Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T09:06:25.877Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Geosocietal Support for Democracy: Survey Evidence from Ukraine

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 April 2024

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

We examine why public support for democracy in Ukraine increased after Russia’s 2014 intervention and surged after Russia’s 2022 invasion—despite concerns that the wartime quest for security would diminish support for political freedoms. We statistically analyze original data generated as part of annual opinion surveys by the Institute of Sociology at Ukraine’s National Academy of Sciences in 2017 (N = 2,199), 2018 (N = 1,800), and a 2021–22 panel survey with the same respondents (N = 475) interviewed before and after Russia’s invasion. Our findings indicate that wartime support for democracy is in significant respects geosocietal—arising from the mobilization of civic national identity conditioned by salient geopolitical threats. Civic pride, attribution of threat to an external authoritarian aggressor, and war onset were the strongest and most robust predictors of multiple democracy support indicators, overriding personal loss and stress. The findings call for more attention to the interaction of geopolitical and social contexts shaping political attitudes, with implications for democratic futures globally.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of American Political Science Association

All those who seek to destroy the freedom of the democratic nations must know that war is the surest and the shortest means to accomplish this.

—Alexis de Tocqueville (1835) Reference Tocqueville and Lawrence1969, 650

Introduction

Public support has long been considered essential for the survival of democracy (Diamond Reference Diamond1999; Easton Reference Easton1975; Lipset Reference Lipset1959; Weber [1919] Reference Weber, Gerth and Wright Mills1965), a claim upheld more recently with systematic rigorous analyses of multinational, multiyear survey data (Claassen Reference Claassen2020). Legitimation of democracy, however, has been shown to decline in states experiencing or threatened with armed conflict. Large longitudinal studies found that involvement in militarized disputes dimmed the democratic prospects of states from 1816 to 1992 (Rasler and Thompson Reference Rasler and Thompson2004; Reuveny and Li Reference Reuveny and Li2003). Likewise, civil wars (Dyrstad Reference Dyrstad2013; Marshall and Cole Reference Marshall and Cole2014; Tir and Singh Reference Tir and Singh2015) and terrorist attacks (Davis and Silver Reference Davis and Silver2004; Merolla and Zechmeister Reference Merolla and Zechmeister2009) have arguably undermined democratic development. While lauding and analyzing Ukrainian society’s inspired resistance to Russia’s 2022 massive and brutal military invasion, The Economist (2022, 19) warned that “there will be a risk of backsliding on democracy and liberalism in a country which will be focused on its security as never before.”

That Ukraine would confound these research findings and dire warnings with sustained democratic resilience has not been a given, as it may now seem in retrospect, particularly going back to 2014. Early that year, Russia annexed Ukraine’s Crimean Peninsula and fomented armed conflict over Ukraine’s Donbas region (Kuzio Reference Kuzio2017). The ensuing Donbas war, fought along a 250-mile front, claimed over 14,000 lives and wounded 30,000; displaced over 1.3 million people within Ukraine; left 3.4 million in need of sustained humanitarian assistance; and damaged or destroyed 50,000 civilian homes from 2014 through 2021, the first two years being the deadliest and most destructive (Melnyk et al. Reference Melnyk, Pavlyuk, Petrov and Glushko2019; OHCHR 2022; Permanent Mission of Ukraine to the UN 2021).Footnote 1 Concomitant drivers of democracy erosion, such as economic stress (Collier Reference Collier2007; Collier et al. Reference Collier, Elliot, Hegre, Hoeffler, Reynal-Querol and Sambanis2003) and incentives for rule breaking (Walter Reference Walter2015), were evident. By 2017, Ukraine’s GDP contracted by half, unemployment hit 11%, the value of the currency (hryvnia) shrank threefold against the dollar, and Transparency International ranked Ukraine within the top third of the world’s most corrupt countries (Transparency International 2020). Considering those factors, the Kremlin’s strategists could plausibly imagine that Ukrainians would abandon democratic aspirations when faced with a wholesale military invasion.

And yet, democratic institutions and public support for democracy across Ukraine held up and rose sharply after Russia’s full-scale invasion of February 24, 2022. Surveys by Ukraine’s National Academy of Sciences Institute of Sociology (UNASIS) saw the percentage of respondents who considered democracy important or very important rise from an average of 67% between 2002 and 2012 to 71% between 2014 and 2021, while the percentage of those who considered freedom of expression important rose from 64% to 72% (N = 1,800/year). Following Russia’s mass invasion, those numbers rose to 81% and 87.2%, respectively, in July 2022 (N = 475).Footnote 2 Onuch (Reference Onuch2022, 37) described the rise in the public’s preference for democracy in Ukraine between 2019 and 2022 as unprecedented in democratization history.

What could explain sustained public support for democracy in the face of grave threats, devastating losses, traumas, and deprivation, as has been the case in Ukraine? Why, in the quest for security and national defense, have Ukrainians overwhelmingly mobilized for political freedoms rather than for strong, autocratic rule?

Drawing from the literature on second-image-reversed theory, rallying, and social identity, we develop an explanation recognizing that wartime democracy legitimation can be geosocietal. This term describes a causal process of democracy support arising from the combined mobilization of two dimensions of social identity: the domestic dimension (civic national identity) and the geopolitical dimension (salience of external authoritarian threats to the nation). Our analysis of multiyear survey data, including our own original questions (see section 4), yields strong evidence for this proposition. In Ukraine amid the 2014–22 Donbas war, civic national identity (pride in Ukrainian citizenship) and geopolitical threat (identification of authoritarian Russia, rather than local separatists or/and foreign volunteers, as the principal enemy) were the strongest and most robust predictors of support for democracy. We found their effects to be not only additive, but in significant ways interactive. And in our separate panel survey with the same respondents about three months before and after Russia’s full-scale invasion in February 2022 that threatened Ukraine’s very existence and mobilized intrinsic national pride, we found that support for democracy had surged across Ukraine, controlling for major confounders. We proceed to explicate the theoretical foundations of geosocietal identity mobilization for democracy, our empirical findings, and their implications for the comparative analysis of support for democracy globally.

Theoretical Framework

Principal Phenomenon under Investigation

The definition and measurement of democracy is one of the most extensively debated subjects in political science,Footnote 3 and it is not our purpose here to engage in these debates. Our focus is on the perceived desirability of democracy as a political system, one of the principal aspects of diffuse support reflecting Lipset’s (Reference Lipset1959, 83) concept of political legitimacy, a “belief that existing political institutions are the most appropriate or proper ones for the society.” A study of the panel data of 2,435 nationally aggregated estimates of support for democracy from 135 countries finds that diffuse support, when compared to other measures, is the most significant correlate of democracy’s survival and endurance (Claassen Reference Claassen2020). Corroborating those findings at the individual level, quantitative analysis of focus group narratives in Chile and Argentina indicate that desirability of democracy as a system, compared to 48 other measures, generated the most consistent variation in strongly held positive and negative orientations across multi-item democracy support profiles in both countries (Carlin Reference Carlin2018). These studies indicate that focusing on this measure is theoretically and substantively warranted and important—in no small part because it has proved to be diagnostic of real-world democratic futures regardless of what individuals specifically mean by democracy when answering survey questions.

We ask how a society’s exposure to war—focusing on ordinary civilians rather than the combatants—may affect this diffuse support for democracy at the individual level.

Theoretical Puzzle

Although we lack cumulative research on individual-level support for democracy in cases like Ukraine during Russia’s invasion, two major literatures and debates therein offer relevant insights.

First, we have research linking political preferences to generalizable psychological factors due to war conditions, notably death and destruction, which Ukraine has continually experienced since 2014 and particularly since 2022. But the assessments of specific attitudinal effects diverge. On the one hand, extensive studies show how war eroded support for democracy in different countries due to “shattered assumptions” of personal invulnerability (Janoff-Bulman Reference Janoff-Bulman1992), “the ball of rage” syndrome (Chemtob et al. Reference Chemtob, Novaco, Hamada and Gross1997), “mortality salience” (Rosenblatt et al. Reference Rosenblatt, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski and Lyon1989), personal loss and traumas (Canetti-Nisim et al. Reference Canetti-Nisim, Halperin, Sharvit and Hobfoll2009; Dyrstad Reference Dyrstad2013), social intolerance (Bakke, O’Loughlin, and Ward Reference Kristin, John and and Michael2009; Tir and Singh Reference Tir and Singh2015), the prioritizing of control over freedom (Davis and Silver Reference Davis and Silver2004), and “authoritarian thinking” (Hetherington and Suhay Reference Hetherington and Suhay2011).

On the other hand, we have research indicating that the impacts of war loss and trauma may be muted or may lay the ground for stronger support for democracy. Individuals may interpret even grave threats of violence as manageable “perturbations” that are followed by the resetting of democratic norms (Sniderman et al. Reference Sniderman, Petersen, Slothuus, Stubager and Petrov2019). The human tendency to become desensitized to repeated stimuli means political attitudes may change less as violence persists (Nussio Reference Nussio2020). Individual post-traumatic growth in response to life-threatening events may boost prodemocratic attitudes (Tedeschi and Calhoun Reference Tedeschi and Calhoun2004) and political tolerance (Carmil and Breznitz Reference Carmil and Breznitz1991).

The second category of relevant research points to generalizable social factors affecting political preferences in wartime, notably group mobilization. Here, too, conclusions diverge. On the one hand, war arguably raises public support for personalist centralized rule. External threats have been shown to bolster “rallying-’round-the-flag” behavior (Mueller Reference Mueller1970) in different contexts, with the public more willing to delegate authority to individual leaders and the executive over other institutions (Frye Reference Frye2019; Lai and Reiter Reference Lai and Reiter2005; Lambert et al. Reference Lambert, Scherer, Schott, Olson, Andrews, O’Brien and Zisser2010; Tudor and Slater Reference Tudor and Slater2021), mute political opposition (Baum and Groeling Reference Baum and Groeling2008; Brody and Shapiro Reference Brody, Shapiro and Brody1991; Oneal, Lian, and Joyner Reference Oneal, Lian and Joyner1996), and bolster social intolerance (Godefroidt Reference Godefroidt2023; Grosjean Reference Grosjean2014). Moreover, rallying for the incumbents has been shown to be more durable and intense under conditions similar to Ukraine’s, such as during major uses of military force, severe hostility between parties, and fighting over territory (Baker and Oneal Reference Baker and Oneal2001; Oneal, Lian, and Joyner Reference Oneal, Lian and Joyner1996, 265; Tir Reference Tir2012)—particularly territory of symbolic significance, such as Ukraine’s Crimea (Alexseev and Hale Reference Alexseev and Hale2019).

On the other hand, we have evidence that the same quest for group unity while under threat, often in the same or similar countries, gives rise to grassroots civic mobilization, favoring social inclusivity within a nation and strengthening liberal democratic values (Berinsky Reference Berinsky2009). Skocpol (Reference Skocpol2002) noted a 45% rise in public trust in the US government in response to the September 11 terrorist attacks and cited historical evidence that the surge in trust was even more significant during World War II, when Americans needed to “roll bandages for GIs or collect scrap metal to make airplanes.” Similar conclusions were drawn about September 11 (Gaines Reference Gaines2002; Woods Reference Woods2011), and about responses to terrorist attacks in Spain (Dinesen and Jæger Reference Dinesen and Jæger2013) and France (Van Hauwaert and Huber Reference Van Hauwaert and Huber2020). In Ukraine, Darkovich, Savisko, and Rabinovych (Reference Darkovich, Savisko and Rabinovych2023) document how Russia’s full-scale invasion boosted political decentralization as citizens mobilized predominantly around local communities (hromady), and President Volodymyr Zelensky’s popularity surged along with—and not at the expense of—support for democratic institutions and values (Onuch and Hale Reference Onuch and Hale2022).

