk.

We plotted and probed the simple slope of stress on anxiety change at one s.d. above and below the group mean for CSM-FIR to better characterise how the fear learning construct related to within-person changes in anxiety. The form of the interaction is described in Fig. 1 (upper left panel). Individuals lower in CSM-FIR (i.e. lower electrodermal reactivity to a safe cue during conditioning) correspondingly reported lower shifts in anxiety in safe moments during daily life (intercept = 1.63(0.10), p < 0.001) and showed significant increases in anxiety in response to daily stressors (Z = 3.67, p < 0.001). However, individuals high in electrodermal reactivity to a safe cue during conditioning reported relatively higher shifts in anxiety during safe moments in daily life (i.e. no stressor, intercept = 2.04(0.10), p < 0.001) and although they showed increased anxiety in the moments after a stressor, this was a non-significant change (Z = 1.16, p = 0.25); thus they remained elevated in anxiety across non-stressful and stressful moments in daily life.

Fig. 1. Anxiety change following no stress and a stressor as a function of low and high skin conductance responses during the early and late stage of the CS- (upper panels) and CS + lower panels during conditioning (left panels) and extinction (right panels).

Last interval SCRs to CS- (CSM – LIR)

We modelled the cross-level interaction terms (Table 2, set of columns on the left) to assess whether the predictive effect of stressor on anxiety change varied across individuals based on person-level predictors (CSM-LIR and gender). In this case, CSM-LIR was not a significant cross-level predictor of the impact of stress on anxiety change (b = −0.08, p = 0.24), though gender did predict significant variance in the level 1 effect. The pattern of the non-significant CSM-LIR interaction is characterised in Fig. 1.

Table 2. Last interval skin conductance responses to CSM and CSP during acquisition and anxiety models

Note. Stressor = 0 = no bad event since last messaged, 1 = bad event since last messaged; Uplift = 0 = no uplift since last messaged, 1 = uplifting event since last messaged; Study stimuli = dogs = 0, shapes = 1; Start day = Monday = 1 to Sunday = 7; STAI-T = Trait anxiety score ÷ 2. Gender = 0 = female, 1 = male. Slope Coefficients represent the cross-level interactions (e.g. level-2 moderator of the level-1 anxiety on stressor regression). Two-level and cross-level model coefficients which met FDR-corrected significance level marked with an asterisk.

First interval SCRs to CS + (CSP – FIR)

Estimating the random coefficients model with level 2 predictors (CSP-FIR, gender) of the random slope indicated both the CSP-FIR and gender cross-level interaction regression coefficient were significant (Table 1, far right columns). The form of the CSP-FIR interaction (b = −0.06, p = 0.04) is described in Fig. 1, bottom left panel. Individuals at low levels of electrodermal reactivity to the CS + correspondingly reported lower anxiety across safe moments during daily life and showed significant increases in anxiety in response to daily stressors. In contrast, individuals who acquired more electrodermal reactivity at the early stage of CS + trials reported less change in anxiety from moments without relative to with a stressor, which was attributable to elevated anxiety to the moments without a stressor which were similar (i.e. non-significant) in response to stressors. Specifically, at low CSP-FIR scores (1 s.d. below the group mean), the intercept (intercept = 1.70(0.09), p < 0.001) and slope were significant (Z = 2.67, p = 0.008). At high CSP-FIR scores (1 s.d. above the group mean), the intercept was relatively higher (intercept = 1.96(0.10), p < 0.001) and the slope was non-significant (Z = 0.91, p = 0.36). Thus, greater electrodermal reactivity during early stages of CS + trials during conditioning predicted greater anxiety in response to safe moments in daily life that did not discriminate in relation to stressful events.

Last interval SCRs to CS + (CSP – LIR)

We next predicted variation in the effect of stressor on anxiety change via individual-level constructs of CSP-LIR and gender. As shown in Table 2 (right-most column), the cross-level interaction for CSP-LIR was b = −0.06(0.03), p = 0.03, but with the FDR applied, the interaction term no longer reached statistical significance. The pattern of the non-significant interaction for CSP-LIR is characterised within Fig. 1.

