Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
  • Cited by 17
Publisher:
Cambridge University Press
Online publication date:
November 2015
Print publication year:
2015
Online ISBN:
9781316403631

Book description

From the barbed, childish taunt on the school playground, to the eloquent sophistry of a lawyer prising open a legal loophole in a court of law, meaning arises each time we use language to communicate with one another. How we use language - to convey ideas, make requests, ask a favour, and express anger, love or dismay - is of the utmost importance; indeed, linguistic meaning can be a matter of life and death. In The Crucible of Language, Vyvyan Evans explains what we know, and what we do, when we communicate using language; he shows how linguistic meaning arises, where it comes from, and the way language enables us to convey the meanings that can move us to tears, bore us to death, or make us dizzy with delight. Meaning is, he argues, one of the final frontiers in the mapping of the human mind.

Reviews

'Evans has reclaimed language from the dry dissection of grammatical structure and returned it to the public as a topic to think deeply about.'

Alun Anderson Source: New Scientist

Refine List

Actions for selected content:

Select all | Deselect all
  • View selected items
  • Export citations
  • Download PDF (zip)
  • Save to Kindle
  • Save to Dropbox
  • Save to Google Drive

Save Search

You can save your searches here and later view and run them again in "My saved searches".

Please provide a title, maximum of 40 characters.
×

Contents

References
Abraham, A., Schubotz, R.I. and Cramon, D.Y.von. (2008). Thinking about the future versus the past in personal and non-personal contexts. Brain Research, 1233: 106–19.
Aitchison, J. (1996). Words in the Mind: An Introduction to the Mental Lexicon. Oxford: Blackwell.
Antón, S.C. (2003). Natural history of Homo erectus. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 122: 126–70.
Alverson, H. (1994). Semantics and Experience. Baltimore, MA: Johns Hopkins Press.
Arbib, M. (2012). How the Brain Got Language: The Mirror System Hypothesis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Arsuaga, J.L., Martínez, L., Lorenzo, C., Gracia, A., Muñoz, A., Alonso, O. and Gallego, J.. (1999). The human cranial remains from Gran Dolina Lower Pleisticene site (Sierra de Atapuerca, Spain). Journal of Human Evolution, 37/3–4: 431–57.
Arterberry, M.E. and Bornstein, M.H.. (2001). Three-month-old infants’ categorization of animals and vehicles based on static and dynamic attributes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80: 333–46.
Atkins, B.T.S. (1987). Semantic-ID tags: Corpus evidence for dictionary senses. In The Uses of Large Text Databases: Proceedings of the Third Annual Conference of the New OED Centre, pp. 17–36. University of Waterloo, Canada.
Bak, T.H., O'Donovan, D.G., Xuereb, J.H., Boniface, S. and Hodges, J.R.. (2001). Selective impairment of verb processing associated with pathological changes in Brodmann areas 44 and 45 in the motor neuron disease-dementia-aphasia syndrome. Brain, 124: 103–20.
Bar-Yosef, O. and Belfer-Cohen, A.. (2001). From Africa to Eurasia – early dispersals. Quaternary International, 75: 19–28.
Barsalou, L.W. (1992). Cognitive Psychology: An Overview for Cognitive Scientists. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Barsalou, L.W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22/4: 577–609.
Barsalou, L.W. (2005). Continuity of the conceptual system across species. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9: 309–11.
Barsalou, L.W., Santo, A., Simmons, W.K. and Wilson, C.D.. (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. In Vega, M.De, Glenberg, A.M. and Graesser, A.C. (Eds.), Symbols, Embodiment, and Meaning, pp. 245–83. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bayle, P., Macchiarelli, R., Trinkaus, E., Duarte, C., Mazurier, A. and Zilhão, J.. (2010). Dental maturational sequence and dental tissue proportions in the early Upper Paleolithic child from Abrigodo Lagar Velho, Portugal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 107: 1338–42.
Behme, C. (2014). A Galilean science of language. Journal of Linguistics, 50/3: 671–704.
Berthenthal, B. (1993). Infants’ perception of biomechanical motions: Intrinsic image and knowledge-based constraints. In Granrud, C. (Ed.), Visual Perception and Cognition in Infancy, pp. 175–214. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Berwick, R.C., Friederici, A.D., Chomsky, N. and Bolhuis, J.J.. (2013). Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17: 89–98.