While showing that war exposure and group mobilization are important to factor into any analysis of war effects on political attitudes, divergent findings point to a theoretical puzzle: Why would the same psychological and social factors associated with war violence affect polity preferences differently? If war brings with it insecurity and the quest for group unity across societies, why may support for democracy in any given society weaken or strengthen in wartime?

While this partly reflects the problem of drawing generalizable inferences from studies that use different variable specifications in different contexts with different data types and methods, the puzzle points to two knowledge gaps. The first one is war type: almost all the individual-level findings on how violence affects support for democracy come from cases of civil wars, insurgencies, terrorist attacks, or militarized interventions in other states.Footnote 4 However, econometric analysis (Besley and Persson Reference Besley and Persson2010) and survey research (Grosjean Reference Grosjean2014) indicate that war type may significantly affect at least some political preferences, with exposure to internal war more likely to drive social antagonisms, but exposure to interstate war more likely to favor social inclusivity. Building on these insights, we further examine how individual-level exposure to interstate wars might affect diffuse support for democracy, particularly under the threat of territorial conquest.

The second factor has to do with society and national identity mobilization. Most of the relevant rallying research focuses on the “shifts in attitudes toward the American president” (Feinstein Reference Feinstein2022; Lambert et al. Reference Lambert, Scherer, Schott, Olson, Andrews, O’Brien and Zisser2010, 886) and on leadership approval elsewhere (Feinstein Reference Feinstein2018, Frye Reference Frye2019; Hale Reference Hale2021; Lai and Reiter Reference Lai and Reiter2005). However, Godefroidt’s (Reference Godefroidt2023) meta-analysis of political attitudes under the threat of terrorism in 30 countries over 35 years (drawing on 326 studies of 400,000 individuals) showed that rallying is contingent on a country’s context and its issues. A significant “rally around President Bush” effect in response to September 11 was not observed in many other countries, particularly in relation to non-Islamist threats. Indications have emerged that rallying is multifaceted. Societies may rally not only for leaders, but also for institutions (Hetherington and Nelson Reference Hetherington and Nelson2003) and military operations (Feinstein Reference Feinstein2018). The growing salience of civic national identity in Ukraine within a year of the onset of the Donbas war in 2014 (Kulyk Reference Kulyk2016; Pop-Eleches and Robertson Reference Pop-Eleches and Robertson2018), in contrast to a drop in then president Petro Poroshenko’s support, implies that societies may literally rally around the flag and other state symbols, for nation and polity, regardless of declining support for the leader.Footnote 5 As a next step, we examine how civic identity mobilization in wartime may translate into support for democracy as a political system.

Geosocietal Support for Democracy: The Logic and the Hypotheses

To develop a generalizable explanation of Ukraine’s democratic resilience, we propose that support for democracy in a state at war can be geosocietal, in the sense that it is likely to be significantly conditioned by both the public salience of geopolitical (external) threats to democracy and the society’s capacity for civic national mobilization. The geosocietal logic indicates that the international system-level context (notably, war type, the nature of the aggressor and target states, and international alliances) would set the stage for whether civic national mobilization may affect support for democracy positively or negatively and reinforce these effects. At the same time, the society’s intrinsic capacity for civic mobilization, based in domestic conditions, will contribute significantly toward the total effect as a fundamental underlying condition.

We know from well-established literatures about the significant impacts of international-level factors—such as power balances, institutions, and norms—on the domestic structures and politics of states (known as the “second image reversed,” as described by Gourevitch [Reference Gourevitch1978]), and more specifically about the importance of war in the formation of states and national identities (Greenfeld Reference Greenfeld1993; Tilly Reference Tilly1992). Expanding on these insights, we recognize that national identity is not only intrinsic (rooted in a sense of common citizenship or traditions and amplified by common effort and interests) but also relational, grounded in the human tendency to “cleave and compare” across groups (Horowitz Reference Horowitz2001; Nair and Sambanis Reference Nair and Sambanis2019). This means national identity must entail a sense of where one’s nation-state stands in relation to others—of its distinctiveness and commonalities, its strengths and weaknesses, and the costs and benefits of its external interactions. We refer to this identity dimension as geopolitical. Factoring this dimension into the analysis of democracy support is essential to our contribution.

Thе extent to which a threat is external is a critical factor. The fundamental insight of social identity theory (SIT) (Tajfel and Turner Reference Tajfel, Turner, Worchel and Austin1986) is that invasive external (out-group) threats play a particularly strong part in identity mobilization by amplifying intergroup differences across multiple categories. The perceived degree to which the threat of war comes from another state or external actor is likely to amplify the perceived differences between the polity of the aggressor and that of the target state. Intergroup bias—a combination of out-group hostility and in-group solidarity—would then translate into individuals casting the opposing state’s polity in a more negative light, and their own polity in a more positive light, than they would in the absence of the external war threat.

The SIT logic also implies that sensitivity to external threats would depend on how salient civic national (in-group) identity is within a society before it comes under external attack. In theory, the stronger the salience of civic national identity (i.e., the degree to which citizens feel pride in their citizenship) in a democratic or democratizing state before such an invasion, the more there is at stake to defend. Consequently, we would expect a stronger response in support of democracy. A relevant indication comes from a meta-analysis by Bauer and colleagues (Reference Bauer, Blattman, Chytilová, Henrich, Miguel and Mitts2016, 268), who note that war violence affects individual political and social attitudes such as trust differently depending on whether the aggressor is internal or external and whether a population under attack “already possesses a national identity.” Though Bauer and colleagues do not examine whether this logic applies to diffuse support for democracy, their analysis suggests it is plausible through evolution of parochial altruism. On these considerations, we may expect to observe both direct and interaction effects of external attack and national identity salience on support for democracy.

A derivation of SIT known as the common in-group identity model (CIIM) (Gaertner and Dovidio Reference Gaertner and Dovidio2000) also indicates that when individuals feel their whole nation or country is under external attack they are more likely to strengthen their civic national identity (i.e., identify more strongly as Americans, French, or Ukrainians) and group cohesion at the national level, while reducing the salience of ethnic, religious, or other subnational group identities within the state (Rodriguez-Carballeira and Javaloy Reference Rodriguez-Carballeira and Javaloy2005) and boosting social capital and trust in institutions (Besley and Persson Reference Besley and Persson2010; Grosjean Reference Grosjean2014). By extension, this logic suggests that when geopolitical threats come from an authoritarian state, the salience of sociodemographic characteristics underlying these subgroup identities in a democratizing state would decline and they would have a less significant impact on support for democracy than civic national identity mobilization and war threat.

SIT also prompts us to recognize that because in-/out-group categorization at the interstate (geopolitical) level is likely to compound intergroup bias, then support for democracy could be mobilized not only by external attack, but also by perceived external (interstate) threats of different types. For example, an external actor may not only threaten to undermine or suppress democracy in the target state by direct force but may also (or instead) undermine the state’s prospects of membership in international democratic coalitions by waging or threatening limited wars or incursions, or by supporting insurgencies, in the hope of depleting the target state’s resources and demoralizing its population. If the external actor’s aggression is indirect and/or obfuscated, (which could well be the case in internationalized hybrid conflicts), the target state’s mobilization of geopolitical identity will reflect the extent to which the target state’s citizens blame the external actor for war. If such aggression is direct and overt, we would expect the war onset to mobilize out-group hostility directed at the external authoritarian aggressor and bolster in-group solidarity in support of democracy in a society. Shortly after Russia’s full-scale invasion in February 2022, one Kyiv resident vividly summarized this logic of geosocietal mobilization: “Don’t you see what kind of monster is attacking us? Of course, we fight for democracy!”Footnote 6

Representing our theoretical propositions in the order laid out above, we formulate the following hypotheses on geosocietal support for democracy applicable to Ukraine (i.e., within a democratic or democratizing state under military attack):

H1: Civic national pride will increase support for democracy.

H2: Threat of war from an authoritarian state will increase support for democracy.

H3: Civic national pride and threat of war will have robust effects on support for democracy across subnational sociodemographic cleavages (such as gender, language, and region).

H4: In a multi-actor (hybrid) war, civic national pride will increase support for democracy more among individuals who also attribute the war to an external authoritarian aggressor.

H5: A direct military attack by an authoritarian state will increase support for democracy compared to support before the attack.

The Context: Ukraine’s Identity Mobilization

In Ukraine, significant capacity for geosocietal mobilization developed over the two decades leading to Russia’s full-scale invasion in 2022. Regarding the societal component, the UNASIS annual nationwide surveys (N = 1,800/year) show a sustained 20–30% cumulative increase from 2002 through 2020 both of pride in Ukrainian citizenship and primary self-identification as citizens of Ukraine (rather than as natives of a province, city, town, or village, or as citizens of the former Soviet Union) (figure 1). The 50% threshold was crossed around the 2004 Orange Revolution,Footnote 7 where domestic issues (electoral manipulation) were dominant (Beissinger Reference Beissinger2013). The next surge followed successful anti-government protests in 2013–14 (Ukraine’s Revolution of Dignity, detailed further below) in which mobilization against corruption and repression was prominent.

Figure 1 Capacity for Civic National Mobilization in Ukraine Shows Long-Term Buildup

Notes: The 2014 survey was conducted in July, about a month before the Russian military significantly escalated the Donbas war. Donbas residents were polled afterwards only in Ukraine’s government-controlled areas (GCAs). Crimea data was excluded in all years.

Sources: See appendix section A12(v)a.

Subsequent surveys showed that citizenship became “a cornerstone of civic national identity” and the central part of post-Soviet nation-building in Ukraine (Barrington Reference Barrington2021). In 2016, 24% of Kyiv-controlled Donbas residents reported feeling more like Ukrainian citizens than before the 2014 Donbas war, compared to only 6% who reported feeling less so (Sasse and Lackner Reference Sasse and Lackner2018).

Regarding the geopolitical dimension of identity mobilization, the surveys demonstrate a sea change in threat perception and a concurrent geopolitical reorientation among Ukrainians after 2014. Following Russia’s militarized annexation of Crimea and intervention in the Donbas in 2014, the number of respondents who felt threatened by external aggression rose from about 10% to 60% (figure 2a). Geopolitical orientation between Russia and Europe flipped.

Figure 2 Geopolitical Identity Recategorization in Ukraine. (a) Fear of Attack by a Foreign Power. (b) Pro-Russia versus Pro-Europe Orientations

Sources: See appendix section A12(v)a.

In terms of SIT, that amounted to a superordinate (geopolitical) identity recategorization. Prior to 2014, for most Ukrainians, the Russia–Belarus Union was their geopolitical in-group (figure 2b). After 2014, it was the European Union. In-group preferences also became polarized. The results in figure 2b are based on two separate questions. Prior to 2014, about 20–30% of respondents answered “yes” to both, thus supporting Ukraine’s membership in both the EU and the Russia–Belarus Union (ignoring the practical impossibility of such an outcome). After 2014, that number dropped to 3–6%. And despite an uptick, opposition to both the EU and the Russia–Belarus Union (implying support for Ukraine’s geopolitical neutrality) stayed under 10% through 2021. Other studies showed a concurrent public reorientation toward NATO (Haran and Zolkina Reference Haran and Zolkina2017).