Extinction

First interval SCRs to CS- (CSM – FIR)

We estimated the random coefficients model with CSM-FIR during extinction and gender as level 2 predictors of the random slope. As described in Table 3 (left), only the cross-level interaction for gender was statistically significant. The non-significant interaction pattern for CSM-FIR (b = −0.13, p = 0.06) is depicted in Fig. 1.

Table 3. First interval skin conductance responses to CSM and CSP during extinction and anxiety models

Note. Stressor = 0 = no bad event since last messaged, 1 = bad event since last messaged; Uplift = 0 = no uplift since last messaged, 1 = uplifting event since last messaged; Study stimuli = dogs = 0, shapes = 1; Start day = Monday = 1 to Sunday = 7; STAI-T = Trait anxiety score ÷ 2. Gender = 0 = female, 1 = male. Slope Coefficients represent the cross-level interactions (e.g. level-2 moderator of the level-1 anxiety on stressor regression). Two-level and cross-level model coefficients which met FDR-corrected significance level marked with an asterisk.

Last interval SCRs to CS- (CSM – LIR)

Described in Table 4 (left), estimating the random coefficients model with CSM-LIR during extinction and gender as the level 2 predictors, only the CSM-LIR cross-level interaction was statistically significant (b = −0.10, p = 0.03). The top right panel of Fig. 1 describes the interaction. As shown, individuals at low CSM-LIR levels during extinction showed significant increases in anxious reactivity in response to stressors. Whereas individuals higher on electrodermal reactivity to the CS- during extinction were generally the same across both non-stressful and stressful moments. Probing of the simple slopes indicate that for individuals at low levels of CSM-LIR during extinction (1 s.d. group mean), the intercept was slightly lower (intercept = 1.83(0.09), p < 0.001) and the slope was steeper (Z = 4.88, p < 0.001) whereas for individuals scoring high (1 s.d. above the group mean) in CSM-LIR during extinction, the intercept was significant and slightly higher (intercept = 1.83(0.08), p < 0.001) and the slope was not significant (Z = 1.73, p = 0.084). Thus, electrodermal reactivity during the later stages of responding to safe stimuli during extinction predicted greater anxiety across safe moments in daily life that did not discriminate in relation to stressful events.

Table 4. Last interval skin conductance responses to CSM and CSP during extinction and anxiety models

Note. Stressor = 0 = no bad event since last messaged, 1 = bad event since last messaged; Uplift = 0 = no uplift since last messaged, 1 = uplifting event since last messaged; Study stimuli = dogs = 0, shapes = 1; Start day = Monday = 1 to Sunday = 7; STAI-T = Trait anxiety score ÷ 2. Gender = 0 = female, 1 = male. Slope Coefficients represent the cross-level interactions (e.g. level-2 moderator of the level-1 anxiety on stressor regression). Two-level and cross-level model coefficients which met FDR-corrected significance level marked with an asterisk.

First interval SCRs to CS + (CSP – FIR)

Characterised within Table 3 (right), the regression coefficients for the cross-level interactions (CSP-FIR and gender) were non-significant. The pattern associated with the non-significant CSP-FIR interaction (b = −0.06, p = 0.08) is characterised in Fig. 1.

Last interval SCRs to CS + (CSP – LIR)

The CSP-LIR coefficient for the cross-level interaction was statistically significant (Table 4, right), as was the coefficient for gender. Examining the negative CSP-LIR coefficient (b = −0.16, p = 0.02) suggests that for participants with higher CSP-LIR during extinction, the impact of stressor on subsequent anxiety change was smaller than expected based on only the direct effect. Figure 1 (bottom right panel) describes the interaction. Individuals high on CSP-LIR during extinction were generally similar following moments when no stressor occurred as when moments followed a stressor. However, individuals low on CSP-LIR during extinction increased their anxious responses in relation to stressors. Hence individuals high on CSP-LIR remained similarly elevated in anxiety across non-stressful and stressful moments in daily life. Probing of the simple slopes showed that for individuals at low levels of CSP-LIR during extinction, the intercept was relatively lower (intercept = 1.83(0.09), p < 0.001) and the slope was significant (Z = 4.87, p < 0.001), whereas for individuals scoring high on CSP-LIR, the intercept was slightly higher and the slope was not significant (intercept = 1.83(0.08), p < 0.001; slope Z = 1.73, p = 0.08). Thus, high electrodermal reactivity during late stages of the CS + during extinction (i.e. absence of the US) predicted relatively similar reactivity across non-stressful moments as stressful moments in daily life.