Bickerton, D. (1990). Language and Species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Bickerton, D. (2002). From proto-language to language. In Crow, T. (Ed.)., The Speciation of Modern Homo Sapiens, pp. 103–20. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94: 115–47.
Bickerton, D. and Calvin, W.H.. (2000). Lingua ex Machina: Reconciling Darwin and Chomsky with the Human Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Boas, F. (1911). The Mind of Primitive Man. London: Macmillan.
Boden, M.A. (1990). The Creative Mind: Myths and Mechanisms. London: Weidenfield and Nicholson.
Boden, M.A. (1994a). Précis of The Creative Mind: Myths and Mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17/3: 519–70.
Boden, M.A. (1994b). Creativity: A framework for research. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 17/3: 558–570.
Boden, M.A. (1995). Modelling creativity: Reply to reviewers. Journal ofArtificial Intelligence, 79: 161–82.
Boden, M.A. (1996). What is creativity? In Boden, M.A. (Ed.), Dimensions of Creativity, pp. 75–118. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Boesch, C. and Boesch-Achermann, H.. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H.. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Tai National Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 78: 547–73.
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H.. (1990). Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatology, 54: 86–99.
Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75/1: 1–28.
Boulenger, V., Mechtou, L., Thobois, S., Broussolle, E., Jeannerod, M. and Nazir, T.A.. (2008). Word processing in Parkinson's disease is impaired for action verbs but not for concrete nouns. Neuropsychologia, 46: 743–56.
Brown, P., Sutikna, T., Morwood, M.J., Soejono, R.P., Jatmiko, E., Saptomo, W. and Rokus, A.D.. (2004). A new small-bodied hominin from the Late Pleistocene of Flores, Indonesia. Nature, 431: 1055–61.
Bruner, J. (1976). From communication to language – a psychological perspective. Cognition, 3: 255–87.
Buccino, G., Riggio, T.L., Melli, G., Binkofski, F., Gallese, V. and Rizzolatti, G.. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: A combined TMS and behavioral study. Cognitive Brain Research, 24: 355–63.
Büchel, C., Price, C. and Friston, K.. (1998). A multimodal language region in the ventral visual pathway. Nature, 394: 274–7.
Buhusi, C.V. and Meck, W.H.. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. National Review of Neuroscience, 6/10: 755–65.
Bybee, J. (2010). Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cai, Z.G. and Connell, L.. (2012). Space-time interdependence and sensory modalities: Time affects space in the hand but not in the eye. In Miyake, N., Peebles, D. and Cooper, R.P. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 168–73. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Cai, Z. G. and Connell, L.. (2015). Space-time interdependence: Evidence against asymmetric mapping between time and space. Cognition, 136: 268–81.
Call, J. and Tomasello, M.. (2007). The Gestural Communications of Apes and Monkeys. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Call, J. and Tomasello, M.. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12/5: 187–92.
Campbell, S.S. (1986). Estimation of empty time. Human Neurobiology, 5, 205–7.
Cartmill, M. and Smith, F.H.. (2009). The Human Lineage. London: John Wiley.
Carretero, J.M., Rodríguez, L., García-González, R., Arsuaga, J.L., Gómez-Olivencia, A., Lorenzo, C., Bonmatí, A., Gracia, A. and Martínez, I.. (2012). Stature estimation from complete long bones in the Middle Pleistocene humans from the Sima de los Huesos, Sierra de Atapuerca (Spain). Journal of Human Evolution, 62/2: 242–55.
Casasanto, D. (2010). Space for thinking. In Evans, V. and Chilton, P. (Eds.), Language, Cognition, and Space: State of the Art and New Directions, pp. 453–78. London: Equinox Publishing.
Casasanto, D. and Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106: 579–93.
Chafe, W. (1994). Discourse, Consciousness, and Time: The Flow and Displacement of Conscious Experience in Speaking and Writing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Cheney, D.L. and Seyfarth, R.M.. (1990). How Monkeys See the World: Inside the Mind of Another Species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cienki, A. (1998). STRAIGHT: An image schema and its metaphorical extensions. Cognitive Linguistics, 9/2: 107–50.
Cisek, P. (1999). Beyond the computer metaphor: Behaviour as interaction. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6/11–12: 125–42.
Clark, H. (1996). Using Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Clark, J., de Heinzelin, J., Schick, K., Hart, W., White, T., WoldeGabriel, G., Walter, R., Suwa, G., Asfaw, B. and Vrba, E.. (1994). African Homo erectus: Old radiometric ages and young Oldowan assemblages in the middle Awash Valley, Ethiopia. Science, 264/5167: 1907–9.