Societal and geopolitical dimensions of identity mobilization converged in the mass popular protests of November 2013–February 2014. Symptomatically, Ukrainians refer to those events as both the Revolution of Dignity and the Euromaidan Revolution, emphasizing, respectively, each dimension: rallying for civil liberties in response to riot police brutality and protesting the then president Viktor Yanukovych’s withdrawal from the EU Association Agreement under pressure from Vladmir Putin. Hundreds of thousands staked out Kyiv’s central Independence Square (Maidan) and defied police batons and sniper fire until Yanukovych was ousted. Ukrainian scholars analyzing the revolution captured this convergence of identity dimensions. Riabchuk and Lushnycky (Reference Riabchuk, Lushnycky, Stepanenko and Pylynskyi2015, 45) called it “an explosive moment of truth … a confrontation of two diametrically opposed worlds, two political systems and sets of values—the ‘Europe’ embodied by the EU and the ‘Eurasia’ embodied by Putinist Russia, Yanukovych’s ‘Family,’ and the hired thugs, ‘titushki,’ that harassed, tortured and killed the protesters.” Stepanenko and Pylynskyi (Reference Stepanenko, Pylynskyi, Stepanenko and Pylynskyi2015, 12) noted Ukrainian identity’s embeddedness in “the cultural and legal landscape that is currently called the European Union” and described Euromaidan as “another stage for Ukrainians’ striving for their freedoms and also for a European future.”

This capacity for geosocietal mobilization would drive Ukrainians’ spirited democratic resilience all the way through Russia’s 2022 invasion, upsetting the Kremlin’s plans to swiftly occupy and erase Ukraine from the world’s map.

Survey Tests and Findings

We test our hypotheses on geosocietal support for democracy with survey data in three settings: (1) within Ukraine’s Donbas region (2017); (2) in Ukraine outside the Donbas (2017 and 2018); and (3) in Ukraine before and after the full-scale Russian invasion (2021 and 2022).

The data comes from our original research project on war and democracy in Ukraine, initially conducted in conjunction with the annual opinion surveys of UNASIS between June 1 and June 19, 2017 (N = 1,800, plus a 399 oversample in the Donbas); between September 13 and October 1, 2018 (N = 1,800); and between November 1 and November 30, 2021 (N = 1,800) (Alexseev and Dembitskyi Reference Alexseev and Dembitskyi2024). These surveys were based on multistage probability cluster sampling of the adult (over the age of 18) population; were proportionate to the estimated population size by gender, age, education, settlement type, and province from 120 primary sampling units randomly selected from the national list of local legislative electoral districts; and were representative of the adult population of Ukraine and of each of Ukraine’s 25 territorial units. Polling took place in every province of Ukraine excluding the Crimea and non-government-controlled areas (NGCAs) of the Donbas. Respondents were selected through face-to-face contact and filled the survey questionnaires in writing.Footnote 8 The survey instrument and procedures followed the methodological and ethical standards of the European Social Survey (Golovakha and Gorbachik Reference Golovakha and Gorbachik2014). In June 2022, UNASIS tasked the Sociological Rating Group,Footnote 9 a reputable polling agency in Ukraine, to contact by phone all of its November 2021 annual survey respondents and to reinterview as many of them as possible. Given telephone survey constraints, the interviews were limited to about 30 questions of primary interest repeated verbatim from 2021. Interviews were completed with 475 verified repeat respondents (polled June 25–July 2, 2022)—generating a panel of the same individuals interviewed approximately three months before and four months after the onset of full-scale war.Footnote 10 That panel closely matched the sociodemographic characteristics of the UNASIS 2021 sample that was representative of the population in territories then under Ukraine’s control.Footnote 11

Principal Variables of Interest

For the dependent variable we use the UNASIS survey question closely related to diffuse support for democracy, which has been shown to be a significant predictor of democracy survival (Claassen Reference Claassen2020) and internally consistent across cultural settings (Carlin Reference Carlin2018). We asked: “How important do you find the following personally to you: the democratic development of our country.” The answers were on a five-point Likert scale.Footnote 12 We use two specifications of this measure: (1) the original scale to estimate the degree of democracy supportFootnote 13 and (2) a dummy variable with “important” and “very important” coded “1” and all other responses coded “0” to estimate the probability of explicit support, rather than differences on a scale.Footnote 14 With other surveys showing that most Ukrainians define democracy as freedom of expression (National Democratic Institute 2022; Szostek and Orlova Reference Szostek and Orlova2022), we use the importance of free speech on the same response scale in robustness checks.

For the civic national identity dimension of geosocietal mobilization, we use National Pride based on the question: “How proud or not proud are you to be a citizen of Ukraine?” Responses were on a five-point Likert scale. For the geopolitical threat dimension, we use Russia Aggressor, based on the question: “Against whom do you believe the Ukrainian forces (the army, the national guard, police, the state security service, and others) mostly fight in the Donbas? (Please select one option from the list.)”Footnote 15 Respondents who stated that Ukraine was primarily fighting against “local rebels financed, armed, and run by Russia,” or “mercenaries (local or foreign fighting for money) supported by Russia,” or “the regular Russian armed forces” were coded “1.” The rest (who picked “local insurgents who act independently,” or “local insurgents and foreign volunteers who act independently,” or “hard to say”) were coded “0.”Footnote 16 In the 2021–22 panel study, War Year serves as a proxy for geosocietal mobilization, given practically no variation on pride and threat after Russia’s full-scale attack.

Control Variables

We selected the following control variables to test for established alternative explanations and, in doing so, to minimize the possibility that some unobserved value may drive the results.

War Exposure

Based on the extensive literature reviewed earlier and following Dyrstad (Reference Dyrstad2013) and Dembitskyi (Reference Dembitskyi2016), we use two dummy variables: War Loss identifies respondents who said they lost a family member, a friend, health, or property due to war; and War Stress identifies respondents who said they experienced tension or anxiety or had disturbing war-related nightmares.Footnote 17 In the 2017 Donbas tests we add Warzone residency (see the next subsection).

Sociodemographic Characteristics

  1. 1. Language and Religion. Given the role of language as an ethnic identity marker (Arel Reference Arel2017; Laitin Reference Laitin1998) and its enduring, complex relationship with political attitudes in Ukraine (Barrington and Faranda Reference Barrington and Faranda2009; Shevel Reference Shevel2002; Szporluk Reference Szporluk1997), we pretested for three linguistic identity specifications following Onuch and Hale (Reference Onuch and Hale2018), found them strongly intercorrelated, and settled on reported language use at home based on Chapman and Gerber (Reference Chapman and Gerber2019) and Pop-Eleches and Robertson (Reference Pop-Eleches and Robertson2018).Footnote 18 For tests outside the Donbas we follow Chapman and Gerber (Reference Chapman and Gerber2019) by using Language Mostly Ukrainian (coding respondents using mostly Ukrainian “1” and mostly Russian or some Russian “0”). For the Donbas study, we use Language Some Ukrainian (coding mostly Ukrainian or some Ukrainian “1” and mostly Russian “0”).Footnote 19 In robustness checks on religious identity, we use respondents’ self-identification with the Russian Orthodox Church of the Moscow Patriarchate.

  2. 2. Modernization. Based on research linking democracy with economic development through the rise of an educated urban middle class (Lipset Reference Lipset1959; Rostow Reference Rostow1971), we use a dummy for Rural residency based on the Ukrainian government registry; Income based on respondents’ valuation of their income on a seven-point scale; and their Education level, measured on a five-point scale.

  3. 3. Socialization. Given the established significant effects of socialization differences across generations (Krosnick and Alwin Reference Krosnick and Alwin1989), including communist legacies (Pop-Eleches and Tucker Reference Pop-Eleches and Tucker2017) and by gender (Lizotte Reference Lizotte2016), we control for Age (six categories, robustness checks for three categories) and Female, based on self-identification.

  4. 4. Regionalism. We control for the well-established political significance of regional differences within Ukraine, which reflect complex interactions of ethnolinguistic, socioeconomic, cultural, and historical factors (Arel Reference Arel2002; Onuch and Hale Reference Onuch and Hale2018), by (1) holding the region constant in our 2017 Donbas tests and (2) including dummy variables for three of Ukraine’s four macroregions (West, Center, and East), following Barrington and Herron (Reference Barrington and Herron2004) in all other tests.Footnote 20

Media Effects

We run robustness checks for reliance on Russian versus Ukrainian news sources (see appendix table 1A.8).

Table 1 War Impact on Democracy Support, Nov. 2021–Jul. 2022: Linear Mixed Effects Model

Note: Estimate of fixed effects; ***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.

Whereas no statistical model can rule out all unobserved value effects, our single-country/region large-N design with theoretically grounded, context-specific control variables provides a strong guardrail.Footnote 21 Reverse causality is controlled with the inclusion of the pre/post-war-onset panel data tracking the same individuals. In cross-sectional tests, for reversed causality to drive the findings would imply that decreases of support for democracy in wartime would systematically depress both national pride and perceived external threat, contradicting the evidence.

The Donbas Regional Study

War Exposure

We tasked UNASIS to oversample from the Donbas GCAs in their 2017 annual national survey, resulting in 399 new respondents for a total regional sample of 565. That gives us enough power to identify statistically significant relationships of interest. The Donbas setting provides three design benefits. First, we control for region. This is important because regional differences capture complex interactions of ethnic, linguistic, socioeconomic, cultural, institutional, and other factors that remain subject to divergent interpretations and scholarly debate (Arel Reference Arel2002; Darden Reference Darden, Colton, Frye and Legvold2010; Frye Reference Frye2015; Kudelia and van Zyl Reference Kudelia and van Zyl2019; Kulyk Reference Kulyk2011; Kuzio Reference Kuzio2017; O’Loughlin Reference O’Loughlin2001; Sasse Reference Sasse2010). In the Donbas these effects have arguably accounted for significant regional specificity in Ukrainian politics, relations with Russia, governance, social identity, and warlordism (Driscoll Reference Driscoll2015; Kudelia Reference Kudelia2017; Osipian and Osipian Reference Osipian and Osipian2006; Reference Osipian and Osipian2012).

Second, we can assess the impacts of war exposure more rigorously. Our Warzone binary variable distinguishes between respondents who reported living in the “zone of combat operations” as “1” (N = 203) and others as “0” (N = 350).Footnote 22 On theoretical, methodological, and empirical grounds, physical proximity to war violence could be an important variable in its own right, as it may capture unanticipated or hard-to-operationalize effects aside from loss and stress (Bakke, O’Loughlin, and Ward 2009; Dyrstad Reference Dyrstad2013).

Using respondents’ self-categorization has significant advantages in the context of the 2014 Donbas war. To begin with, this measure identifies individuals to whom war exposure has been salient enough to report (thus controlling for otherwise hard-to-estimate individual-level salience and the effect duration of any discrete war event that count measures, typically based on media or observer reports, cannot control). It also captures views of respondents in areas that did experience war damage, but whose specific personal experiences may not have been significant enough to register in media or observer reports (notably, in our sample, the Sartana settlement).

At the same time, this measure captures respondents who experienced both principal types of war exposure in the Donbas in 2014–17: (1) in settlements seized by Russia’s proxy forces but retaken by Ukrainian forces in 2014, mostly after fierce battles (Melnyk et al. Reference Melnyk, Pavlyuk, Petrov and Glushko2019, 7–13), and (2) in settlements with sustained war fighting of variable intensity along a 250-mile “line of control” cutting through the Donbas from north to south. The line changed little from mid-2015 through survey time in mid-2017. Figure 3 illustrates how the distribution of reported war exposure in our survey broadly reflected the uneven patterns of fighting in the Donbas war.