Discussion

This is the first empirical study, to our knowledge, to examine whether fear learning processes translate to real world, ‘in the moment’ experience of anxiety in daily life. Findings indicated that larger first intervals SCRs to threat conditioned stimuli (CS + ) and safe stimuli (CS-) during acquisition predicted sustained elevations in anxiety across non-stressful moments and stressful moments in daily life. Late interval SCRs during acquisition did not condition the relation between experiencing stressors and momentary anxiety changes in daily life. Likewise, during extinction, first interval SCRs did not condition the relation between stressors and anxiety changes. However, larger last interval SCRs to safe stimuli (CS-) and threat stimuli (CS + ) (i.e. absence of the US) were predictive of sustained elevations in anxiety across non-stressful moments and stressful moments in daily life. Study findings were above and beyond the explanatory role of trait anxiety.

Fear learning models and empirical evidence suggest that individuals who more readily acquire heightened conditioned fear responses extrapolate these associations to safe stimuli and have trouble tamping down conditioned responses with new information indicating safety (Scheveneels et al., Reference Scheveneels, Boddez and Hermans2021; Vervliet, Craske, & Hermans, Reference Vervliet, Craske and Hermans2013; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Nazarian, Mineka, Zinbarg, Griffith, Naliboff and Craske2014). The key contribution of this work is that because individuals showing heightened fear responses generalise such heightened associative processes to both daily stressful and non-stressful events (the latter of which do not require an emotionally reactive response), they are at increased risk for anxiety disorders and treatment resistance (Guthrie & Bryant, Reference Guthrie and Bryant2006; Lommen et al., Reference Lommen, van de Schoot and Engelhard2014; Orr et al., Reference Orr, Lasko, Macklin, Pineles, Chang and Pitman2012). Indeed, we found that electrodermal reactivity to a threat conditioned stimulus (i.e. the CS + ), overgeneralisation to safe stimuli (i.e. CS-) and persistent electrodermal elevations during extinction (when both the CS- and CS + are now safe) predicted real-world overgeneralisation of anxious reactivity to benign (e.g. safe) moments in which no stressful events occurred in daily life. This suggests that vulnerability to acquiring and generalising conditioned fear responses, and difficulty inhibiting these responses with new safety information, may be reflective of aberrations in underlying mechanisms by which a wide range of benign situations in daily life become associated with threat and anxiety (Waters & Craske, Reference Waters and Craske2016).

One potential mechanism for this effect is that enhanced acquisition and generalisation of conditioned physiological reactivity is associated with the propensity to evaluate other events in daily life as similarly threatening as genuine stressors. This would result in minimal discrimination in the evaluation of safe and threat situations and thus similar shifts in anxiety in response to safe situations and genuine threats in daily life (Waters & Craske, Reference Waters and Craske2016). The influence of fear learning experiences on elaborative processing can also be interpreted as consistent with the model of emotional contrast avoidance in anxiety (e.g. Newman & Liera, Reference Newman and Llera2011; Newman et al., Reference Newman, Jacobson, Zainal, Shin, Szkodny and Sliwinski2019). That is, elevated reactivity to threat and safe stimuli may reflect a baseline hypersensitivity to upward shifts in emotion to negative events that persists across non-stressful situations. By sustaining negative emotion, worry enables the avoidance of negative emotional shifts in response to unexpected stressors. This may explain the observation that elevated last interval SCRs to the CS- in extinction (i.e. the interval temporally associated with when the US is delivered on CS + trials during acquisition) predicted less change in anxiety in response to genuine stressors in daily life. That is, maintaining reactivity to a safe stimulus that has a very weak association with threat (i.e. same timing of the US) as well as to a threat stimulus (i.e. the CS + ) that is now safe is related to daily life patterns wherein less of an upward shift in anxiety is experienced in response to genuine stressors. Thus, the unpleasant experience of moving from neutral to negative emotion can be avoided by sustained, elevated worry (Newman & Llera, Reference Newman and Llera2011). Further studies are required that include momentary sampling of stressors and situational evaluations alongside emotions across daily life. This would permit the examination of the extent to which aberrant fear learning and extinction influences elevated emotional reactivity in daily life via the impact on threat evaluation and worry (Newman & Llera, Reference Newman and Llera2011; Waters & Craske, Reference Waters and Craske2016).