Corballis, M. (2002). From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Corballis, M. (2008). Not the last word. Book review of The First Word: The Search for the Origins of Language by Christine Kenneally in American Scientist, Jan-Feb, Scientist's Nightstand, available online at www.americanscientist.org/bookshelf/pub/not-the-last-word
Coulson, S. (2000). Semantic Leaps: Frame-Shifting and Conceptual Blending in Meaning Construction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Coveney, P. and Highfield, R.. (1991). The Arrow of Time: The Quest to Solve Science's Greatest Mystery. London: Flamingo.
Croft, W. (2001). Radical Construction Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Crystal, D. (1998). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dąbrowska, E. (2009). Words as constructions. In Evans, V. and Pourcel, S. (Eds.), New Directions in Cognitive Linguistics, pp. 201–24. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Dahan, D. and Tanenhaus, M.K.. (2004). Continuous mapping from sound to meaning in spoken-language comprehension: Evidence from immediate effects of verb-based constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30: 498–513.
Damasio, A. (1995). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. London: Vintage.
Darwin, C. (1859). On the Origin of Species (). London: John Murray.
Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex (). London: John Murray.
Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dawkins, R. (2010). The Greatest Show on Earth: The Evidence for Evolution. London: Black Swan.
Deacon, T. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. New York: W.W. Norton & Co.
Deecke, V. (2012). Tool-use in the brown bear (Ursus arctos). Animal Cognition, 15/4: 725–30.
Dennett, D. (1989). The Intentional Stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
d'Errico, F., Backwell, L., Villa, P. and colleagues. (2012). Early evidence of San material culture represented by organic artifacts from Border Cave, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 10.1073/pnas.1204213109.
d'Errico, F. and Soressi, M.. (2002). Systematic use of manganese pigment by the Pech-de-l'Azé Neandertals: Implications for the origin of behavioural modernity. Journal of Human Evolution, 42: A13.
d'Errico, F. and Vanhaeren, M.. (2009). Earliest personal ornaments and their significance for the origin of language debate. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (Eds.), The Cradle of Language, pp. 16–40. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
DiMaggio, E.N., Campisano, C.J., Rowan, J., Dupont-Nivet, G., Deino, A.L., Bibi, F., Lewis, M.E., Souron, A., Garello, D., Werdelin, L., Reed, K.E. and Arrowsmith, J. Ramón. (2015). Late Pliocene fossiliferous sedimentary record and the environmental context of early Homo from Afar, Ethiopia. Science, 347/6228: 1355–9.
DuBrul, E. (1977). Origins of the speech apparatus and its reconstruction in fossils. Brain and Language, 4: 365–81.
Dediu, D. and Levinson, S.C.. (2013). On the antiquity of language: The reinterpretation of Neandertal linguistic capacities and its consequences. Frontiers in Psychology, 4: 397.
Dunbar, R.I.M. (1996). Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language. London: Faber and Faber.
Evans, V. (2004). The Structure of Time: Language, Meaning and Temporal Cognition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Evans, V. (2009). How Words Mean: Lexical Concepts, Cognitive Models and Meaning Construction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Evans, V. (2010). Figurative language understanding in LCCM Theory. Cognitive Linguistics 21/4: 601–62.
Evans, V. (2013). Language and Time: A Cognitive Linguistics Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Evans, V. (2014). The Language Myth: Why Language is Not an Instinct. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Evans, V. (2015). What's in a concept? Analogue versus parametric concepts in LCCM Theory. In Margolis, E. and Laurence, S. (Eds.), The Conceptual Mind: New Directions in the Study of Concepts, pp. 251–90. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Evans, V. and Green, M.. (2006). Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Everett, D. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã. Current Anthropology, 46/4: 621–646.
Everett, D. (2009). Don't Sleep, There are Snakes: Life and Language in the Amazonian Jungle. London: Profile Books.
Everett, D. (2012). Language: The Cultural Tool. London: Profile Books.
Falk, D. (1975). Comparative anatomy of the larynx in man and the chimpanzee: Implications for language in Neanderthal. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 43: 123–32.
Fauconnier, G. (1994). Mental Spaces. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fauconnier, G. (1997). Mappings in Thought and Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fauconnier, G. and Turner, M.. (2000). Compression and global insight. Cognitive Linguistics. 11/3-4: 283–304.