Figure 3 Respondent’s Warzone Experience in the 2017 Donbas Survey

Note: N = 565.

We see larger proportions of respondents reporting war fighting in the region’s center where Russia-backed forces made the most advances in 2014; yet within that area we see more respondents reporting warzone experience in places more intensely contested (Lyman [75%], Kramatorsk [100%], Slov’yansk [70%], and Druzhkivka [60%]) than many settlements closer to the front lines—for example, Chasiv Yar (26%), Bakhmut (27%), and Pokrovs’ke (10%). In the south, we see respondents reporting war exposure in Mariupol (17%) and Pokrovs’ke (20%), where fighting took place in the spring of 2014 as local residents and Ukrainian forces overturned the Russia-backed takeovers, but was less intense than in Kramatorsk and Slov’yansk.

Coding respondents’ reports also captures indirect war experiences that event counts typically miss. According to our Donbas focus groups (in Druzhkivka and Mariupol, 2017), those experiences included witnessing troop movements; encountering and commiserating with war victims; fleeing the horrors of war and not knowing if one might return; stressing out when discovering unexploded bombs at children’s playgrounds; or knowing people whose hair turned gray overnight or who had been wounded, had disappeared, or had died (illustrative citations in the appendix, section A4).

Ordinary Donbas residents in our survey did not choose war experiences, war experiences chose them. Since most intense battles in 2014–17 were for cities, significantly more urban than rural residents in our sample reported living through war fighting (reflected in Pearson Rs for Warzone of 0.183** with urban residency; 0.107* with education; and 0.119* with income). In contrast, factors uncorrelated with the urban–rural divide—age, gender, and language use—were also uncorrelated with Warzone. Further validating our measure, we found that reporting war exposure significantly correlated with Loss (R = 0.338***) and Stress (R = 0.146**).Footnote 23 Yet, we also found Warzone residents to be 10–12% more likely to view democracy and free speech as important compared to other Donbas residents (both binary relationships being significant).

Statistical Analysis

To test our hypothesis in the context of the Donbas war, we estimated full effects of each independent variable while holding all other predictors constant using multiple linear regression with robust standard errors for Democracy Importance Scale and logistic regression for Democracy Importance Probability. Footnote 24

The results show strong support for H1 through H4. Figure 4a reports the estimated percentage-point change in Democracy Importance Scale from minimum to maximum value for each predictor (all rescaled from 0 to 1). National Pride and Russia Aggressor both have highly significant positive full effects (at p < 0.001, there is less than a 0.1% probability that these relationships were due to chance). Respondents who were very proud of their Ukrainian citizenship ranked the importance of democracy about 21% higher than respondents who were not proud at all. Respondents who saw Russia as the aggressor in the Donbas war ranked it 12% higher than others. Confidence intervals indicate, however, that Russia Aggressor effects are less prone to variation (about half of respondents who saw Russia as the aggressor were likely to rate the importance of democracy about the same or higher than about a quarter of the respondents who were very proud of their Ukrainian citizenship).

Figure 4 Donbas GCAs, 2017: Geosocietal Factors Consistently Predict Democracy Support

Figure 4b shows that the average probability of a respondent considering democracy to be important increases by nearly 24% when National Pride changes from “not proud” to “very proud,” and by over 21% when Russia Aggressor changes from “no” to “yes” with all other predictors held constant.Footnote 25 Once again, the Russia Aggressor factor is more reliable in the sense that the probability of support for democracy among 95% of respondents who considered Russia to be an aggressor is higher than among about one-fifth of respondents at the highest National Pride level. This is reflected in the higher significance level for Russia Aggressor (p < 0.001 compared to p = 0.005 for Pride).

H3 is supported in that (1) most social cleavage markers are not significant, and (2) for those that are, the significance levels are lower than for Pride and Russia Aggressor (cf. above, with p = 0.003 for education and p = 0.014 for age on the democracy support scale, and p = 0.012 for income and p = 0.019 for age). The sole exception was the effect of education on support probability (p = 0.002) being slightly more significant than Pride (p = 0.005).

We also considered that demographic variables may have indirect effects on democracy support by affecting related attitudes, and estimated them with a procedure known as seemingly unrelated regression (sureg in Stata). It shows that the proportion of Pride and Russia Aggressor total effects on the democracy support scale—mediated by education, income, age, rural residency, language, and female gender taken together—while statistically significant (p = 0.005 for Pride and p = 0.002 for Russia Aggressor), was substantively minor (13% and 13.2%, respectively). While supporting H3, these findings indicate that pride and threat do not displace well-established sociodemographic factors, but act alongside them, albeit more strongly and consistently.

To test H4 we estimated marginal effects of the interaction term between Pride and Russia Aggressor on Democracy Importance Scale using Stata. Figure 5 shows the effects of pride and the Russian threat on democracy support while controlling for all other variables, with the estimates from the same regression model as reported above. Based on the visual inference rule (Cumming and Finch Reference Cumming and Finch2005) for error bar overlaps, the interaction effect of Pride with Russia Aggressor was statistically significant except for those respondents who were not at all proud of their Ukrainian citizenship. That total interaction effect (the difference between the leftmost bottom dot and the rightmost top dot) is about 1.5 on a five-point scale—that is, 50% higher than the effect of Pride while holding Russia Aggressor constant (about a one-point total difference from left to right on either line), and three times as high as the effect of Russia Aggressor while holding Pride constant at any level (a 0.5-point vertical gap between the lines). This upholds H4.Footnote 26

Figure 5 Pride and Threat Interaction on the Democracy Importance Scale (Donbas GCAs, 2017)

With free speech importance as the dependent variable, we got near-identical results, with the main difference being that Pride related to the probability of free speech support at 2.4 percentage points below the conventional 95% significance level (at p = 0.074) (see appendix section A5).

Finally, we observe that while macrolevel geosocietal context measures (pride and threat) are statistically significant, none of the direct microlevel measures of war exposure (Loss, Stress, and Warzone) are. Warzone’s relationship to democracy importance (significant at the bivariate level, R = 0.144**) is no longer significant in the multiple regression. Sureg tests indicate this probably happens because about 41% of the Warzone effect is mediated by Russia Aggressor.

Out-of-Initial-Sample Tests: Ukraine 2017 and 2018

Our Donbas results held up across the rest of Ukraine in the same year and the year after with the same regression tests (the only two UNASIS annual surveys when all measures of interest to us were available).Footnote 27 As figure 6 shows, both National Pride and Russia Aggressor are significant predictors of democracy support in all four tests (at p < 0.001, except for Russia on Democracy Importance Scale in 2017 at p = 0.003).

Figure 6 Pride and Threat Boost Democracy Support across Ukraine outside Donbas (2017–18)

Note: STRESS is a measure of anxiety, tension, and nightmares; STRESS1 is a measure of anxiety and tension only. See appendix section A8 for more details.

Full effects are substantively meaningful. Respondents who were very proud to be Ukrainian citizens valued democracy 17% and 14% higher (for 2017 and 2018, respectively) and were 24% and 21% more likely to view democracy as important than respondents who were not at all proud. For respondents who saw Russia as the aggressor compared to those who did not, those effects were 10% and 5% and 18% and 9%, respectively. This upholds H1 and H2.

H3 is supported given that all other social subgroup markers are shown as either insignificant or less significant than Pride and Russia Aggressor or contingent on other sociodemographic characteristics (e.g., age having negative effects in a historically stronger Sovietized Donbas, but being insignificant or positively related to democracy support elsewhere). The proportion of total effects of Pride and Russia Aggressor on Democracy Importance Scale mediated by sociodemographic variables in the sureg test was insignificant in both years (see appendix section A6).

Replicating our Donbas test of marginal effects for the interaction term between Pride and Russia Aggressor, we find strong support for H4 in 2017 and partial support in 2018. While smaller than in the Donbas, the full interaction effect in 2017 is about twice as strong as the effect of Pride controlling for Russia and of Russia controlling for Pride (figure 7a). The results are mixed for 2018 (figure 7b). Russia Aggressor still gives a boost to Pride effects on the democracy support scale, yet for all levels of Pride except “mostly proud” it is not statistically significant at p < 0.05. However, that exception is substantively important, because the “mostly proud” group comprises 44% of all respondents. It means that for nearly half of Ukraine’s respondents the interaction of Pride and Russia was significant.

Figure 7 Pride and Threat Interaction on the Democracy Importance Scale in Ukraine outside Donbas

The decline of Russia Aggressor’s boost of Pride in 2018 reflects the reduction of Russia Aggressor’s direct effect on democracy support in 2018 compared to 2017 (figure 6). Macrolevel context offers a plausible explanation in that the battle death rates in the Donbas war dropped fivefold after mid-2016 and the front line stabilized, leading to its decreasing salience over time. Another indication is that in 2017 the combined effect size of Pride–Russia Aggressor within the Donbas, a region more affected by the war, was substantially larger than outside the Donbas.

Checking for free speech support, we again got near-identical results supporting H1H4, the exception being that Russian threat was not a significant predictor of free speech importance scale in 2018 and consequently not a significant booster of Pride’s effect, although it was a highly significant predictor that same year of probability of support for free speech (p = 0.006).

Finally, turning to the microlevel war exposure factors, War Loss has no significant association with democracy support and the effects of Stress are much weaker than the macrolevel (Pride and Russia Aggressor) factors. However, we see that outside the Donbas, Stress reduced democracy support in 2017–18. While these effects are small (between 2.7% and 5%), they remind us of the severe strains and grueling trials of war that Ukrainians are overcoming in pursuit of their democratic aspirations.Footnote 28

Russia’s Full-Scale Invasion Effects: Ukraine 2021–22 Pre/Post War-Onset Panel Study

The geosocietal identity mobilization we observed in 2017–18 paved the way for a massive, spirited surge of democracy support across Ukraine in response to Russia’s full-scale attack in February 2022. Our November 2021–June/July 2022 panel survey data enables a longitudinal pre-post (repeated measures) design with a continuous treatment condition (war), where prewar baseline data represents a de facto control group, and using the same subjects controls for confounders as the treatment continues.Footnote 29 In our case we have two repeated measures of the same subjects with an intervening “treatment” event (war) continuously affecting all subjects at the time of the repeated measurement. This design has significantly more leverage than (and should not be confused with) pretest-posttest one-group designs. Critically, the continuation of the treatment/intervening condition through the second measurement minimizes or removes common shortcomings of pretest-posttest designs, particularly intervening posttreatment effects and competing developments at treatment time. The scale and brutality of Russia’s war on Ukraine generated conditions overshadowing any other putative intervening event at the time of the repeat survey, and it also minimized the likelihood of instrument reactivity and Hawthorne effects (Spector Reference Spector1981, 28–30).

