Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T14:45:40.666Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Holocene Selection for Variants Associated With General Cognitive Ability: Comparing Ancient and Modern Genomes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 July 2017

Michael A. Woodley*
Affiliation:
Unz Foundation, Palo Alto, CA, USA Center Leo Apostel for Interdisciplinary Studies, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
Shameem Younuskunju
Affiliation:
Weill Cornell Medicine, Cornell University, Dohar, Qatar
Bipin Balan
Affiliation:
Department of Agriculture and Forestry Science, University of Palermo, PalermoItaly
Davide Piffer*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Ben Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
*
address for correspondence: Michael Woodley of Menie, Center Leo Apostel for Interdisciplinary Studies, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium. E-mail: Michael.Woodley@vub.ac.be
Davide Piffer, Department of Psychology, Ben Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel. E-mail: piffer@post.bgu.ac.il

Abstract

Human populations living during the Holocene underwent considerable microevolutionary change. It has been theorized that the transition of Holocene populations into agrarianism and urbanization brought about culture-gene co-evolution that favored via directional selection genetic variants associated with higher general cognitive ability (GCA). To examine whether GCA might have risen during the Holocene, we compare a sample of 99 ancient Eurasian genomes (ranging from 4.56 to 1.21 kyr BP) with a sample of 503 modern European genomes (Fst = 0.013), using three different cognitive polygenic scores (130 SNP, 9 SNP and 11 SNP). Significant differences favoring the modern genomes were found for all three polygenic scores (odds ratios = 0.92, p = 001; .81, p = 037; and .81, p = .02 respectively). These polygenic scores also outperformed the majority of scores assembled from random SNPs generated via a Monte Carlo model (between 76.4% and 84.6%). Furthermore, an indication of increasing positive allele count over 3.25 kyr was found using a subsample of 66 ancient genomes (r = 0.22, pone-tailed = .04). These observations are consistent with the expectation that GCA rose during the Holocene.

Type
Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2017

The Holocene (11.7 kyr BP to the present) was a time of considerable microevolutionary change among human populations (Hawks et al., Reference Hawks, Wang, Cochran, Harpending and Moyzis2007). Based on various converging lines of molecular evidence, it has been estimated that the rate of adaptive evolution among these populations may have been 100 times greater than during the preceding Pleistocene (Cochran & Harpending, Reference Cochran and Harpending2009; Frost, Reference Frost2011; Hawks et al., Reference Hawks, Wang, Cochran, Harpending and Moyzis2007). Novel adaptations that are known to have arisen and spread during this period include lactase persistence (McCracken, Reference McCracken1971) and alterations in hemoglobin, permitting enhanced tolerance to diminished oxygen levels among populations living at high altitude (Bigham et al., Reference Bigham, Bauchet, Pinto, Mao, Akey and Shriver2010).

Other significant adaptations that may have been favored by selection during this period are the learning and problem-solving mechanisms that give rise to general cognitive ability (GCA). At the level of individual differences in cognitive performance, GCA is characterized by positive correlations among different measures of cognitive abilities (Jensen, Reference Jensen1998). GCA is highly heritable, with behavior genetic twin studies indicating that between 22% and 88% of its variance, depending on age, may be due to the action of additive genetic effects (Bouchard, Reference Bouchard2004). Consistent with this, studies employing genome-wide complex trait analysis genomic-relatedness-based restricted maximum-likelihood (GCTA GREML) have found that a significant amount of the variance in GCA can be directly attributed to large numbers of variants with individually small, but cumulatively large (additive) effects on the phenotype (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Tenesa, Payton, Yang, Harris and Deary2011).

GCA is not just present in human populations, but is present (at lower levels) also in a variety of animal taxa, including chimpanzees and other primates, raccoons, mice, rats, and ravens (Galsworthy et al., Reference Galsworthy, Arden, Chabris, Finkel and Reynolds2014). The existence of these species’ differences furthermore indicates that GCA has been under directional selection, particularly in the primate clade. This seems to be especially true in the case of abilities associated with tool use, which in primates are among the most strongly associated with GCA, in addition to being the most heritable. They are also associated with the strongest signals of recent directional selection (Fernandes et al., Reference Fernandes, Woodley and te Nijenhuis2014; Woodley of Menie et al., Reference Woodley of Menie, Fernandes and Hopkins2015). This is consistent with the theoretical expectation (Geary, Reference Geary2005) that GCA is an adaptation to coping with evolutionarily novel challenges—these being challenges that occur infrequently throughout the phylogeny of a lineage—thus cannot be solved with recourse to specialized cognitive systems adapted to coping with recurrent problems (such as those associated with cheater detection or language acquisition). Instead, such problems require generalized and open-ended problem-solving systems, such as learning and working memory, in order to tailor solutions to them. The ability to innovate a solution to such a problem (via the development of a tool) is a key manifestation of the action of these generalized problem-solving systems (Geary, Reference Geary2005).