Fauconnier, G. and Turner, M.. (2002). The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind's Hidden Complexities. New York: Basic Books.
Fauconnier, G. and Turner, M.. (2008). The origin of language as a product of the evolution of double-scope blending. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31/5: 520–1.
Fitch, W.T. (2009a). Musical Protolanguage: Darwin's Theory of Language Evolution Revisited. Available online at http://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=1136.
Fitch, W.T. (2009b). Fossil cues to the evolution of speech. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (Eds.), The Cradle of Language, pp. 112–34. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Flaherty, M. (1999). A Watched Pot: How We Experience Time. New York: New York University Press.
Floss, M. (2003). Did they meet or not. Observations on Châtelperronian and Aurignacian settlement patterns in eastern France. In Zilhão, J. and d'Errico, F. (Eds.), The Chronology of the Aurignacian and of the Transitional Technocomplexes, pp. 273–88. Lisbon, Portugal: Instituto Português de Arqueologia.
Fodor, J. (1975). The Language of Thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. (1981). The current status of the innateness controversy. In Representations, pp. 257–316. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. (1983). Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. (1995). The Elm and Expert: Mentalese and its Semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Fodor, J. (1998). Concepts: Where Cognitive Science Went Wrong. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Fodor, J. (2008). LOT2: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Fraisse, P. (1984). Perception and estimation of time. Annual Review of Psychology, 35: 1–36.
Freeman, N.H., Lloyd, S. and Sinha, C.. (1980). Infant search tasks reveal early concepts of containment and canonical usage of objects. Cognition, 8: 243–62.
Frye, D., Rawling, P., Moore, C. and Myers, I.. (1983). Object–person discrimination at 3 and 10 months. Developmental Psychology, 19: 303–9.
Galton, A. (2011). Time flies but space doesn't: Limits to the spatialization of time. Journal of Pragmatics, 43: 695–703.
Gamble, C. and Marshall, G.. (2001). The shape of handaxes, the structure of the Acheulian world. In Milliken, S. and Cook, J.D. (Eds.), A Very Remote Period Indeed. Papers on the Palaeolithic presented to Derek Roe, pp. 19–27. Oxford: Oxbow.
Gavins, J. and Steen, G.. (2003). Cognitive Poetics in Practice. London: Routledge.
Gazzaniger, M. (2006 ).Leon Festinger: Lunch with Leon. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1: 88–94.
Geeraerts, D. (2009). Theories of Lexical Semantics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gentner, D., Imai, M. and Boroditsky, L.. (2002). As time goes by: Evidence for two systems in processing space time metaphors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17/5: 537–65.
Gibbs, R.W. (2008). The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Gibson, J. (1986). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Verb Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Goldberg, A. (2006). Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalizations in Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gomes, C.M. and Boesch, C.. (2009). Wild chimpanzees exchange meat for sex on a long-term basis. ONE, 4/4: e5116.
Goodwin, C. (2003). Conversation and Brain Damage. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Glenberg, A.M. and Kaschak, M. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9/3: 558–65.
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing Gesture: How Our Hands Help Us Think. Harvard: Harvard University Press.
Grady, J. (1997a). Foundations of Meaning: Primary Metaphors and Primary Scenes. Doctoral thesis, Linguistics Dept, University of California, Berkeley (available from UMI Dissertation Services: www.il.proquest.com/umi/dissertations/).
Grady, J. (1997b). THEORIES ARE BUILDINGS revisited. Cognitive Linguistics, 8/4: 267–90.
Grady, J. (1999). A typology of motivation for conceptual metaphor: Correlation vs. resemblance. In Gibbs, R.W. and Steen, G. (Eds.), Metaphor in Cognitive Linguistics, pp. 79–100. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Grady, J. and Johnson, C.. (2000). Converging evidence for the notions of ‘subscene’ and ‘primary scene’, Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, pp. 123–36. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.
Gräfenhaim, M., Behne, T., Carpenter, M. and Tomasello, M.. (2009). Young children's understanding of joint commitments. Developmental Psychology, 45/5: 1430–43.
Green, R.E., Krause, J., Briggs, A.W., Maricic, T., Stenzel, U. and Kircher, M.. (2010). A draft sequence of the Neandertal genome. Science, 328: 710–722.