Russia’s full-scale invasion in 2022 also erased the ambiguity about enemy identification during the 2014–22 Donbas war, making the Russia Aggressor measure undiagnostic. In fact, our polling agency reported the question commonly elicited anger among respondents at the implied suggestion that an entity other than Russia could have attacked Ukraine. In this context, we use War Year (coded as 2022 = “1” and 2021 = “0”) as a proxy for geosocietal mobilization while controlling for microlevel war exposure effects with Loss and Stress. Footnote 30

We use a linear mixed-effects model in SPSS with respondents’ ID as subjects, War Year as a repeated measure, fixed effects for all variables, and random intercepts estimated for subjects with default covariance type (variance components). Model 1 features only War Year and model 2 adds the same controls as in the 2017–18 tests: time-invariant factors and covariates based on 2021 data (Ukrainian language use, region, age aggregated into three groups, gender, and education level) plus time-variant covariates (income level, loss, and stress).Footnote 31 Using the opportunity to analyze panel data, we estimate war effects on a broader range of attitudes related to democracy support than in 2017–18. First, we examine trust in key democratic institutions (media, parliament) and the leadership (presidency). Second, we assess support for Ukraine’s international (geopolitical) identity as a democracy with support for EU and NATO membership: while the former could be valued primarily as an economic union and the latter as a military alliance, democratic government is a core requirement for new members in both. Seeking membership in such organizations is a natural way for a democratizing state to “reinforce the commitment to democracy and provide an external anchor against retrogression to authoritarianism” (Huntington Reference Huntington1991, 87). We use only scale measures. Unlike binary measures they capture longitudinal shifts across response categories that otherwise would fall within either “0” or “1”—particularly from “rather important” to “very important” on Democracy Importance (55% shift) and Free Speech Importance (60% shift).

We find unequivocal support for H5. The coefficients (table 1) show War Year giving a statistically significant, strong, and robust boost to support for democracy: 12.5% on democracy and free speech importance, over 22% on trust in the media and parliament, 50% on trust in the president, and 16% and 20% on support for EU and NATO membership. The effects are nearly identical with or without control variables (cf., models 1 and 2) and all are highly significant (p < 0.001). Notably, respondents reporting personal war losses are significantly more, not less, likely to support democracy. It means rallying for democracy in Ukraine has been resilient in the face of extensive, devastating, and persistent hardship: close to 70% of respondents in June–July 2022 reported at least one form of war loss (losing or fleeing their homes, getting wounded, losing friends, or having family or friends displaced), a more than threefold increase from December 2021 among the same individuals. Stress has a statistically significant negative effect only on one democracy support indicator.

We see the war galvanizing democracy support both among previously less supportive groups (older respondents) and more supportive groups (the better educated, the economically better-off, residents of Ukraine’s west and center in 2021). We also do not see regional polarization (no significant negative estimates for Ukraine’s east). Robustness checks are not indicative of social desirability bias. Exclusive reliance on Ukrainian national news media—a proxy for less-than-sincere rallying (Hale Reference Hale2021)—had inconsistent and weaker effects on democracy support indicators than War Year. Footnote 32 Other empirical data (Onuch and Hale Reference Onuch and Hale2022, chap. 7) characterizes the surge in support for democracy as predominantly endogenous and not top-down, reflecting Ukraine as “a country of 44 million Zelenskys,” a civic nation with spirited resilience and surging determination to join global democratic coalitions.

Conclusions and Implications

Survey evidence from Ukraine, including the war-torn Donbas region and a panel survey with the same respondents shortly before and after Russia’s invasion in February 2022, indicates that support for democracy in the face of war is in significant respects geosocietal—arising from mobilization of civic national identity in a democratizing state and in response to a geopolitical threat from authoritarian aggressors. Not only do the effects of these factors obtain in defiance of massive personal losses and traumas and across sociodemographic cleavages, but we also see how civic pride boosts democracy support more when the salience of external threat rises. Similar results for supporting free speech indicate that this boost reflects a shift in values rather than an instrumental endorsement.Footnote 33 This is more than an interesting scholarly outcome. Russia’s daily mass bombardments, which have caused more destruction in Ukraine since February 2022 than in any other contemporaneous human conflict, show that Moscow remains set on crushing Ukraine’s democratic resilience. This is why those normatively committed to democracy need to keep supporting Ukraine in its war against Russian aggression.

Our findings contribute to three strands in the literature. First, our evidence shows that war type matters: war violence affects not only social capital (Besley and Persson Reference Besley and Persson2010; Grosjean Reference Grosjean2014), but also polity preferences. Second, we demonstrate that wartime mobilization of national identity could rally support not only for incumbent leaders and institutions, but also for political systems, including, in our case, democracy. The findings support a recent argument that wartime societal mobilization represents not only a quest for security, but also for national honor (Feinstein Reference Feinstein2022). This contributes to research showing that the “Zelensky effect” (Onuch and Hale Reference Onuch and Hale2022) was due less to the president of Ukraine’s special powers to induce followership, and more to his acute sense of long-evolving civic Ukrainian identity, exemplified by his asking the US president for “not a ride, but ammunition” to fight against Russia’s full-scale invasion.

Third, we contribute to the second-image-reversed theory (Gourevitch Reference Gourevitch1978; Rogowski Reference Rogowski1989; Zarakol Reference Zarakol2013) by showing that geosocietal mobilization may bolster democracy legitimation above and beyond well-established domestic factors, such as economic development (Kitschelt Reference Kitschelt1992; Przeworski Reference Przeworski1991), revenue mobilization (Skocpol et al. Reference Skocpol, Munson, Karch, Camp, Katznelson and Shefter2001; Tilly Reference Tilly1992), social structure (Moore Reference Moore1966), institutional performance (Chu et al. Reference Chu, Bratton, Lagos, Shastri and Tessler2008; Rose, Mishler, and Haerpfer Reference Rose, Mishler and Haerpfer1998), political culture (Almond and Verba Reference Almond and Verba1965; Inglehart Reference Inglehart1990), and communist legacies (Pop-Eleches and Tucker Reference Pop-Eleches and Tucker2017), and regardless of international-level factors, such as the global balance of power between democracies and nondemocracies (Boix Reference Boix2011; Gunitsky Reference Gunitsky2017) and the West’s democratization efforts (Huntington Reference Huntington1991; Samuels Reference Samuels2023).

In terms of geosocietal mobilization’s plausibility beyond present-day Ukraine, one can think of Soviet military interventions in Hungary (1956), Czechoslovakia (1968), and Poland (1980s) boosting popular support for liberalization; of the greater success of US democratization efforts during the Cold War in regions where the Soviet Union posed a stronger geopolitical challenge (Europe and East Asia) compared to Latin America, and more so to Africa, where the Soviet Union positioned itself as a geopolitical ally against Western neocolonialism (Huntington Reference Huntington1991); and of how China’s growing military assertiveness may bolster support for democracy in Taiwan, Korea, and Japan.

Another way to probe external validity is to ask if geosocietal logic may work in reverse, undermining democracy. First, autocrats may frame democracy as a geopolitical threat. The Economist (2023) has attributed much of the autocratization trend that has “wiped out” 35 years of global democracy advances (Papada et al. Reference Papada, Altman, Angiolillo, Gastaldi, Köhler, Lundstedt and Natsika2023) to “paranoid nationalism”—to autocrats legitimating their rule by “vowing to defend people against confected threats” from abroad. Second, within consolidated democracies including the US, a diminution of the geopolitical threat posed by autocracies could contribute to domestic political polarization and authoritarian populism. Third, support for democracy may decline in states whose prospects of joining international democratic coalitions diminish, with Turkey and the EU a plausible illustration.

One specific suggestion for future research is to develop geopolitical threat and civic national identity indicators for the V-Dem dataset, with a focus on individual-level data. Related to that, extending longitudinal analysis is important, since our evidence is limited in time and more needs to be learned about how long geosocietal mobilization effects may last and how they may change as the war continues. Finally, future research can formulate and examine broader, policy-relevant questions on how to balance domestic reforms with external support, including military and security assistance, as well as inclusion in international democratic alliances to win hearts and minds locally for enduring democratic resilience.

Supplementary Materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/S1537592724000422.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge funding provided by the Hostler Institute, the Bruce E. Porteus Fund, and the College of Arts and Letters of San Diego State University; the National Academy of Sciences of Ukraine; and the Program on New Approaches to Research and Security in Eurasia (PONARS Eurasia) at George Washington University. Our special thanks go to Evhen Golovakha for assistance in designing the study and interpreting and contextualizing the findings. We are also grateful to the anonymous reviewers for constructive critiques and to Henry Hale, Stephen Hanson, Serhii Makeiev, Kyle Marquardt, and Maxim Parashchevin for valuable comments and encouragement.

Footnotes

*

Data replication sets are available in Harvard Dataverse at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/GO0VG6

1 The Donbas war refers to Ukraine’s armed conflict with Russian and Russian-backed forces in Ukraine’s Donbas region that began on April 12, 2014 and continued until February 24, 2022, when it was subsumed by Russia’s full-scale invasion of Ukraine.

2 The Crimea exclusion effect after 2014 was insignificant; other surveys corroborated these trends. See appendix sections A12(v) and (vi).

3 Pemstein, Meserve, and Melton (Reference Pemstein, Meserve and Melton2010), for example, identify 10 different scales.

4 Indicatively, Bauer and colleagues’ (Reference Bauer, Blattman, Chytilová, Henrich, Miguel and Mitts2016) meta-analysis of the impacts of war on sociopolitical attitudes captured only two studies out of 34 on exposure to interstate conflict, one of them a survey (on the effects of World War II after 65 years).

5 Pop-Eleches and Robertson (Reference Pop-Eleches and Robertson2018, 111) essentially define “a rallying-around-the-flag effect among Ukrainian citizens” in 2013–15 as “a substantial awakening of Ukrainian identities … of a civic rather than ethnic nature,” not as support for individual leadership.

6 Personal correspondence with an anonymized Kyiv resident, December 6, 2022.

7 Months of protests after the poisoning of Viktor Yushchenko, a candidate in the 2004 presidential election (whose backers wore orange), and mass electoral fraud by his rival, Viktor Yanukovych, which led to a fair revote and Yushchenko’s win.

8 Response rates varied from 25% to 57% and the sampling error margin was about +/− 3.5% for 50/50 response distribution. For more details on sampling, data collection, and ethnical practices, see appendix sections A10A11.

9 ANONYMIZED03 in the appendix.

10 All polls complied with the US Institutional Review Board requirements for research with human subjects.

11 For more details, see appendix section A3.

12 For the distributional properties and wording of all survey questions, see appendix section A1.

13 Although support for democracy is high, distributional properties and our use of statistical procedures guard against significant distortion of results due to skewness (see appendix section A1[v]).

14 Coding neutral responses comprising about a third of the samples as “0” was substantively justified: when recoded into a separate dummy variable, they turned out to be significantly more common at 95% confidence interval among respondents in sociodemographic groups that viewed democracy as unimportant; this coding therefore partially controls for preference falsification, given that openly expressing a lack of support for democracy may be viewed as politically incorrect (see appendix section A9).

15 This question captured variation on the perceived origins and the course of the 2014 Donbas war, given that multiple actors were engaged, and Russia denied and obfuscated its involvement (Fischer Reference Fischer2019).

16 Cross-paneling with 23 other variables showed that coding “hard to say” as “0” was substantively justified.

17 For 2018 we used only the first item; the second was unavailable (see appendix section A12[i] for details).

18 Robustness tests with all three specifications had closely similar results (see appendix table 1.A8).

19 This way we avoided almost 100% of left- or right-skewed variables either in the Donbas or in Ukraine’s west. We also did not include self-identification by nationality due to extreme skewness (over 90% picking Ukrainian) and based on input from supervisors of focus groups we conducted in May 2017 in Lviv, Odesa, Kyiv, and Donetsk Oblast (Druzhkivka and Mariupol) indicating it was substantively uninformative.