The Holocene is believed to have been an environment of evolutionary relevance for GCA as it was characterized by major transitions among human populations, especially in Eurasia, away from a hunter-gatherer mode of subsistence, towards a sedentary agriculture-based one and beyond that to urbanization (Cochran and Harpending, Reference Cochran and Harpending2009; Hawks et al., Reference Hawks, Wang, Cochran, Harpending and Moyzis2007). This brought with it many evolutionarily novel problems, such as having to cope with increased population densities (Cochran and Harpending, Reference Cochran and Harpending2009; Hawks et al., Reference Hawks, Wang, Cochran, Harpending and Moyzis2007). Innovations that played a major role in facilitating this transition would have included the domestication of cultivars and animals and the development of novel tools for raising the productivity of land (such as the plough; Cochran and Harpending, Reference Cochran and Harpending2009). Cultural innovations such as monotheism, monarchy, aristocracy, feudalism, and currency-based economics arose in response to the need for coping with the hierarchical power distribution characteristic of large, static populations (Cochran and Harpending, Reference Cochran and Harpending2009). Those populations that were successful in using innovations to solve novel problems would furthermore have had an advantage in warfare, being better able to innovate weapons and tactics, allowing them to replace less successful populations. Population growth would have increased the chances of rare GCA-enhancing mutations arising, which would have favored the aggregate fitness of those populations, permitting them to expand to the greatest extent (Cochran and Harpending, Reference Cochran and Harpending2009; Darlington, Reference Darlington1970; Hawks et al., Reference Hawks, Wang, Cochran, Harpending and Moyzis2007). Thus, positive culture-gene co-evolutionary feedback occurring during the Holocene might have rapidly increased GCA (Cochran and Harpending, Reference Cochran and Harpending2009). Consistent with this, there is evidence that contemporary populations, which historically made the transition from a hunter-gatherer to an agrarian subsistence paradigm, have higher frequencies of working memory-enhancing genetic variants, compared to populations that never or only recently made this transition (Piffer, Reference Piffer2013).

High-resolution genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have identified a number of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) primarily associated with the development of the central nervous system that predict variance in both GCA and educational attainment—with which GCA shares approximately 60% of its linkage-pruned genetic variance (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Marioni, Liewald, Hill, Hagenaars and Deary2016; Domingue et al., Reference Domingue, Belsky, Conley, Harris and Boardman2015; Okbay et al., Reference Okbay, Beauchamp, Fontana, Lee, Pers and Benjamin2016; Rietveld et al., Reference Rietveld, Medland, Derringer, Yang, Esko and Koellinger2013, Reference Rietveld, Esko, Davies, Pers, Turley and Koellinger2014; Selzam et al., Reference Selzam, Krapohl, von Stumm, O'Reilly, Rimfeld and Plomin2017). These SNPs can be concatenated into cognitive polygenic scores (henceforth POLYCOG). The term ‘cognitive’ is being used here as a broader term than GCA, as it includes not only the cognitive ability measured by standardized tests but also other sources of variance that contribute to educational attainment. Polygenic scores are more or less normally distributed and can be used as a molecular index of GCA and educational attainment in standard regression-type analyses.

Recent advances in the recovery of ancient DNA have led to the accumulation of a wealth of genomic data on ancient human and other hominid populations living in the Holocene and prior epochs. These data permit comparisons to be made between ancient and modern human populations using POLYCOG, thus permitting the question of whether GCA rose during the Holocene to be tested directly. This analysis will here be conducted via the comparison of an ancient Eurasian genome sample from between 4.56 and 1.21 kyr BP with a modern, ancestrally matched European one.

Materials and Methods

Modern Genomes

Previous studies comparing ancient and modern genomes have utilized the Phase3 1000 Genomes dataset (1000 Genomes Project Consortium, 2015) as a modern reference set, on the basis that it is the most globally representative set of genomes currently available (Allentoft et al., Reference Allentoft, Sikora, Sjögren, Rasmussen, Rasmussen and Willerslev2015). The 2,504 samples in the Phase3 release are sourced from 26 populations, which can be categorized into five super-populations by continent. These are East Asian (EAS), South Asian (SAS), African (AFR), European (EUR), and American (AMR). These genomes were obtained in the form of VCF files, via the online 1000 Genomes FTP. The EUR sample is comprised of 503 individuals from five different countries (Finland, Great Britain, Italy, Spain, and the United States—specifically, north-western Europeans from Utah).

Ancient Genomes

Data on the ancient Holocene genomes were obtained from the European Nucleotide Archive (Leinonen et al., Reference Leinonen, Akhtar, Birney, Bower, Cedeno-Tárraga and Cochrane2011) in the form of BAM files (accession number PRJEB9021). These genomes, which have been utilized in previous research to analyze the spread of variants associated with skin and eye color, among other traits (Allentoft et al., Reference Allentoft, Sikora, Sjögren, Rasmussen, Rasmussen and Willerslev2015), were aged between 4.56 and 1.21 kyr (M = 3.44 kyr, SD = 0.62 kyr), covering the early Bronze to early Iron Ages. Their genome-wide coverage is relatively low (0.01–7.4X average depth, overall average = 0.7X; Allentoft et al., Reference Allentoft, Sikora, Sjögren, Rasmussen, Rasmussen and Willerslev2015). There are 102 genomes in total; however, two samples (RISE507 and RISE508) were sourced from the same individual (Allentoft et al., Reference Allentoft, Sikora, Sjögren, Rasmussen, Rasmussen and Willerslev2015). The total allele count was therefore calculated employing the full sample, alternately using each version of the genome—the results of the two analyses were then averaged. The majority of the ancient genomes were sourced from regions that are presently considered to be parts of Europe, the remainder being sourced from Central Asia. Late Bronze Age European and Central Asian gene pools resemble present-day Eurasian genetic structure (Allentoft et al., Reference Allentoft, Sikora, Sjögren, Rasmussen, Rasmussen and Willerslev2015). Consistent with this, the Fst values range from 0 to 0.08, when present-day 1000 Genomes EUR are compared with the various populations comprising the Ancient samples, indicating little to modest genetic differentiation (little differentiation corresponds to an Fst range of 0 to 0.05, and modest to an Fst range of 0.05 to 0.15; Hartl & Clark, Reference Hartl and Clark1989). These values are lower than the distance between present-day Europeans and East Asians (Fst = 0.11; Allentoft et al., Reference Allentoft, Sikora, Sjögren, Rasmussen, Rasmussen and Willerslev2015). The two ancient genomes belonging to the Siberian Okunevo culture (RISE515 and RISE516) were somewhat exceptional however, exhibiting modest differentiation relative to the EUR sample when compared with the sample size and coverage weighted average Fst value computed for the additional 92 ancient genomes for which these data were available (Fst = 0.074 vs. 0.013 for the remainder of the sample). On this basis, they were removed from the final ancient sample in order to reduce the genetic differentiation between the ancient and modern samples. The final sample of ancient genomes was therefore comprised of 99 genomes, sourced from sites located in present-day Armenia (8%), Czech Republic (6%), Denmark (6%), Estonia (1%), Germany (10%), Hungary (10%), Italy (3%), Kazakhstan (1%), Lithuania (1%), Montenegro (2%), Poland (7%), Russia (36%), and Sweden (8%).