Although in need of replication, the present findings may have important experimental and clinical implications. First, they highlight that experimental analogue tasks have the potential to elucidate aberrant processes under baseline conditions that are distinct from pre-anxiety baseline conditions (Newman et al., Reference Newman, Jacobson, Zainal, Shin, Szkodny and Sliwinski2019). They also suggest that mere exposure to fear stimuli alone during exposure therapy is insufficient to attenuate generalised reactivity in daily life. If the propensity for heightened conditioned fear and generalisation is underpinned by biases in elaborative processes such as evaluations and worry, then exposure therapy may be enhanced by the inclusion of exposure to threat outcomes, not just the stimulus. This may enhance the ability to tolerate shifts in distress rather than avoid the stimulus and sustain distress through worrying (Newman et al., Reference Newman, Jacobson, Zainal, Shin, Szkodny and Sliwinski2019). It may also be beneficial to couple exposure trials to the stimulus and outcome with strategies such as memory rehearsal to maximise the retention of new learning, such as memory rehearsal to maximise safe-threat stimulus discrimination and prevent overgeneralisation (Craske et al., Reference Craske, Roy-Byrne, Stein, Sullivan, Sherbourne and Bystritsky2009; Waters & Craske, Reference Waters and Craske2016).

Study limitations also merit consideration. First, we did not collect data on ethnicity, and this would be an important consideration for future work. Second, our sample size is relatively small. Additional research would benefit from both a larger sample size and inclusion of clinical samples. Third, we included fear conditioning and extinction phases only. Future studies should include an extinction retest phase or return of fear manipulation (e.g. renewal) to examine longer-term and relapse mechanisms as predictors of daily emotion reactivity. Finally, we explored anxiety, stressors, and uplifts across the course of a week. Exploration of the impact on other emotions such as depressed mood and anger would be important next steps.

Our findings show that aberrant electrodermal (skin conductance) reactivity during laboratory-based Pavlovian fear conditioning and extinction tasks is associated with increased anxious reactivity during benign moments of everyday life and this same level of reactivity translates to stressful moments. These findings highlight the validity of laboratory-based measures of fear learning impairments in predicting anxious reactivity in daily life and warrants further study, in particular, the inclusion of evaluations of everyday events and stressors.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722002379.

Financial support

Support for this work was provided by a Seed Funding Award from Griffith University's School of Applied Psychology and from an Australian Research Council Discovery Award DP#210103986

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 They also provided subjective ratings of CS valence and arousal and anxiety before and after each phase and SCRs were measured during each trial.

2 All models were built up methodically. Random slopes were also tested for uplifts, which resulted in increased BIC values and little change in unexplained level 1 variance. Cross-level interaction terms were introduced independently for each covariate to ensure models were correctly specified. Neither STAI, Study Stimuli, nor Start Day were significant level 2 predictors of the within-person effects.