20 See appendix section A1.1 for scale specifications and A12(iii) for the list of provinces by region.

21 Generally, inflating the number of attitudinal control variables is not recommended as it complicates the interpretation of coefficients (Pop-Eleches and Robertson Reference Pop-Eleches and Robertson2018, 111).

22 We coded as “missing” six respondents who refused to answer the question and six who said they were combatants. Diagnostic tests showed no difference when this variable had the six combatants coded as “1.”

23 Admittedly, self-reporting could have generated errors. However, since our surveys were explicitly depersonalized and questions were answered in writing without the interviewer present, respondents who reported having lived in the war zone had no plausible incentives to report or not report their experiences for extraneous reasons, such as putative economic gains or costs, publicity, social affirmation, etc.

24 For regression equations, the full effects definition, and regression tables, see appendix sections A2(i), A2(ii), A2(iv), and A5.

25 The full effect in logit is the average marginal effect of predictors scaled from 0 to 1.

26 For command syntax and estimation results for sureg and margins in Stata, see appendix sections A6A7.

27 We excluded Warzone since only 2% reported it outside the Donbas in 2017 and 1.9% in 2018.

28 In robustness checks with multiple specifications of independent and control variables, 98.3% of 249 new effect sizes of Pride and Russia had the same significance levels and none were insignificant (see appendix section A8).

29 See, e.g., clinical studies (Cao et al. Reference Cao, Jin, Brietzke, McIntyre, Wang, Rosenblat and Ragguett2019) and psychology (Jasinskaja‐Lahti, Mähönen, and Ketokivi Reference Jasinskaja‐Lahti, Mähönen and Ketokivi2012).

30 See appendix sections A8 and A12(iv).

31 Rural was not available for 2022. Education and income are partial proxies.

32 See appendix table 1.A8.

33 Other surveys corroborate this interpretation, notably a marked improvement in acceptance of the LGBTQ community in May 2022 compared to 2016 (Kravchuk, Zinchenkov, and Lyashchenko Reference Kravchuk, Zinchenkov and Lyashchenko2023, 13–15).