Reference Genome

Paired-end reads are aligned to the reference human genome obtained (in the form of a FASTA file) from the UCSC database (GRCh37/hg19).

Read Realignment and Base Recalibration

After removing duplicate reads, the reads were realigned around the known indels from the 1000 Genome sample using the GenomeAnalysisTKLite-2.3-9 (GATK) toolkit. The known indels set was obtained from the GATK resource page (McKenna et al., Reference McKenna, Hanna, Banks, Sivachenko, Cibulskis and DePristo2010). After performing realignment, the base recalibration step is performed (DePristo et al., Reference DePristo, Banks, Poplin, Garimella, Maguire and Daly2011; Van der Auwera et al., Reference Van der Auwera, Carneiro, Hartl, Poplin, Del Angel and DePristo2013). Known variant position is taken into account to recalibrate the quality score.

Variant Calling and Computing POLYCOG

After performing realignment, the GenomeAnalysis- TKLite-2.3-9 toolkit UnifiedGenotyper was used to identify the SNPs and short indels. The positive allele counts (positive here refers to the sign on the β value derived from GWAS regressions—indicating that they increase the trait of interest) and allele frequencies of the target SNPs were calculated from the Ancient Genome VCF files and also from the 1000 Genomes VCF files using a custom-made PERL script (available on request). Variant calling was used to construct the first POLYCOG score from a list of 162 SNPs that were found to have high genome wide significance (p ≤ 5 × 10−8) as ‘hits’ for educational attainment in a recent large-scale, meta-analytic GWAS study (Okbay et al., Reference Okbay, Beauchamp, Fontana, Lee, Pers and Benjamin2016). After controlling for ascertainment bias by excluding those variants that were completely absent from the ancient genomes (due to the ancient samples having approximately 30% of the 1000 Genomes coverage) the resultant POLYCOG score consisted of 130 ‘hits’ that were present to some degree in both the ancient and modern genomes.

A second POLYCOG score was constructed using data from another GWAS study of educational attainment and related phenotypes (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Marioni, Liewald, Hill, Hagenaars and Deary2016). In this study, 1,115 SNPs reached GWAS significance, of which 15 were independent (linkage pruned) signals. Four SNPs were absent from the ancient genomes; thus, a POLYCOG was calculated using the remaining 11 SNPs. A third POLYCOG score was constructed utilizing a technique developed in Piffer (Reference Piffer2015, Reference Piffer2017; cf. Woodley of Menie, Piffer et al., Reference Woodley of Menie, Piffer, Peñaherrera and Rindermann2016), employing only the nine GWAS hits from Okbay et al. (Reference Okbay, Beauchamp, Fontana, Lee, Pers and Benjamin2016) that were in close linkage disequilibrium (linkage cut-off r ≥ 0.8, 500 kb linkage window) with ‘hits’ predicting educational attainment across two other large GWAS studies (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Marioni, Liewald, Hill, Hagenaars and Deary2016; Rietveld et al., Reference Rietveld, Medland, Derringer, Yang, Esko and Koellinger2013). Linkage was determined using the NIH LDLink program (Machiela & Chanock, Reference Machiela and Chanock2015) with the 1000 Genomes Phase3 CEU population as a reference group. These are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1 Nine Linked Replicated SNPs With Linkage Disequilibrium (D′) and R 2 Coefficients

By focusing on only the smaller numbers of linked replicates, the third POLYCOG score was developed as a means of negating the likely high numbers of false positive SNPs present in larger POLYCOG scores. This approach is based on maximizing the signal of directional selection by focusing on the GWAS hits with the highest significance (or replication rate across databases), as opposed to maximizing the amount of variance explained, which instead is the aim of the classical within-population GWAS design. While it is true that the effect of selection on a phenotype is proportional to the additive genetic variance, the selection coefficient for a given SNP is proportional to the amount of variance explained by that SNP. Since the rate of allele frequency change is proportional to the selection coefficient, and because both GWAS data and evolutionary processes are notoriously very noisy, it is important to maximize signal by limiting the analysis to the SNPs with higher GWAS significance, which are also more likely to account for phenotypic variance. This is the same reason why detecting selection on phenotypes that are influenced by a few genes with large effects can be accomplished with traditional, less sensitive tools (e.g., Fst index) than detecting a signal of polygenic selection (Berg & Coop, Reference Berg and Coop2014).

Analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using R v. 3.3.2 (R Core Team, 2016). In order to test the hypothesis that POLYCOG had different proportions of the positive and negative alleles between the two populations against the null hypothesis that there would be no differences, we used Fisher's Exact and chi-squared tests, since the data are binary (positive vs. negative allele/ancient vs. modern populations). At the same time, the contingency table dealt with unequal representation of genomes for each SNP within the ancient group (i.e., some SNPs were called if they were present in only one individual) by incorporating this information (each SNP is assigned a weight proportional to the number of genomes in which the SNP was called); odds ratios (OR) computed utilizing Fisher's Exact Test were preferred over the chi-squared test because it allows exact estimation of the likelihood of the null hypothesis, rather than relying on an approximation. An alternative to Fisher's Exact test is the log-likelihood ratio G-test, which is employed here as an additional test of significance. This was implemented using the R package DescTools (Signorell, Reference Signorell2016). Additional analyses were also performed on the two smaller POLYCOG (11 and 9 SNPs) using two unconditional tests (considered more powerful alternatives); specifically, Barnard's (Reference Barnard1945, Reference Barnard1947) and Boschloo's (Reference Boschloo1970) Exact tests, utilizing the R package Exact (Calhoun, Reference Calhoun2016).

In constructing POLYCOG for conventional GWAS-type analyses, it is common practice to weight each SNP by its associated GWAS regression β value, as these polygenic scores are typically the sum of very large numbers of SNPs with highly heterogeneous effect sizes as predictors of the phenotype of interest (e.g., Okbay et al., Reference Okbay, Beauchamp, Fontana, Lee, Pers and Benjamin2016). These regression weights were not found to significantly correlate with the frequency differences between modern and ancient populations for the largest of our POLYCOG, however (130 SNP; r = -0.128, p = .07), likely because selecting only the SNPs with the highest p values introduced range restriction among the regression weights. Therefore, given that the SNPs comprising our POLYCOG could be considered to be roughly ‘equally good’ as predictors of the trait of interest, the inclusion of regression β in the analysis as weights is therefore unlikely to add additional power, which in turn justifies the use of a more straightforward and also parsimonious binarization approach in comparing the ancient and modern genomes.

A second analysis was carried out on a subsample of 66 ancient genomes for which sample age was known (age range: 1.21–4.46 kyr). The correlation was computed between individual positive allele counts derived using the 130 SNP POLYCOG with genome age (ascertained by utilizing radiocarbon dating), in order to test for change over time. Since on average the ancient samples had approximately 30% of the 1000 Genomes coverage, carrying out this analysis using the smaller POLYCOG would add too much noise and produce unreliable estimates for individual genomes. Two additional genomes for which age data were present (RISE413 and RISE42) were identified as having significantly outlying values of positive allele count based on the outlier-labeling rule (Hoaglin et al., Reference Hoaglin, Iglewicz and Tukey1986) because there was only one target SNP present per genome. For the analysis only genomes whose coverage was limited to two or more of the 130 SNPs were retained. Removing these two outlying genomes also reduced the skew of the regression residual from 1.16 to 0.698, making the remaining dataset more amenable to parametric regression.

Random SNP Polygenic Scores

As an additional robustness test, a method developed by Piffer (Reference Piffer2017) was employed, in which the Monte Carlo model was run using randomly drawn and non-overlapping sets of SNPs matched for minor allele frequency (MAF). The expectation is that the three POLYCOG should outperform the majority of random polygenic scores in terms of revealing a frequency difference, favoring modern populations, consistent with theory. The random polygenic scores were computed for non-overlapping sets of SNPs for the modern and ancient populations and for each allele (A, B). A 2 × 2 matrix was computed for each comparison (Allele A/Ancient population, Allele A/Modern population, Allele B/Ancient population, Allele B/Modern population). As with the POLYCOG, OR for each matrix were then computed. Finally, the proportion of ORs with a weaker effect (i.e., higher OR value) than what was observed for the three POLYCOG was computed. Frequency matching was carried out using SNPSnap (Pers et al., Reference Pers, Timshel and Hirschhorn2015), by entering the nine linked replicate SNPs, which it must be recalled are likely the closest SNPs to the true causal variants, and setting LD at r 2 < 0.1 (for EUR). This resulted in a total of 7,369 SNPs being retrieved.

Results

A 2 × 2 contingency table was created for each of three alternative POLYCOG assigning allele counts by GWAS effect status (positive or negative effect) to ancient and modern genomes. OR of the association between positive or negative alleles and ancient or modern populations were computed and are reported in Table 1. As Fisher's Exact and chi-squared tests revealed nearly identical results, only the former are reported. In addition, the G-test (log-likelihood ratio) is reported as an alternative test of the significance of the difference. As a further robustness test, the difference in POLYCOG between the two sets of genomes was examined using the unconditional Barnard's and Boschloo's Exact tests. These statistics are extremely computationally intensive when large numbers of pair-wise comparisons are employed; therefore, they could only be used for the two smaller POLYCOG (9 and 11 SNPs). The results were identical to those obtained via the conditional techniques.

The Monte Carlo model was run with 6,740 random SNPs (after removal of the ones absent from the ancient genome in order to control for ascertainment bias). Hence, 748, 613, and 52 random SNP polygenic scores (corresponding to the 9, 11, and 130 SNPs POLYCOG, respectively) were computed. The POLYCOG outperformed the random polygenic scores (producing lower OR values) in the majority of instances (9 SNPs = 76.4 % [572 out of 749 draws]; 130 SNPs = 84.6% [44 out of 52 draws]; 11 SNPs = 77.8% [477 out of 613 draws]), which indicates a low probability that the results presented in Table 2 are due to chance.

TABLE 2 Two by Two Contingency Tables With Positive and Negative GWAS Effect Allele Counts for Ancient and Modern Genomes for Each of the Three POLYCOG, Along with Fisher's Exact Test OR and G-Test Log-Likelihood Ratios

Note: The non-integer allele counts result from averaging the genomes of two samples (RISE507 and RISE508) that came from the same individual. The samples had different allele counts likely resulting from different coverage.

These results indicate that modern European genomes have higher POLYCOG relative to ancient ones (sourced from Europe and Central Asia).