Green, D.J. and Alemseged, Z.. (2012). Australopithecus afarensis scapular ontogeny, function, and the role of climbing in human evolution. Science, 338/6106: 514–517.
Gregory, R. (1998). Eye and Brain (). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gries, S. Th. (2006). Corpus-based methods and cognitive semantics: The many meanings of to run. In Gries, S. Th. and Stefanowitsch, A. (Eds.), Corpora in Cognitive Linguistics: Corpus-based Approaches to Syntax and Lexis, pp. 57–99. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Haley, D. (Ed.) (1986). Sea otter. Marine Mammals of Eastern North Pacific and Arctic Waters (). Seattle, WA: Pacific Search Press.
Hampe, B. (2005). From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42: 335–46.
Hart, B.J., Hart, L.A., McCory, M. and Sarath, C.R.. (2001). Cognitive behaviour in Asian elephants: Use and modification of branches for fly switching. Animal Behaviour 62/5: 839–47.
Haviland, J.B. (1974). A last look at Cook's Guugu-Yimidhirr wordlist. Oceania, 44/3: 216–32.
Hawks, J. (2013). Neandertal Ancestry and Founder Effects. John Hawks Weblog. Available at: http://johnhawks.net/weblog/topics/introgression.
Hebb, D.O. (2002). The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory (new edition). Hove: Psychology Press.
Heine, B., Claudi, U. and Hünnemeyer, F.. (1991). Grammaticalization: A Conceptual Framework. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Heine, B. and Kuteva, T.. (2007). The Genesis of Grammar: A Reconstruction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hespos, S.J. and Baillargeon, R.. (2001). Reasoning about containment events in very young infants. Cognition, 78: 207–45.
Hofstadter, D. (1979). Gödel, Escher, Bach: An Eternal Golden Braid. New York: Basic Books.
Houghton, P. (1993). Neandertal supralaryngeal vocal tract. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 90: 139–46.
Hublin, J.-J. (2009). Out of Africa: Modern human origins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 106: 16022–7.
Hulme, M. (2008). The conquering of climate: Discourses of fear and their dissolution. The Geographical Journal, 174/1: 5–16.
Hurford, J. (2007). The Origins of Meaning. Language in the Light of Evolution I. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hurford, J. (2012). The Origins of Grammar. Language in the Light of Evolution II. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Isenberg, N., Silbersweig, D., Engelien, A., Emmerich, S., Malavade, K., Beattie, B., Leon, A.C., and Stern, E.. (1999). Linguistic threat activates the human amygdala. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 96: 10456–9.
Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the Computational Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Jackendoff, R. (1992). Languages of the Mind: Essays on Mental Representation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Jackendoff, R. (2010). Your theory of language evolution depends on your theory of language. In Larson, R., Déprez, V. and Yamakido, H. (Eds.), The Evolution of Human Language: Biolinguistic Perspectives, pp. 63–72. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Jakobson, R. (1942). Six Lectures on Sound and Meaning. Boston, MA: MIT Press.
Jelinek, E. (1995). Quantification in Straits Salish. In Bach, E., Jelinek, E., Kratzer, A. and Partee, B. (Eds.), Quantification in Natural Languages, pp. 487–540. New York: Kluwer Academic.
Johanson, D.C. and Edey, M.A.. (1981). Lucy: The Beginnings of Humankind. London: Penguin.
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14: 201–11.
Johnson, C. (1999). Metaphor vs. conflation in the acquisition of polysemy: The case ofsee. In Hiraga, M., Sinha, C. and Wilcox, S. (Eds.), Cultural, Typological and Psychological Perspectives in Cognitive Linguistics, pp. 155–69. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Johnson, M. (1987). The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination and Reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Joormens, J., d' Errico, F., Wesselingh, F.P. and colleagues. (2014). Homo erectus at Trinil on Java used shells for tool production and engraving. Nature, 10.1038/nature13962.
Julesz, B. (1981). Textons: The elements of texture perception and their interactions. Nature, 290: 91–7.
Julian, R. (2005). A John Dee Chronology, 1509–1609. Renaissance Man: The Reconstructed Libraries of European Scholars: 1450–1700 Series One: The Books and Manuscripts of John Dee, 1527–1608. Marlborough, Wiltshire: Adam Matthew Publications.
Kellman, P.J. and Spelke, E.S. (1983). Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 15: 483–524.
Kendon, A. (1991). Some considerations for a theory of language origins. Man, 26/2: 199–221.