References

Alexseev, Mikhail A., and Dembitskyi, Serhii. 2024. “Replication Data for: Geosocietal Support for Democracy: Survey Evidence from Ukraine.” Harvard Dataverse. DOI: 10.7910/DVN/GO0VG6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexseev, Mikhail A., and Hale, Henry E.. 2019. “Crimea Come What May: Do Economic Sanctions Backfire Politically?Journal of Peace Research 57 (2): 344–59. DOI: 10.1177/0022343319866879.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Almond, Gabriel A., and Verba, Sidney. 1965. The Civic Culture: Political Attitudes and Democracy in Five Nations. Newbury Park: Sage.Google Scholar
Arel, Dominique. 2002. “Interpreting ‘Nationality’ and ‘Language’ in the 2001 Ukrainian Census.” Post-Soviet Affairs 18 (3): 213–49. DOI: 10.2747/1060-586X.18.3.213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arel, Dominique. 2017. “Language, Status, and State Loyalty in Ukraine.” Harvard Ukrainian Studies 35 (1–4): 233–63. https://www.husj.harvard.edu/uploads/files/HUS%2035_2017-2018_Arel.pdf.Google Scholar
Baker, William D., and Oneal, John R.. 2001. “Patriotism or Opinion Leadership? The Nature and Origins of the ‘Rally ’Round the Flag’ Effect.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 45 (5): 661–87. DOI: 10.1177/0022002701045005006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakke, Kristin, M., John, O’Loughlin, and Michael, D. Ward. 2009. “Reconciliation in Conflict-Affected Societies: Multilevel Modeling of Individual and Contextual Factors in the North Caucasus of Russia.” Annals of the Association of American Geographers 99 (5): 1012–21. DOI: 10.1080/00045600903260622.Google Scholar
Barrington, Lowell. 2021. “Citizenship as a Cornerstone of Civic National Identity in Ukraine.” Post-Soviet Affairs 37 (2): 155–73. DOI: 10.1080/1060586X.2020.1851541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrington, Lowell W., and Herron, Erik S.. 2004. “One Ukraine or Many? Regionalism in Ukraine and Its Political Consequences.” Nationalities Papers 32 (1): 5386. DOI: 10.1080/0090599042000186179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrington, Lowell, and Faranda, Regina. 2009. “Reexamining Region, Ethnicity, and Language in Ukraine.” Post-Soviet Affairs 25 (3): 232–56. DOI: 10.2747/1060-586X.24.3.232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Michal, Blattman, Christopher, Chytilová, Julie, Henrich, Joseph, Miguel, Edward, and Mitts, Tamar. 2016. “Can War Foster Cooperation?Journal of Economic Perspectives 30 (3): 249–74. DOI: 10.1257/jep.30.3.249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baum, Matthew A., and Groeling, Tim. 2008. “Shot by the Messenger: Partisan Cues and Public Opinion Regarding National Security and War.” Political Behavior 31 (2): 157–86. DOI: 10.1007/s11109-008-9074-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beissinger, Mark R. 2013. “The Semblance of Democratic Revolution: Coalitions in Ukraine’s Orange Revolution.” American Political Science Review 107 (3): 574–92. DOI: 10.1017/s0003055413000294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berinsky, Adam J. 2009. In Time of War: Understanding American Public Opinion from World War II to Iraq. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI: 10.7208/chicago/9780226043463.001.0001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Besley, Timothy, and Persson, Torsten. 2010. “State Capacity, Conflict, and Development.” Econometrica 78 (1): 134. DOI: 10.3982/ECTA8073.Google Scholar
Boix, Carles. 2011. “Democracy, Development, and the International System.” American Political Science Review 105 (4): 809–28. DOI: 10.1017/S0003055411000402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brody, Richard A., and Shapiro, Catherine R.. 1991. “The Rally Phenomenon in Public Opinion.” In Assessing the President: The Media, Elite Opinion and Public Support, ed. Brody, Richard A., 526–46. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. DOI: 10.1515/9780804779876-005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canetti-Nisim, Daphna, Halperin, Eran, Sharvit, Keren, and Hobfoll, Stevan E.. 2009. “A New Stress-Based Model of Political Extremism: Personal Exposure to Terrorism, Psychological Distress, and Exclusionist Political Attitudes.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 53 (3): 363–89. DOI: 10.1177/0022002709333296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cao, Bing, Jin, Min, Brietzke, Elisa, McIntyre, Roger S., Wang, Dongfang, Rosenblat, Joshua D., Ragguett, Renee‐Marie, et al. 2019. “Serum Metabolic Profiling Using Small Molecular Water‐Soluble Metabolites in Individuals with Schizophrenia: A Longitudinal Study Using a Pre–Post‐Treatment Design.” Psychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences 73 (3): 100–8. DOI: 10.1111/pcn.12779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlin, Ryan E. 2018. “Sorting out Support for Democracy: A Q-Method Study.” Political Psychology 39 (2): 399422. DOI: 10.1111/pops.12409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carmil, Devora, and Breznitz, Shlomo. 1991. “Personal Trauma and World View—Are Extremely Stressful Experiences Related to Political Attitudes, Religious Beliefs, and Future Orientation?Journal of Traumatic Stress 4 (3): 393405. DOI: 10.1002/jts.2490040307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, Hannah S., and Gerber, Theodore P.. 2019. “Opinion-Formation and Issue-Framing Effects of Russian News in Kyrgyzstan.” International Studies Quarterly 63 (3): 756–69. DOI: 10.1093/isq/sqz046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chemtob, Claude M., Novaco, Raymond W., Hamada, Roger S., and Gross, Douglas M.. 1997. “Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment for Severe Anger in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder.” Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 65 (1): 184–89. DOI: 10.1037/0022-006X.65.1.184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chu, Yun-han, Bratton, Michael, Lagos, Marta, Shastri, Sandeep, and Tessler, Mark. 2008. “Public Opinion and Democratic Legitimacy.” Journal of Democracy 19 (2): 7487. DOI: 10.1353/jod.2008.0032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claassen, Christopher. 2020. “Does Public Support Help Democracy Survive?American Journal of Political Science 64 (1): 118–34. DOI: 10.1111/ajps.12452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collier, Paul. 2007. The Bottom Billion: Why the Poorest Countries Are Failing and What Can Be Done about It. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Collier, Paul, Elliot, V. L., Hegre, Håvard, Hoeffler, Anke, Reynal-Querol, Marta, and Sambanis, Nicholas. 2003. Breaking the Conflict Trap: Civil War and Development Policy. Washington, DC: World Bank. DOI: 10.1596/978-0-8213-5481-0.Google Scholar
Cumming, Geoff, and Finch, Sue. 2005. “Inference by Eye: Confidence Intervals and How to Read Pictures of Data.” The American Psychologist 60 (2): 170–80. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.2.170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darden, Keith A. 2010. “Conditional Property and Regional Political Cultures: Challenges for U.S. Foreign Policy in the Post-Soviet Space.” In The Policy World Meets Academia: Designing U.S. Policy toward Russia, eds. Colton, Timothy, Frye, Timothy, and Legvold, Robert, 7078. Cambridge, MA: American Academy of Arts & Sciences.Google Scholar
Darkovich, Andrii, Savisko, Myroslava, and Rabinovych, Maryna. 2023. “Explaining Ukraine’s Resilience to Russia’s Invasion: The Role of Local Governance and Decentralization Reform.” Policy Memo No. 855, September. Washington, DC: PONARS Eurasia. https://www.ponarseurasia.org/wp-content/uploads/2023/09/Pepm855_Darkovich_Savisko_Rabinovych_September2023-UPDATED.pdf. Accessed October 20, 2023.Google Scholar
Davis, Darren W., and Silver, Brian D.. 2004. “Civil Liberties vs. Security: Public Opinion in the Context of the Terrorist Attacks on America.” American Journal of Political Science 48 (1): 2846. DOI: 10.1111/j.0092-5853.2004.00054.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dembitskyi, Serhii. 2016. “Sotsial’nye Faktory Psikhologicheskogo Distressa: Na Primere Respondentov Prozhivavshikh v Zone Boevykh Deystviy” [Social factors of psychological distress: The case of respondents who lived in the combat zone]. Soc-research.info blog, September 26. http://soc-research.info/blog/index_files/scl-tbd.html.Google Scholar
Diamond, Larry. 1999. Developing Democracy: Toward Consolidation. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. DOI: 10.56021/9780801860140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinesen, Peter Thisted, and Jæger, Mads Meier. 2013. “The Effect of Terror on Institutional Trust: New Evidence from the 3/11 Madrid Terrorist Attack.” Political Psychology 34 (6): 917–26. DOI: 10.1111/pops.12025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Driscoll, Jesse. 2015. Warlords and Coalition Politics in Post-Soviet States. Cambridge Studies in Comparative Politics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI: 10.1017/cbo9781107478046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dyrstad, Karin. 2013. “Does Civil War Breed Authoritarian Values? An Empirical Study of Bosnia-Herzegovina, Kosovo and Croatia.” Democratization 20 (7): 1219–42. DOI: 10.1080/13510347.2012.688032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Easton, David. 1975. “A Re-Assessment of the Concept of Political Support.” British Journal of Political Science 5 (4): 435–57. DOI: 10.1017/s0007123400008309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feinstein, Yuval. 2018. “One Flag, Two Rallies: Mechanisms of Public Opinion in Israel During the 2014 Gaza War.” Social Science Research 69: 6582. DOI: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2017.09.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feinstein, Yuval. 2022. Rally ’Round the Flag: The Search for National Honor and Respect in Times of Crisis. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Sabine. 2019. “The Donbas Conflict: Opposing Interests and Narratives, Difficult Peace Process.” SWP Research Paper 2019/RP 05, April 17. Berlin: Stiftung Wissenschaft und Politik, German Institute for International and Security Affairs. DOI: 10.18449/2019RP05. Accessed July 15, 2021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frye, Timothy. 2015. “What Do Voters in Ukraine Want? A Survey Experiment on Candidate Ethnicity, Language, and Policy Orientation.” Problems of Post-Communism 62 (5): 247–57. DOI: 10.1080/10758216.2015.1026200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frye, Timothy. 2019. “Economic Sanctions and Public Opinion: Survey Experiments from Russia.” Comparative Political Studies 52 (7): 967–94. DOI: 10.1177/0010414018806530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaertner, Samuel L., and Dovidio, John F.. 2000. Reducing Intergroup Bias: The Common Ingroup Identity Model. New York: Psychology Press. DOI: 10.4324/9781315804576.Google Scholar
Gaines, Brian J. 2002. “Where’s the Rally? Approval and Trust of the President, Cabinet, Congress, and Government since September 11.” PS: Political Science & Politics 35 (3): 531–36. DOI: 10.1017/S1049096502000793.Google Scholar
Godefroidt, Amélie. 2023. “How Terrorism Does (and Does Not) Affect Citizens’ Political Attitudes: A Meta‐Analysis.” American Journal of Political Science 67 (1): 2238. DOI: 10.1111/ajps.12692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golovakha, Evhen, and Gorbachik, Andriy. 2014. Tendentsii Sotsial’nykh Zmin v Ukraini Ta Evropi za Rezul’tatami Evropeys’kogo Sotsial’nogo Doslidzhennya 2005–2007–2009–2011–2013 [Trends of social changes in Ukraine and Europe—the results of the European Social Survey 2005–2007–2009–2011–2013]. Kyiv: National Academy of Sciences of Ukraine, Institute of Sociology.Google Scholar
Gourevitch, Peter. 1978. “The Second Image Reversed: The International Sources of Domestic Politics.” International Organization 32 (4): 881912. DOI: 10.1017/S002081830003201X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfeld, Liah. 1993. Nationalism: Five Roads to Modernity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grosjean, Pauline. 2014. “Conflict and Social and Political Preferences: Evidence from World War II and Civil Conflict in 35 European Countries.” Comparative Economic Studies 56 (3): 424–51. DOI: 10.1057/ces.2014.2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunitsky, Seva. 2017. Aftershocks: Great Powers and Domestic Reforms in the Twentieth Century. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. DOI: 10.23943/princeton/9780691172330.001.0001.Google Scholar
Hale, Henry E. 2021. “Authoritarian Rallying as Reputational Cascade? Evidence from Putin’s Popularity Surge after Crimea.” American Political Science Review 116 (2): 580–94. DOI: 10.1017/S0003055421001052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haran, Olexiy, and Zolkina, Mariia. 2017. “The Demise of Ukraine’s ‘Eurasian Vector’ and the Rise of Pro-NATO Sentiment.” Policy Memo No. 458, February. Washington, DC: PONARS Eurasia. https://www.ponarseurasia.org/wp-content/uploads/attachments/Pepm458_Haran_Feb2017_4.pdf. Accessed July 10, 2021.Google Scholar
Hetherington, Marc J., and Suhay, Elizabeth. 2011. “Authoritarianism, Threat, and Americans’ Support for the War on Terror.” American Journal of Political Science 55 (3): 546–60. DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2011.00514.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hetherington, Marc J., and Nelson, Michael. 2003. “Anatomy of a Rally Effect: George W. Bush and the War on Terrorism.” PS: Political Science & Politics 36 (1): 3742. DOI: 10.1017/S1049096503001665.Google Scholar
Horowitz, Donald L. 2001. Ethnic Groups in Conflict. Berkeley: University of California Press. DOI: 10.1525/9780520926318.Google Scholar
Huntington, Samuel P. 1991. The Third Wave: Democratization in the Late Twentieth Century. Norman: University of Oklakhoma Press.Google Scholar
Inglehart, Ronald. 1990. Culture Shift in Advanced Industrial Society. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. DOI: 10.2307/j.ctv346rbz.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janoff-Bulman, Ronnie. 1992. Shattered Assumptions: Towards a New Psychology of Trauma. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Jasinskaja‐Lahti, Inga, Mähönen, Tuuli Anna, and Ketokivi, Mikko. 2012. “The Dynamics of Ethnic Discrimination, Identities and Outgroup Attitudes: A Pre–Post Longitudinal Study of Ethnic Migrants.” European Journal of Social Psychology 42 (7): 904–14. DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.1916.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitschelt, Herbert. 1992. “The Formation of Party Systems in East Central Europe.” Politics and Society 20 (1): 750. DOI: 10.1177/0032329292020001003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kravchuk, Andrii, Zinchenkov, Oleksandr, and Lyashchenko, Oleh. 2023. “The Battle for Freedom: LGBTQ Situation in Ukraine in 2022.” LGBTQ Human Rights Report, February 12. Kyiv: Nash Svit Center. https://gay.org.ua/publications/Situation-of-LGBT-in-Ukraine-2022-ENG.pdf.Google Scholar
Krosnick, Jon A., and Alwin, Duane F.. 1989. “Aging and Susceptibility to Attitude Change.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57 (3): 416–25. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudelia, Serhiy. 2017. “The Donbas Rift.” Russian Social Science Review 58 (2–3): 212–34. DOI: 10.1080/10611428.2017.1316062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kudelia, Serhiy, and van Zyl, Johanna. 2019. “In My Name: The Impact of Regional Identity on Civilian Attitudes in the Armed Conflict in Donbas.” Nationalities Papers 47 (S5): 801–21. DOI: 10.1017/nps.2019.68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kulyk, Volodymyr. 2011. “Language Identity, Linguistic Diversity and Political Cleavages: Evidence from Ukraine.” Nations and Nationalism 17 (3): 627–48. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8129.2011.00493.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kulyk, Volodymyr. 2016. “National Identity in Ukraine: Impact of Euromaidan and the War.” Europe–Asia Studies 68 (4): 588608. DOI: 10.1080/09668136.2016.1174980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuzio, Taras. 2017. Putin’s War against Ukraine: Revolution, Nationalism, and Crime: Toronto, ON: University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Lai, Brian, and Reiter, Dan. 2005. “Rally ’Round the Union Jack? Public Opinion and the Use of Force in the United Kingdom, 1948–2001.” International Studies Quarterly 49 (2): 255–72. DOI: 10.1111/j.0020-8833.2005.00344.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laitin, David D. 1998. Identity in Formation: The Russian-Speaking Populations in the Near Abroad. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Lambert, Alan J., Scherer, Laura D., Schott, John Paul, Olson, Kristina R., Andrews, Rick K., O’Brien, Thomas C., and Zisser, Alison R.. 2010. “Rally Effects, Threat, and Attitude Change: An Integrative Approach to Understanding the Role of Emotion.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 98 (6): 886903. DOI: 10.1037/a0019086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipset, Seymour Martin. 1959. “Some Social Requisites of Democracy: Economic Development and Political Legitimacy.” American Political Science Review 53 (1): 69105. DOI: 10.2307/1951731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lizotte, Mary-Kate. 2016. “Gender Differences in Support for Torture.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 61 (4): 772–87. DOI: 10.1177/0022002715595698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, Monty G., and Cole, Benjamin R.. 2014. “Global Report 2014: Conflict, Governance, and State Fragility.” Vienna, VA: Center for Systemic Peace. https://www.systemicpeace.org/vlibrary/GlobalReport2014.pdf.Google Scholar
Melnyk, Nataliia, Pavlyuk, Alina, Petrov, Maksim, and Glushko, Olena. 2019. Armed Conflict in the East of Ukraine: The Damage Caused to the Housing of the Civilian Population. Kharkiv: LLB Human Rights Publisher. https://www.undp.org/sites/g/files/zskgke326/files/migration/ua/02-3_d-sos_Armed-conflict-in-eastern-Ukraine_eng_light.pdf.Google Scholar
Merolla, Jennifer L., and Zechmeister, Elizabeth J.. 2009. Democracy at Risk: How Terrorist Threats Affect the Public. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI: 10.7208/chicago/9780226520568.001.0001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, Barrington. 1966. Social Origins of Dictatorship and Democracy: Lord and Peasant in the Making of the Modern World. Boston: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Mueller, John E. 1970. “Presidential Popularity from Truman to Johnson.” American Political Science Review 64 (1): 1834. DOI: 10.2307/1955610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nair, Gautam, and Sambanis, Nicholas. 2019. “Violence Exposure and Ethnic Identification: Evidence from Kashmir.” International Organization 73 (2): 329–63. DOI: 10.1017/S0020818318000498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Democratic Institute. 2022. “Opportunities and Challenges Facing Ukraine’s Democratic Transition.” Report, September 19. Washington, DC: National Democratic Institute. https://kiis.com.ua/materials/pr/20220920_o/August%202022_wartime%20survey%20Public%20fin.pdf. Accessed January 9, 2023.Google Scholar
Nussio, Enzo. 2020. “Attitudinal and Emotional Consequences of Islamist Terrorism: Evidence from the Berlin Attack.” Political Psychology 41 (6): 1151–71. DOI: 10.1111/pops.12679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
OHCHR (Office of the High Commissioner for Human Rights). 2022. “Conflict-Related Civilian Casualties in Ukraine.” Kyiv: Office of the High Commissioner for Human Rights Monitoring Mission in Ukraine. https://ukraine.un.org/sites/default/files/2022-02/Conflict-related%20civilian%20casualties%20as%20of%2031%20December%202021%20%28rev%2027%20January%202022%29%20corr%20EN_0.pdf. Accessed January 27, 2022.Google Scholar
O’Loughlin, John. 2001. “The Regional Factor in Contemporary Ukrainian Politics: Scale, Place, Space, or Bogus Effect?Post-Soviet Geography and Economics 42 (1): 133. DOI: 10.1080/10889388.2001.10641161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oneal, John R., Lian, Brad, and Joyner, James H. Jr. 1996. “Are the American People ‘Pretty Prudent’? Public Responses to U.S. Uses of Force, 1950–1988.” International Studies Quarterly 40 (2): 261–79. DOI: 10.2307/2600959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Onuch, Olga. 2022. “Why Ukrainians Are Rallying around Democracy.” Journal of Democracy 33 (4): 3746. DOI: 10.1353/jod.2022.0045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Onuch, Olga, and Hale, Henry E.. 2018. “Capturing Ethnicity: The Case of Ukraine.” Post-Soviet Affairs 34 (2–3): 84106. DOI: 10.1080/1060586X.2018.1452247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Onuch, Olga, and Hale, Henry E.. 2022. The Zelensky Effect. London: Hurst Publishers. DOI: 10.1093/oso/9780197684511.001.0001.Google Scholar
Osipian, Ararat L., and Osipian, Alexandr L.. 2006. “Why Donbass Votes for Yanukovych: Confronting the Ukrainian Orange Revolution.” Demokratizatsiya 14 (4): 495517. DOI: 10.3200/demo.14.4.495-517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osipian, Ararat L., and Osipian, Alexandr L.. 2012. “Regional Diversity and Divided Memories in Ukraine: Contested Past as Electoral Resource, 2004–2010.” East European Politics and Societies 26 (3): 616–42. DOI: 10.1177/0888325412447642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papada, Evie, Altman, David, Angiolillo, Fabio, Gastaldi, Lisa, Köhler, Tamara, Lundstedt, Martin, Natsika, Natalia, et al. 2023. “Defiance in the Face of Autocratization” Democracy Report 2023, March. Gothenburg: Varieties of Democracy (V-Dem) Institute. Accessed November 1, 2023. https://www.v-dem.net/documents/29/V-dem_democracyreport2023_lowres.pdf.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pemstein, Daniel, Meserve, Stephen A., and Melton, James. 2010. “Democratic Compromise: A Latent Variable Analysis of Ten Measures of Regime Type.” Political Analysis 18 (4): 426–49. DOI: 10.1093/pan/mpq020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Permanent Mission of Ukraine to the UN. 2021. “Russian Aggression.” New York: Permanent Mission of Ukraine to the UN. https://ukraineun.org/en/ukraine-and-un/russian-aggression. Accessed January 25, 2022.Google Scholar
Pop-Eleches, Grigore, and Robertson, Graeme B.. 2018. “Identity and Political Preferences in Ukraine—Before and after the Euromaidan.” Post-Soviet Affairs 34 (2–3): 107–18. DOI: 10.1080/1060586X.2018.1452181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pop-Eleches, Grigore, and Tucker, Joshua A.. 2017. Communism’s Shadow: Historical Legacies and Contemporary Political Attitudes. Princeton Studies in Political Behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. DOI: 10.23943/princeton/9780691175591.001.0001.Google Scholar
Przeworski, Adam. 1991. Democracy and the Market: Political and Economic Reforms in Eastern Europe and Latin America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI: 10.1017/CBO9781139172493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rasler, Karen, and Thompson, William R.. 2004. “The Democratic Peace and a Sequential, Reciprocal, Causal Arrow Hypothesis.” Comparative Political Studies 37 (8): 879908. DOI: 10.1177/0010414004267980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reuveny, Rafael, and Li, Quan. 2003. “The Joint Democracy–Dyadic Conflict Nexus: A Simultaneous Equations Model.” International Studies Quarterly 47 (3): 325–46. DOI: 10.1111/1468-2478.4703002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riabchuk, Mykola, and Lushnycky, Andrej N.. 2015. “Ukraine’s Third Attempt.” In Ukraine after the Euromaidan: Challenges and Hopes, eds. Stepanenko, Viktor and Pylynskyi, Yaroslav, 4758. Interdisciplinary Studies on Central and Eastern Europe, vol. 13. Bern: Peter Lang. DOI: 10.3726/978-3-0351-0798-2/13.Google Scholar
Rodriguez-Carballeira, Alvaro, and Javaloy, Federico. 2005. “Psychosocial Analysis of the Collective Processes in the United States after September 11.” Conflict Management and Peace Science 22 (3): 201–16. DOI: 10.1080/07388940500200716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogowski, Ronald. 1989. Commerce and Coalitions: How Trade Affects Domestic Political Alignments. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. DOI: 10.1515/9780691219431.Google Scholar
Rose, Richard, Mishler, William, and Haerpfer, Christian. 1998. Democracy and Its Alternatives: Understanding Post-Communist Societies. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. DOI: 10.56021/9780801860379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenblatt, Abram, Greenberg, Jeff, Solomon, Sheldon, Pyszczynski, Tom, and Lyon, Deborah. 1989. “Evidence for Terror Management Theory: I. The Effects of Mortality Salience on Reactions to Those Who Violate or Uphold Cultural Values.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57 (4): 681. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rostow, Walt W. 1971. Politics and the Stages of Growth. New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI: 10.1017/cbo9780511562778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samuels, David J. 2023. “The International Context of Democratic Backsliding: Rethinking the Role of Third Wave ‘Prodemocracy’ Global Actors.” Perspectives on Politics 21 (3): 1001–12. DOI: 10.1017/S1537592722003334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasse, Gwendolyn. 2010. “Ukraine: The Role of Regionalism.” Journal of Democracy 21 (3): 99106. DOI: 10.1353/jod.0.0177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasse, Gwendolyn, and Lackner, Alice. 2018. “War and Identity: The Case of the Donbas in Ukraine.” Post-Soviet Affairs 34 (2–3): 139–57. DOI: 10.1080/1060586X.2018.1452209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shevel, Oxana. 2002. “Nationality in Ukraine: Some Rules of Engagement.” East European Politics and Societies 16 (2): 386413. DOI: 10.1177/088832540201600203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skocpol, Theda. 2002. “Will 9/11 and the War on Terror Revitalize American Civic Democracy?PS: Political Science & Politics 35 (3): 537–40. DOI: 10.1017/S104909650200080X.Google Scholar
Skocpol, Theda, Munson, Ziad, Karch, Andrew, and Camp, Bayliss. 2001. “Six Patriotic Partnerships: Why Great Wars Nourished American Civic Voluntarism.” In Shaped by War and Trade: International Influences on American Political Development, eds. Katznelson, Ira and Shefter, Martin, 134–80. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. DOI: 10.2307/j.ctv301f0h.10.Google Scholar
Sniderman, Paul M., Petersen, Michael Bang, Slothuus, Rune, Stubager, Rune, and Petrov, Philip. 2019. “Reactions to Terror Attacks: A Heuristic Model.” Political Psychology 40 (S1): 245–58. DOI: 10.1111/pops.12575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spector, Paul E. 1981. Research Designs. Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences. New York: Sage. DOI: 10.4135/9781412985673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stepanenko, Viktor, and Pylynskyi, Yaroslav. 2015. “Ukraine’s Revolution: The National Historical Context and the New Challenges for the Country and the World.” In Ukraine after the Euromaidan: Challenges and Hopes, eds. Stepanenko, Viktor and Pylynskyi, Yaroslav, 1125. Interdisciplinary Studies on Central and Eastern Europe, vol. 13. Bern: Peter Lang. DOI: 10.3726/978-3-0351-0798-2/11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szostek, Joanna, and Orlova, Dariya. 2022. “Understandings of Democracy and ‘Good Citizenship’ in Ukraine: Utopia for the People, Participation in Politics Not Required.” Post-Soviet Affairs 38 (6): 479–96. DOI: 10.1080/1060586X.2022.2084280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szporluk, Roman. 1997. “Ukraine: From an Imperial Periphery to a Sovereign State.” Daedalus 126 (3): 85119.Google Scholar
Tajfel, Henri, and Turner, John C.. 1986. “The Social Identity Theory of Inter-Group Behavior.” In Psychology of Intergroup Relations, eds. Worchel, Steven and Austin, William G., 724. Chicago: Nelson-Hall.Google Scholar
Tedeschi, Richard G., and Calhoun, Lawrence G.. 2004. “The Posttraumatic Growth: Conceptual Foundations and Empirical Evidence.” Psychological Inquiry 15 (1): 118. DOI: 10.1207/s15327965pli1501_01.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The Economist . 2022. “Volodymyr Zelensky’s Ukraine Is Defined by Self-Organisation.” The Economist, April 16. https://www.economist.com/briefing/2022/04/16/volodymyr-zelenskys-ukraine-is-defined-by-self-organisation.Google Scholar
The Economist . 2023. “How Cynical Leaders Are Whipping up Nationalism to Win and Abuse Power.” The Economist, August 31. https://www.economist.com/briefing/2023/08/31/how-cynical-leaders-are-whipping-up-nationalism-to-win-and-abuse-power.Google Scholar
Tilly, Charles. 1992. Coercion, Capital and European States, AD 990–1990, rev. edition. Cambridge: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tir, Jaroslav. 2012. “Territorial Diversion: Diversionary Theory of War and Territorial Conflict.” Journal of Politics 72 (2): 413–25. DOI: 10.1017/s0022381609990879.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tir, Jaroslav, and Singh, Shane P.. 2015. “Get Off My Lawn: Territorial Civil Wars and Subsequent Social Intolerance in the Public.” Journal of Peace Research 52 (4): 478–91. DOI: 10.1177/0022343315571008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tocqueville, Alexis de. (1835) 1969. Democracy in America, trans. Lawrence, G.. 2 vols. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books.Google Scholar
Transparency International. 2020. “Corruption Perceptions Index: Ukraine.” Berlin: Transparency International. https://www.transparency.org/en/cpi/2020/table/ukr. Accessed July 1, 2021.Google Scholar
Tudor, Maya, and Slater, Dan. 2021. “Nationalism, Authoritarianism, and Democracy: Historical Lessons from South and Southeast Asia.” Perspectives on Politics 19 (3): 706–22. DOI: 10.1017/S153759272000078X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Hauwaert, Steven M., and Huber, Robert A.. 2020. “In-Group Solidarity or Out-Group Hostility in Response to Terrorism in France? Evidence from a Regression Discontinuity Design.” European Journal of Political Research 59 (4): 936–53. DOI: 10.1111/1475-6765.12380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walter, Barbara F. 2015. “Why Bad Governance Leads to Repeat Civil War.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 59 (7): 1242–72. DOI: 10.1177/0022002714528006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber, Max. (1919) 1965. Politics as a Vocation, trans. Gerth, H. H. and Wright Mills, C.. Philadelphia: Fortress Press.Google Scholar
Woods, Joshua. 2011. “The 9/11 Effect: Toward a Social Science of the Terrorist Threat.” The Social Science Journal 48 (1): 213–33. DOI: 10.1016/j.soscij.2010.06.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zarakol, Ayşe. 2013. “Revisiting Second Image Reversed: Lessons from Turkey and Thailand.” International Studies Quarterly 57 (1): 150–62. DOI: 10.1111/isqu.12038.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Capacity for Civic National Mobilization in Ukraine Shows Long-Term BuildupNotes: The 2014 survey was conducted in July, about a month before the Russian military significantly escalated the Donbas war. Donbas residents were polled afterwards only in Ukraine’s government-controlled areas (GCAs). Crimea data was excluded in all years.Sources: See appendix section A12(v)a.