A second analysis was conducted to investigate the association between POLYCOG and sample year within the subset of 66 ancient genomes for which radiocarbon age estimates were available (from Allentoft et al., Reference Allentoft, Sikora, Sjögren, Rasmussen, Rasmussen and Willerslev2015). If selection is operating on these variants throughout the 3.25 kyr covered by this sample, then less ancient genomes should have higher positive allele counts relative to more ancient ones. The correlation of positive allele frequency with the sample year (scaled using the BCE/CE calendar era) was in the expected positive direction (samples from more recent years had higher positive allele counts); the result is furthermore statistically significant when a one-tailed test is used (r = 0.217, 95% CI [-0.026, 0.043], p one-tailed = .04), which is theoretically justified on the basis that the direction of the effect was anticipated (Kimmel, Reference Kimmel1957). The scatter plot is presented in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1 Scatter plot of 130 SNP POLYCOG positive allele counts for 66 ancient genomes as a function of year (scaled in terms of the BCE/CE calendar eras). Note: r = 0.217, pone-tailed = .04, N = 66. Gray area around trend line corresponds to the 95% CI.

Coverage was not correlated with either genome age (r = 0.022) or positive allele count (r = -0.054). Additionally, a multiple linear regression was run, with positive allele count along with coverage and age as dependent and independent variables respectively. This did not alter the effect size compared to the sample without the outliers for models with and without coverage controlled (β = 0.209 vs. 0.211). The regression models were run with each duplicate genome (RISE508 and RISE507) separately, the resultant β values were then averaged.

Discussion

The Holocene appears to have been an environment of evolutionary relevance for GCA, as modern European genomes have higher POLYCOG relative to those sourced (predominantly) from Bronze Age Europe and Central Asia. A Monte Carlo model employing polygenic scores comprised of non-overlapping sets of random SNPs revealed that the three POLYCOG outperformed between 76.4% and 84.6% of random polygenic scores. It must be noted also that a major strength of our analysis is that it used real allele frequency data, as opposed to traditional Monte Carlo simulation-based approaches, which rely on computer-generated (virtual) data.

A second analysis revealed that within a subset of the ancient genome sample, there was a (one-tailed) significant trend toward increased positive allele counts over time. As was mentioned in the section Introduction, these SNPs are assumed to exert their effects on educational attainment mainly through GCA, but also through other traits relevant for educational attainment. While the increase in these variants over time is certainly consistent with the expectation of rising GCA, the possibility that their increase indicates a simultaneous rise in other factors that make unique contributions to educational attainment (such as ‘slow’ life history or ‘high-K’ social cognitive characteristics; Giosan, Reference Giosan2006) cannot be ruled out.

This increase in POLYCOG can realistically be accounted for in two different ways: (1) microevolutionary selection pressures on standing genetic variation, resulting in soft sweeps (Hermisson & Pennings, Reference Hermisson and Pennings2005; Orr & Betancourt, Reference Orr and Betancourt2001), and (2) population expansion, replacement, and admixture due to migration.

The idea that selection takes many millennia to produce noticeable changes in a population is at odds with data indicating that within human populations, changes in the means of heritable traits such as height, weight, cholesterol levels, blood pressure, age at first birth, and age at menopause can occur due to selection over a relatively short period of time (Stearns et al., Reference Stearns, Byars, Govindaraju and Ewbank2010). Selection operating over the course of millennia (as in the present case) would be expected to produce quite considerable microevolutionary change. As was discussed in the section Introduction, Holocene populations appear to have undergone accelerated adaptive microevolution relative to those living in the Pleistocene (Cochran & Harpending, Reference Cochran and Harpending2009; Hawks et al., Reference Hawks, Wang, Cochran, Harpending and Moyzis2007). Increasing cultural complexity and technological sophistication among these populations may therefore have arisen in part from selection favoring GCA. Cultural and technological change can in turn create, via culture-gene co-evolutionary feedback, conditions favoring higher GCA (Cochran & Harpending, Reference Cochran and Harpending2009; Piffer, Reference Piffer2013). This process likely continued until the Late Modern Era, where it has been noted that among Western populations living between the 15th and early 19th centuries, those with higher social status (which shares genetic variance with, and is therefore a proxy for GCA; Trzaskowski et al., Reference Trzaskowski, Harlaar, Arden, Krapohl, Rimfeld and Plomin2014) typically produced the most surviving offspring. These in turn tended toward downward social mobility due to intense competition, replacing the reproductively unsuccessful low-status stratum and effectively ‘bootstrapping’ those populations via the application of high levels of skill to solving problems associated with production and industry, eventually leading to the Industrial Revolution in Europe (Clark, Reference Clark2007, Reference Clark2014). The millennia-long microevolutionary trend favoring higher GCA not only ceased, but likely went into reverse among European-derived populations living in the 19th century (Lynn, Reference Lynn1996; Lynn & Van Court, Reference Lynn and Van Court2004), largely in response to factors such as the asymmetric use of birth control and prolonged exposure to education among those with high GCA (Lynn, Reference Lynn1996). Consistent with this, it has been found that various POLYCOG negatively predict reproductive success in contemporary Western populations (Beauchamp, Reference Beauchamp2016; Conley et al., Reference Conley, Laidley, Belsky, Fletcher, Boardman and Domingue2016; Kong et al., Reference Kong, Banks, Poplin, Garimella, Maguire and Daly2017; Woodley of Menie, Schwartz et al., Reference Woodley of Menie, Piffer, Peñaherrera and Rindermann2016). It is important to note that this recent microevolutionary trend (working in the opposite direction) has likely attenuated the difference in POLYCOG between the modern and ancient genomes noted in the present study.