References

Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 57(1), 289300.Google Scholar
Craske, M. G., Roy-Byrne, P. P., Stein, M. B., Sullivan, G., Sherbourne, C., & Bystritsky, A. (2009). Treatment for anxiety disorders: Efficacy to effectiveness to implementation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(11), 931937.10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craske, M. G., Treanor, M., Conway, C. C., Zbozinek, T., & Vervliet, B. (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy: An inhibitory learning approach. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 58, 1023.10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craske, M. G., Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R., Waters, A. M., Vrshek-Schallhorn, S., … Ornitz, E. (2012). Elevated responding to safe conditions as a specific risk factor for anxiety versus depressive disorders: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(2), 315.10.1037/a0025738CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duits, P., Cath, D. C., Lissek, S., Hox, J. J., Hamm, A. O., Engelhard, I. M., … Baas, J. M. (2015). Updated meta-analysis of classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 32, 239253. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dymond, S., Dunsmoor, J. E., Vervliet, B., Roche, B., & Hermans, D. (2015). Fear generalization in humans: Systematic review and implications for anxiety disorder research. Behavior Therapy, 46, 561582. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2014.10.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, C. K. (2022). Applied missing data analysis. New York, New York: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Enders, C. K., & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods, 12(2), 121.10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grillon, C., Ameli, R., Goddard, A., Woods, S. W., & Davis, M. (1994). Baseline and fear-potentiated startle in panic disorder patients. Biological Psychiatry, 35(7), 431439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guthrie, R. M., & Bryant, R. A. (2006). Extinction learning before trauma and subsequent posttraumatic stress. Psychosomatic Medicine. 68(2):307311. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000208629.67653.cc. PMID: 16554398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamaker, E. L. (2012). Why researchers should think within-person: A paradigmatic view. In Mehl, M. R. & Conner, T. S. (Eds.), Handbook of research methods for studying daily life (pp. 4361), New York: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Heinig, I., Pittig, A., Richter, J., Hummel, K., Alt, I., Dickhöver, K., … Wittchen, H. U. (2017). Optimizing exposure-based CBT for anxiety disorders via enhanced extinction: Design and methods of a multicentre randomized clinical trial. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 26(2), e1560.10.1002/mpr.1560CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, L., & Rovine, M. J. (2007). Multilevel models for the experimental psychologist: Foundations and illustrative examples. Behavior Research Methods, 39(1), 101117.10.3758/BF03192848CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593602.10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lommen, M. J., van de Schoot, R., & Engelhard, I. M. (2014). The experience of traumatic events disrupts the measurement invariance of a posttraumatic stress scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1304. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lonsdorf, T. B., Menz, M. M., Andreatta, M., Fullana, M. A., Golkar, A., Haaker, J., … Merz, C. J. (2017). Don't fear ‘fear conditioning’: Methodological considerations for the design and analysis of studies on human fear acquisition, extinction, and return of fear. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 77, 247285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marciniak, M., Lage, M. J., Landbloom, R. P., Dunayevich, E., & Bowman, L. (2004). Medical and productivity costs of anxiety disorders: Case-control study. Depression and Anxiety, 19(2), 112120.10.1002/da.10131CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Modecki, K. L., Duvenage, M., Uink, B., Barber, B. L., & Donovan, C. L. (2022). Adolescents’ online coping: When less is more but none is worse. Clinical Psychological Science, 10(3), 467481.10.1177/21677026211028983CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Modecki, K. L., Goldberg, R. E., Ehrenreich, S. E., Russell, M., & Bellmore, A. (2019). The practicalities and perils of ambulatory assessment's promise. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 29(3), 542550.10.1111/jora.12532CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myin-Germeys, I., Kasanova, Z., Vaessen, T., Vachon, H., Kirtley, O., Viechtbauer, W., & Reininghaus, U. (2018). Experience sampling methodology in mental health research: New insights and technical developments. World Psychiatry, 17(2), 123132.10.1002/wps.20513CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myin-Germeys, I., Oorschot, M., Collip, D., Lataster, J., Delespaul, P., & Van Os, J. (2009). Experience sampling research in psychopathology: Opening the black box of daily life. Psychological Medicine, 39(9), 15331547.10.1017/S0033291708004947CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumann, D. L., & Waters, A. M. (2006). The use of an unpleasant sound as an unconditional stimulus in a human aversive Pavlovian conditioning procedure. Biological Psychology, 73(2), 175185.10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.03.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, M. G., Jacobson, N. C., Zainal, N. H., Shin, K. E., Szkodny, L. E., & Sliwinski, M. J. (2019). The effects of worry in daily life: An ecological momentary assessment study supporting the tenets of the contrast avoidance model. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(4), 794810.10.1177/2167702619827019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, M. G., & Llera, S. J. (2011). A novel theory of experiential avoidance in generalized anxiety disorder: A review and synthesis of research supporting a contrast avoidance model of worry. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(3), 371382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Öhman, A. (1983). Evaluating evaluative conditioning: Some comments on” cognitions, evaluations, and conditioning: Rules of sequence and rules of consequence” by Levey and Martin. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 4, 213218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Öhman, A., & Bohlin, G. (1973). Magnitude and habituation of the orienting reaction as predictors of discriminative electrodermal conditioning. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 5, 293299.Google Scholar