Kingdon, J. (2004). Lowly Origin: Where, When, and Why Our Ancestors First Stood Up. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Klatzky, R.L., Pellegrino, J.W., McCloskey, B.P and Doherty, S.. (1989). Can you squeeze a tomato? The role of motor representations in semantic sensibility judgements. Journal of Memory and Language, 28: 56–77.
Klein, R.G. (2009). The Human Career: Human Biological and Cultural Origins (). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kojima, S. (1990). Comparison of auditory functions in the chimpanzee and human. Folia Primatology, 55: 62–72.
Koestler, A. (1964). The Act of Creation. London: Hutchinson.
Koops, K., McGrew, W.C. and Matsuzawa, T.. (2010). Do chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) use cleavers and anvils to fracture Treculia africana fruits? Preliminary data on a new form of percussive technology. Primates, 51/2: 175–8.
Kövescses, Z. (2010). Metaphor: A Practical Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kranjec, A., Lehet, M. and Chatterjee, A.. (2013). Space and time are mutually contagious in sound. In Knauff, M., Pauen, M., Sebanz, N. and Wachsmuth, I. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Thirty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 829–33, http://cognitivesciencesociety.org/conference_past.html
Kubovy, M. (1988). Should we resist the seductiveness of the space:time::vision:audition analogy?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14/2: 318–20.
Lafon, J.-C. (1968). Auditory basis of phonetics. In Malmberg, B. (Ed.), Manual of Phonetics, pp. 76–104. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal About the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lakoff, G. (1990). The invariance hypothesis: Is abstract reason based on image schemas?Cognitive Linguistics, 1/1: 39–74.
Lakoff, G. (1991). Metaphor and War: The Metaphor System Used to Justify War in the Gulf. An open letter to the Internet.
Lakoff, G. (1993). The contemporary theory of metaphor. In Ortony, A. (Ed.), Metaphor and Thought (second edition), pp. 202–51. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lakoff, G. (2004). Don't Think of an Elephant: Know Your Values and Frame the Debate. White River Junction, VM: Chelsea Green.
Lakoff, G. (2008). The neural theory of metaphor. In Gibbs, R.W. (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought, pp. 17–38. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M.. (1980). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M.. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and its Challenge to Western Thought. New York: Basic Books.
Lakoff, G. and Turner, M.. (1987). More Than Cool Reason: A Field-guide to Poetic Metaphor. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Lalueza-Fox, C., Rosas, A., Estalrrich, A., Gigli, E., Campos, P.F. and García-Tabernero, A.. (2010). Genetic evidence for patrilocal mating behaviour among Neandertal groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108: 250–3.
Langacker, R. (2008). Cognitive Grammar: A Basic Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Langacker, R.W. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar (Vol. I). Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Langacker, R.W. (2013). Essentials of Cognitive Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Langone, J. (2000). The Mystery of Time: Humanity's Quest for Order and Measure. Washington DC: National Geographic Press.
Larsen, C. (2003). Equality for the sexes in human evolution? Early hominid sexual dimorphism and implications for mating systems and social behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 100/16: 9103–4.
Leakey, M.G., Spoor, F., Brown, F.H., Gathogo, P.N., Kiarie, C., Leakey, L.N. and McDougallet, I.. (2001). New hominin genus from eastern Africa shows diverse middle Pliocene lineages. Nature, 410: 433–40.
Leakey, R.E. (1981). The Making of Mankind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
LeDoux, J.E. (1995). Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46: 209–35.
Lee, N., Mikesell, L., Joaquin, A.D., Mates, A.W. and Schumann, J.H.. (2009). The Interactional Instinct: The Evolution and Acquisition of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Legerstee, M. (1992). A review of the animate-inanimate distinction in infancy: Implications for models of social and cognitive knowing. Early Development and Parenting, 1: 59–67.
Le Poidevin, R. (2003). Travels in Four Dimensions: The Enigmas of Space and Time. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Leslie, A.M. (1982). The perception of causality in infants. Perception, 11: 173–86.
Leslie, A.M. (1984). Infant perception of a manual pick-up event. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2: 19–32.
Levinson, S.C. (2003). Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cultural Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Levinson, S.C. (2006). On the human ‘interaction engine’. In Enfield, N.J. and Levinson, S.C. (Eds.). Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction, pp. 39–69. Oxford: Berg.