Changes in allele frequencies can also occur via population expansion and replacement. The end of the Last Glacial Maximum initiated major cultural changes, leading to the development of agriculture, population growth, and the subsequent Neolithicization process (8–5 kyr BP), which involved the spread of genes as well as culture from the Middle East (Sokal et al., Reference Sokal, Oden and Wilson1991). Another big cultural change associated with major population movements was the Bronze Age, whose culture started replacing the Neolithic farming cultures in temperate Eastern Europe 5 kyr BP (Allentoft et al., Reference Allentoft, Sikora, Sjögren, Rasmussen, Rasmussen and Willerslev2015). This Pontic-Caspian Steppe genetic component among contemporary Europeans is associated with the Corded Ware and Yamnaya peoples, and possibly accounts for the spread of Indo-European languages (Allentoft et al., Reference Allentoft, Sikora, Sjögren, Rasmussen, Rasmussen and Willerslev2015). Being potentially highly fitness enhancing, admixture among populations may have furthermore led to the spread of the causal variants that are tapped by POLYCOG. However, recent evidence suggests that the European and Central Asian gene pools toward the end of the Bronze Age closely mirror present-day West Eurasian genetic structure (Allentoft et al., Reference Allentoft, Sikora, Sjögren, Rasmussen, Rasmussen and Willerslev2015). Hence, population movements and replacements likely played a larger role in the evolution of GCA prior to the period covered by the present sample. Sicily and Sardinia, and the southern fringes of Europe in general are notable exceptions however, as they tend to cluster genetically with Neolithic farmers (Allentoft et al., Reference Allentoft, Sikora, Sjögren, Rasmussen, Rasmussen and Willerslev2015).

The first theoretical scenario (microevolutionary change involving standing genetic variation) most likely therefore played the largest role in the POLYCOG increase during the time period covered by our samples.

It has been noted that concomitant in time with the apparent increase in POLYCOG observed presently is an apparent parallel selection trend favoring smaller brains (Hawks, Reference Hawks2011). These trends may at first appear to be contradictory, as GCA is positively associated with brain volume. It must be noted, however, that meta-analysis reveals that the phenotypic association is weak (ρ = .24; Pietschnig et al., Reference Pietschnig, Penke, Wicherts, Zeiler and Voracek2015) with the genetic correlation being slightly stronger (r g ~0.30; Okbay et al., Reference Okbay, Beauchamp, Fontana, Lee, Pers and Benjamin2016), indicating that the majority of the phenotypic and genotypic variance in brain volume is unrelated to variance in GCA. Furthermore, studies utilizing POLYCOG have found that it does not predict variation in brain volume, despite both POLYCOG and brain volume making independent contributions to GCA (Deary et al., Reference Deary, Cox and Ritchie2016). These findings suggest that declining brain volume during the Holocene may have been a consequence of enhanced brain efficiency stemming from increased corticalization and neuronal connectivity, with more bioenergetically optimized brains simply requiring less mass to achieve greater processing power. It may therefore be this process that the increase in POLYCOG is tracking in our samples. A prediction stemming from Hawks (Reference Hawks2011) is that variants that predict brain volume and not GCA should show the opposite trend in time to POLYCOG when examined in the context of the present samples.

A potential limitation of the present work is linkage disequilibrium (LD) decay. This takes place when a pair of SNPs on a chromosome in a population moves from linkage disequilibrium to linkage equilibrium over time, due to recombination events eventually occurring between every possible point on the chromosome (Bush & Moore, Reference Bush and Moore2012). LD decay can be an issue when comparing polygenic scores computed using genetically different populations (i.e., either across time or space). Since most GWAS hits are actually tag SNPs, decay in LD implies that the causal SNPs will be less efficiently flagged by the tag SNPs, resulting in the tag SNPs coming to resemble a sample of random SNPs. Since the frequency of the average SNP allele is 50%, the GWAS hits will tend to converge towards an average frequency of 50%, with increasing LD decay. The implication of this for our analysis is that our estimates of the difference in POLYCOG between the reference population (modern European) and the ancient population are therefore conservative when the mean frequency is lower than 50%, while the opposite is true when the frequency is higher (the mean frequencies in the modern 1000 Genomes samples were the following: for the 130 SNP POLYCOG = 47%, 9 SNP POLYCOG = 44%, and the 11 SNP POLYCOG = 53%). Furthermore, because GCA likely involves around 10,000 causal variants (Hsu, Reference Hsu2014), and as there is relatively little genetic differentiation between the ancient and modern genomes, as indicated by the sample size and coverage weighted Fst value of 0.013, the results of simulations indicate that LD decay is ultimately expected to be minimal (Scutari et al., Reference Scutari, Mackay and Balding2016) and should not be confounding the results.

The present effort constitutes a direct attempt to show the theoretically anticipated change in POLYCOG over time utilizing relatively simple methodology, which makes few modeling assumptions. The difference in POLYCOG between the ancient and modern genomes is indifferent to the use of alternate versions of POLYCOG and to the use of various statistical techniques for quantifying the significance of the group difference. The results of the Monte Carlo model indicate that the majority of frequency-matched random SNP polygenic scores underperform relative to the POLYCOG. Among a subsample of the ancient genomes, indications of increasing positive allele counts as a function of time are also present. It is acknowledged that there are limitations in the data employed—larger samples of ancient genomes would certainly increase confidence in the present finding. Furthermore, it is acknowledged that any of the discrete lines of evidence that have been brought to bear on the question can, in isolation, of course be criticized, with some lines being considered weaker than others. It must be noted, however, that multiple operationalizations of the variable of interest and multiple methods have been employed here, which, even if each individual line of evidence is to be considered ‘weak’, should nevertheless greatly increase confidence in our conclusion when the evidence is considered collectively as part of a larger nomological net work (Cronbach & Meehl, Reference Cronbach and Meehl1955).