Orr, S. P., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., Pineles, S. L., Chang, Y., & Pitman, R. K. (2012). Predicting post-trauma stress symptoms from pre-trauma psychophysiologic reactivity, personality traits and measures of psychopathology. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 2, 8. https://doi.org/10.1186/2045-5380-2-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prokasy, W. F. (1977). First interval skin conductance responses: Conditioned or orienting responses?. Psychophysiology, 14(4), 360367.10.1111/j.1469-8986.1977.tb02965.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prokasy, W. F., & Kumpfer, K. L. (1973). Classical conditioning. In Prokasy, W. F. & Raskin, D. C. (Eds.), Electrodermal activity in psychological research (pp. 157202). New York: Academic Press.10.1016/B978-0-12-565950-5.50008-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheveneels, S., Boddez, Y., & Hermans, D. (2021). Predicting clinical outcomes via human fear conditioning: A narrative review. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 142, Article 103870. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2021.103870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheveneels, S., Boddez, Y., Vervliet, B., & Hermans, D. (2016). The validity of laboratory-based treatment research: Bridging the gap between fear extinction and exposure treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 86, 8794. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2016.08.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneiders, J., Nicolson, N. A., Berkhof, J., Feron, F. J., Van Os, J., & Devries, M. W. (2006). Mood reactivity to daily negative events in early adolescence: Relationship to risk for psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 42(3), 543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberger, C. D., Jacobs, G., Russell, S., & Crane, R. S. (1983). Assessment of anger: The state-trait anger scale. Advances in Personality Assessment, 2, 161189.Google Scholar
Telford, C. R. R. G., McCarthy-Jones, S., Corcoran, R., & Rowse, G. (2012). Experience sampling methodology studies of depression: The state of the art. Psychological Medicine, 42(6), 11191129.10.1017/S0033291711002200CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uink, B., Modecki, K. L., Barber, B. L., & Correia, H. M. (2018). Socioeconomically disadvantaged adolescents with elevated externalizing symptoms show heightened emotion reactivity to daily stress: An experience sampling study. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 49(5), 741756.10.1007/s10578-018-0784-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uink, B. N., Modecki, K. L., & Barber, B. L. (2017). Disadvantaged youth report less negative emotion to minor stressors when with peers: An experience sampling study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 41(1), 4151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vervliet, B., Craske, M. G., & Hermans, D. (2013). Fear extinction and relapse: State of the art. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 215248.10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185542CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waters, A. M., & Craske, M. G. (2016). Towards a cognitive-learning formulation of anxiety disorders in youth: A narrative review of theory and evidence and implications for treatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 50, 5066.10.1016/j.cpr.2016.09.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waters, A. M., Nazarian, M., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R. E., Griffith, J. W., Naliboff, B., … Craske, M. G. (2014). Context and explicit threat cue modulation of the startle reflex: Preliminary evidence of distinctions between adolescents with principal fear disorders versus distress disorders. Psychiatry Research, 217(1-2), 9399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waters, A. M., Theresiana, C., Neumann, D. L., & Craske, M. G. (2017). Developmental differences in aversive conditioning, extinction, and reinstatement: A study with children, adolescents, and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 263278.10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. First interval skin conductance responses to CSM and CSP during acquisition and anxiety models

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Anxiety change following no stress and a stressor as a function of low and high skin conductance responses during the early and late stage of the CS- (upper panels) and CS + lower panels during conditioning (left panels) and extinction (right panels).

Figure 2

Table 2. Last interval skin conductance responses to CSM and CSP during acquisition and anxiety models

Figure 3

Table 3. First interval skin conductance responses to CSM and CSP during extinction and anxiety models

Figure 4

Table 4. Last interval skin conductance responses to CSM and CSP during extinction and anxiety models

Supplementary material: File

Modecki et al. supplementary material

Modecki et al. supplementary material

Download Modecki et al. supplementary material(File)
File 973.4 KB