Lieberman, D.E. (2001). Another face in our family tree. Nature, 410: 419–20.
Lieberman, P. (2015). Review of The Science of Language: Interviews with James McGilvray, by Chomsky, Noam. Modern Language Review, 110/1: 222–4.
Lieberman, P. and Crelin, E.. (1971). On the speech of Neanderthal man. Linguistic Inquiry, 2: 203–22.
Linden, S.J. (2003). The Alchemy Reader: from Hermes Trismegistus to Isaac Newton. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Loftus, E., Schooler, J.W., Boone, S.M. and Kline, D.. (1987). Time went by so slowly: Overestimation of event duration by males and females. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 1/1: 3–13.
Lozano, M., Mosquera, M., Castro, J.De, Bermúdez, M., Arsuaga, J.L. and Carbonell, E.. (2009). Right handedness of Homo heidelbergensis from Sima de los Huesos (Atapuerca, Spain) 500,000 years ago. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30/5: 369–76.
Lucy, J.A. (1992). Language Diversity and Thought: A reformulation of the linguistic relativity hypothesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lurz, R. (2011). Mindreading Animals: The Debate about What Animals Know, and Other Minds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
MacIntosh, J.J. and Anstey, P.. (2014). Robert Boyle. In Zalta, E.N. (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2014/entries/boyle/.
Mandler, J.M. (1988). How to build a baby: On the development of an accessible representational system. Cognitive Development, 3: 113–36.
Mandler, J.M. (1992). How to build a baby II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99: 567–604.
Mandler, J.M. (1996). Preverbal representation and language. In Bloom, P., Peterson, M.A., Nadel, L. and Garrett, M.F. (Eds.), Language and Space, pp. 365–84. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Mandler, J.M. (2004). The Foundations of Mind: Origins of Conceptual Thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Martin, A. and Chao, L.L.. (2001). Semantic memory and the brain: Structure and processes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11: 194–201.
Martínez, I., Quam, R.M., Rosa, M., Jarabo, P., Lorenzo, C. and Arsuaga, J.L.. (2008). Auditory capacities of human fossils: A new approach to the origin of speech. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 123: 3606–3606.
Martínez, I., Rosa, M., Arsuaga, J.-L., Jarabo, P., Quam, R. and Lorenzo, C.. (2004). Auditory capacities in Middle Pleistocene humans from the Sierrade Atapuerca in Spain. Proceedings of the National Academy Sciences, USA, 101: 9976–81.
McPherron, S.P., Alemseged, Z., Marean, C., Wynn, J.G., Reed, D., Geraads, D., Bobe, R. and Béarat, H.. (2011). Tool-marked bones from before the oldowan change the paradigm. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108/21: e116.
Meltzoff, A. and Moore, M.. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198: 75–8.
Mithen, S. (1996). The Prehistory of the Mind: A Search for the Origins of Art, Science and Religion. London: Phoenix.
Miracle, A.W. Jr. and de Dios Yapita Moya, J.. (1981). Time and space in Aymara. In Hardman, M. J.. (ed), The Aymara Language and Its Social and Cultural Context. Gainesville, FL: University Presses of Florida, 33–56.
Moore, B.C. (1977). Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing. Baltimore, MA: University Park Press.
Moore, K.E. (2006). Space-to-time mappings and temporal concepts. Cognitive Linguistics, 17/2: 199–244.
Moore, K.E. (2013). The Spatial Language of Time: Metaphor, Metonymy and Frames of Reference. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Mounier, A., Marchal, F. and Condemi, S.. (2009). Is Homo heidelbergensis a distinct species? New insight on the Mauer mandible. Journal of Human Evolution, 56/3: 219–46.
Müller, M. (1861). The theoretical stage, and the origin of language. Lecture 9 from Lectures on the Science of Language. Reprinted in Harris, R. (Ed.), 1996, The Origin of Language, pp. 7–41. Bristol, Avon: Thoemmes Press.
Nerlich, B., Evans, V. and Koteyko, N.. (2011). Low carbon diet: Reducing the complexities of climate change to human scale. Language and Cognition, 3/1: 45–82.
Newman, W.R. and Principle, L.M.. (2002). Alchemy Tried in the Fire. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Núñez, R., Motz, B. and Teuscher, U.. (2006). Time after time: The psychological reality of the Ego- and Time-Reference-Point distinction in metaphorical construals of time. Metaphor and Symbol, 21: 133–46.