Footnotes

Joint first author.

References

1000 Genomes Project Consortium. (2015). A global reference for human genetic variation. Nature, 526, 6874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allentoft, M. E., Sikora, M., Sjögren, K.-G., Rasmussen, S., Rasmussen, M., . . . Willerslev, E. (2015). Population genomics of Bronze Age Eurasia. Nature, 522, 167174.Google Scholar
Barnard, G. A. (1945). A new test for 2 × 2 tables. Nature, 156, 783784.Google Scholar
Barnard, G. A. (1947). Significance tests for 2 × 2 tables. Biometrika, 34, 123138.Google Scholar
Beauchamp, J. P. (2016). Genetic evidence for natural selection in humans in the contemporary United States. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113, 77747779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berg, J. J., & Coop, G. (2014). A population genetic signal of polygenic adaptation. PLoS Genetics, 10, e1004412.Google Scholar
Bigham, A., Bauchet, M., Pinto, D., Mao, X., Akey, J. M., . . . Shriver, M. D. (2010). Identifying signatures of natural selection in Tibetan and Andean populations using dense genome scan data. PLOS Genetics, 6, e1001116.Google Scholar
Boschloo, R. D. (1970). Raised conditional level of significance for the 2 × 2-table when testing the equality of two probabilities. Statistics Neerlandica, 24, 135.Google Scholar
Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (2004). Genetic influence on human psychological traits – A survey. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 148151.Google Scholar
Bush, W. S., & Moore, J. H. (2012). Chapter 11: Genome-wide association studies. PLoS Computational Biology, 8, e1002822.Google Scholar
Calhoun, P. (2016). Exact: unconditional exact test. R Package Version 1.7. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=Exact Google Scholar
Clark, G. (2007). Farewell to alms: A brief economic history of the world. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, G. (2014). The son also rises: Surnames and the history of social mobility. Princeton, NJ; Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Cochran, G., & Harpending, H. (2009). The 10,000 year explosion: How civilization accelerated human evolution. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Conley, D., Laidley, T., Belsky, D. W., Fletcher, J. M., Boardman, J. D., & Domingue, B. J. (2016). Assortative mating and differential fertility by phenotype and genotype across the 20th century. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113, 66476652.Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281302.Google Scholar
Darlington, C. D. (1970). The evolution of man and society. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Davies, G., Marioni, R. E., Liewald, D. C., Hill, W. D., Hagenaars, S. P., . . . Deary, I. J. (2016). Genome-wide association study of cognitive functions and educational attainment in UK Biobank (N = 112151). Molecular Psychiatry, 21, 758767.Google Scholar
Davies, G., Tenesa, A., Payton, A., Yang, J., Harris, S. E., . . . Deary, I. J. (2011). Genome-wide association studies establish that human intelligence is highly heritable and polygenic. Molecular Psychiatry, 16, 9961005.Google Scholar
Deary, I. J., Cox, S. R., & Ritchie, S. J. (2016). Getting Spearman off the skyhook: One more in a century (since Thomson, 1916) of attempts to vanquish g. Psychological Inquiry, 27, 192199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DePristo, M. A., Banks, E., Poplin, R., Garimella, K. V., Maguire, J. R., . . . Daly, M. J. (2011). A framework for variation discovery and genotyping using next-generation DNA sequencing data. Nature Genetics, 43, 491498.Google Scholar
Domingue, B. W., Belsky, D. W., Conley, D., Harris, K. M., & Boardman, J. D. (2015). Polygenic influence on educational attainment. AERA Open, 1, e13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandes, H. B. F., Woodley, M. A., & te Nijenhuis, J. (2014). Differences in cognitive abilities among primates are concentrated on G: Phenotypic and phylogenetic comparisons with two meta-analytical databases. Intelligence, 46, 311322.Google Scholar
Frost, P. (2011). Human nature or human natures? Futures, 43, 740748.Google Scholar
Galsworthy, M. J., Arden, R., & Chabris, C. F. (2014). Animal models of general cognitive ability for genetic research into cognitive functioning. In Finkel, D. & Reynolds, C. A. (Eds.), Behavior genetics of cognition across the lifespan (pp. 257278). New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Giosan, C. (2006). High-K strategy scale: A measure of the high-K independent criterion of fitness. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 394405.Google Scholar
Hartl, D. L., & Clark, A. G. (1989). Principles of population genetics (2nd ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Hawks, J. (2011). Selection for smaller brains in Holocene human evolution. arXiv: 1102.5604.Google Scholar
Hawks, J., Wang, E. T., Cochran, G. M., Harpending, H. C., & Moyzis, R. K. (2007). Recent acceleration of human adaptive evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 2075320758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermisson, J., & Pennings, P. S. (2005). Soft sweeps: Molecular population genetics of adaptation from standing genetic variation. Genetics, 169, 23352352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoaglin, D. C., Iglewicz, B., & Tukey, J. W. (1986). Performance of some resistant rules for outlier labeling. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 81, 991999.Google Scholar
Hsu, S. D. H. (2014). On the genetic architecture of intelligence and other quantitative traits. arXiv: 1408.3421.Google Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
Kimmel, H. D. (1957). Three criteria for the use of one-tailed tests. Psychological Bulletin, 54, 351353.Google Scholar
Kong, A., Banks, E., Poplin, R., Garimella, K. V., Maguire, J. R., . . . Daly, M. J. (2017). Selection against variants in the genome associated with educational attainment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114, E727–E732.Google ScholarPubMed