Figure 1

Figure 2 Geopolitical Identity Recategorization in Ukraine. (a) Fear of Attack by a Foreign Power. (b) Pro-Russia versus Pro-Europe OrientationsSources: See appendix section A12(v)a.

Figure 2

Table 1 War Impact on Democracy Support, Nov. 2021–Jul. 2022: Linear Mixed Effects Model

Figure 3

Figure 3 Respondent’s Warzone Experience in the 2017 Donbas SurveyNote: N = 565.

Figure 4

Figure 4 Donbas GCAs, 2017: Geosocietal Factors Consistently Predict Democracy Support

Figure 5

Figure 5 Pride and Threat Interaction on the Democracy Importance Scale (Donbas GCAs, 2017)

Figure 6

Figure 6 Pride and Threat Boost Democracy Support across Ukraine outside Donbas (2017–18)Note: STRESS is a measure of anxiety, tension, and nightmares; STRESS1 is a measure of anxiety and tension only. See appendix section A8 for more details.

Figure 7

Figure 7 Pride and Threat Interaction on the Democracy Importance Scale in Ukraine outside Donbas

Supplementary material: File

Alexseev and Dembitskyi supplementary material

Alexseev and Dembitskyi supplementary material
Download Alexseev and Dembitskyi supplementary material(File)
File 16.5 MB
Supplementary material: Link

Alexseev and Dembitskyi Dataset

Link