Leinonen, R., Akhtar, R., Birney, E., Bower, L., Cedeno-Tárraga, A., . . . Cochrane, G. (2011). The European Nucleotide Archive. Nucleic Acids Research, 39, D28– D31.Google Scholar
Lynn, R. (1996). Dysgenics: Genetic deterioration in modern populations. Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
Lynn, R., & Van Court, M. (2004). New evidence of dysgenic fertility for intelligence in the United States. Intelligence, 32, 193201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machiela, M. J., & Chanock, S. J. (2015). LDlink: A web-based application for exploring population-specific haplotype structure and linking correlated alleles of possible functional variants. Bioinformatics, 31, 35553557.Google Scholar
McCracken, R. D. (1971). Lactase deficiency: An example of dietary evolution. Current Anthropology, 12, 479517.Google Scholar
McKenna, A., Hanna, M., Banks, E., Sivachenko, A., Cibulskis, K., . . . DePristo, M. A. (2010). The Genome Analysis Toolkit: A MapReduce framework for analyzing next-generation DNA sequencing data. Genome Research, 20, 12971303.Google Scholar
Okbay, A., Beauchamp, J. P., Fontana, M. A., Lee, J. J., Pers, T. H., . . . Benjamin, D. J. (2016). Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature, 533, 539542.Google Scholar
Orr, H. A., & Betancourt, A. J. (2001). Haldane's sieve and adaptation from the standing gene variation. Genetics, 157, 875884.Google Scholar
Pers, T. H., Timshel, P., & Hirschhorn, J. N. (2015). SNPsnap: A web-based tool for identification and annotation of SNPs. Bioinformatics, 31, 418420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pietschnig, J., Penke, L., Wicherts, J. M., Zeiler, M., & Voracek, M. (2015). Meta-analysis of associations between human brain volume and intelligence differences: How strong are they and what do they mean? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 411432.Google Scholar
Piffer, D. (2013). Correlation of the COMT Val158Met polymorphism with latitude and a hunter-gatherer lifestyle suggests culture-gene coevolution and selective pressure on cognition genes due to climate. Anthropological Science, 121, 161171.Google Scholar
Piffer, D. (2015). A review of intelligence GWAS hits: Their relationship to country IQ and the issue of spatial autocorrelation. Intelligence, 53, 4350.Google Scholar
Piffer, D. (2017). Evidence for recent polygenic selection on educational attainment and underlying cognitive abilities inferred from GWAS hits: A Monte Carlo simulation using random SNPs. Preprints 2017, 2017010127, doi:10.20944/preprints201701.0127.v3.Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rietveld, C. A., Esko, T., Davies, G., Pers, T. H., Turley, P., . . . Koellinger, P. D. (2014). Common genetic variants associated with cognitive performance identified using the proxy-phenotype method. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States America, 111, 1379013794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rietveld, C. A., Medland, S. E., Derringer, J., Yang, J., Esko, T., . . . Koellinger, P. D. (2013). GWAS of 126,559 individuals identifies genetic variants associated with educational attainment. Science, 340, 14671471.Google Scholar
Scutari, M., Mackay, I., & Balding, D. (2016). Using genetic distance to infer the accuracy of genomic prediction. PLOS Genetics, 12, e1006288.Google Scholar
Selzam, S., Krapohl, E., von Stumm, S., O'Reilly, P. F., Rimfeld, K., . . . Plomin, R. (2017). Predicting educational achievement from DNA. Molecular Psychiatry, 22, 267272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Signorell, A. (2016). DescTools: Tools for descriptive statistics. R package version 0.99.18. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/package=DescTools Google Scholar
Sokal, R. R., Oden, N. L., & Wilson, C. (1991). Genetic evidence for the spread of agriculture in Europe by demic diffusion. Nature, 351, 143145.Google Scholar
Stearns, S. C., Byars, S. G., Govindaraju, D. R., & Ewbank, D. (2010). Measuring selection in contemporary human populations. Nature Reviews Genetics, 11, 611622.Google Scholar
Trzaskowski, M., Harlaar, N., Arden, R., Krapohl, E., Rimfeld, K., . . . Plomin, R. (2014). Genetic influence on family socioeconomic status and children's intelligence. Intelligence, 42, 8388.Google Scholar
Van der Auwera, G. A., Carneiro, M. O., Hartl, C., Poplin, R., Del Angel, G., . . . DePristo, M. A. (2013). From FastQ data to high-confidence variant calls: The genome analysis toolkit best practices pipeline. Current Protocols in Bioinformatics, 43, 11.10.1–11.10.33.Google Scholar
Woodley of Menie, M. A., Fernandes, H. B. F., & Hopkins, W. D. (2015). The more g-loaded, the more heritable, evolvable, and phenotypically variable: Homology with humans in chimpanzee cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 50, 159163.Google Scholar
Woodley of Menie, M. A., Piffer, D., Peñaherrera, M. A., & Rindermann, H. (2016). Evidence of contemporary polygenic selection on the Big G of national cognitive ability: A cross-cultural sociogenetic analysis. Personality & Individual Differences, 102, 9097.Google Scholar
Woodley of Menie, M. A., Schwartz, J. A., & Beaver, K. M. (2016). How cognitive genetic factors influence fertility outcomes: A mediational SEM analysis. Twin Research & Human Genetics, 19, 628637.Google Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Nine Linked Replicated SNPs With Linkage Disequilibrium (D′) and R2 Coefficients

Figure 1

TABLE 2 Two by Two Contingency Tables With Positive and Negative GWAS Effect Allele Counts for Ancient and Modern Genomes for Each of the Three POLYCOG, Along with Fisher's Exact Test OR and G-Test Log-Likelihood Ratios

Figure 2

FIGURE 1 Scatter plot of 130 SNP POLYCOG positive allele counts for 66 ancient genomes as a function of year (scaled in terms of the BCE/CE calendar eras). Note: r = 0.217, pone-tailed = .04, N = 66. Gray area around trend line corresponds to the 95% CI.