Núñez, R. and Sweetser, E.. (2006). With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30: 401–50.
Paget, R. (1930). Human Speech: Some Observations, Experiments, and Conclusions as to the Nature, Origin, Purpose and Possible Improvement of Human Speech. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Pansini, R. and de Ruiter, J.R.. (2011). Observation of tool use and modification for apparent hygiene purposes in a mandrill. Behavioural Processes 88/1: 53–5.
Patel, A. (2008). Music, Language and the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Peretz, I. (2006). The nature of music from a biological perspective. Cognition, 100: 1–32.
Pettitt, P.B. (2002).The Neanderthal dead: Exploring mortuary variability in Middle Palaeolithic Eurasia. Before Farming, 1: 1–19.
Peterson, D. (2011). The Moral Lives of Animals. London: Bloomsbury Press.
Piaget, J. (1962). The Language and Thought of the Child. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Pierce, C.S. (1903). A syllabus of certain topics of logic. EP, 2: 272–3.
Pika, S., Liebal, K., Call, J. and Tomasello, M.. (2005). Gestural communication of apes. Gesture, 5/1–2: 41–56.
Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works. New York: W.W. Norton and co.
Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: Theory, Research, and Experience. New York: Academic Press.
Plutchik, R. (2002). Emotions and Life: Perspectives from Psychology, Biology, and Evolution. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Pöppel, E. (1994). Temporal mechanisms in perception. In Sporns, O. and Tononi, G. (Eds.), Selectionism and the Brain: International Review of Neurobiology, 37: 185–201.
Pöppel, E. (2009). Pre-semantically defined temporal windows for cognitive processing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364: 1887–96.
Prinz, J. (2002). Furnishing the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Pruetz, J. and Bertolani, P.. (2007). Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, hunt with tools. Current Biology, 17: 412–17.
Pulvermüller, F. (1999). Words in the brain's language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22: 253–79.
Raichlen, D.A., Gordon, A.D., Harcourt-Smith, W.E.H., Foster, A.A. and Haas, Wm.R. Jr. (2010). Laetoli footprints preserve earliest direct evidence of human-like bipedal biomechanics. PLoS ONE, 5/3: e9769.
Rak, Y., Ginzburg, A. and Geffen, E.. (2007). Gorilla-like anatomy on Australopithecus afarensis mandibles suggests Au. Afarensis link to robust australopiths. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 104/16: 6568.
Ratliff, F., and Hartline, H.K.. (1974). Studies on Excitation and Inhibition in the Retina. New York: Rockefeller University Press.
Renfrew, C. (1990). Archaeology and Language: The Puzzle of Indo-European Origins. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Renfrew, C. (2008). Prehistory: The Making of the Human Mind. London: Phoenix.
Reynolds, S. and Gallagher, A.. (2012). African Genesis: Perspectives on Hominin Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Roebroeks, W. and Villa, P.. (2011). On the earliest evidence for habitual use of fire in Europe. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108: 5209–14.
Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In Lloyd, B. and Rosch, E. (Eds.), Cognition and Categorization, pp. 27–48. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ryle, G. (1949). The Concept of Mind. London: Penguin Books.
Sapir, E. (1921). Language: The Scientific Study of Speech. NY: Harcourt Brace and company.
Sandler, W., Aronoff, M., Meir, I. and Padden, C.. (2011). The gradual emergence of phonological form in a new language. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 29/2: 503–43.
Sandler, W., Meir, I., Padden, C. and Aronoff, M.. (2005). The emergence of grammar in a new sign language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 102/7: 2661–5.
Sandra, D. and Rice, S.. (1995). Network analyses of prepositional meaning: mirroring whose mind – the linguist's or the language user's?Cognitive Linguistics, 6/1: 89–130.
Saussure, F. de. (1916). Course in General Linguistics. London: Harcourt.
Schacter, D.L., Addis, D.R. and Buckner, R.L.. (2007). The prospective brain: Remembering the past to imagine the future. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8: 657–61.
Schmelz, M., Call, J. and Tomasello, M.. (2012). Chimpanzees know that others make inferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108/7: 3077–9.
Schmid, H.-J. (2000). English Abstract Nouns as Conceptual Shells: From Corpus to Cognition. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Senut, B., Pickford, M., Gommery, D., Mein, P., Cheboi, K. and Coppens, Y. (2001). First hominid from the Miocene (Lukeino Formation, Kenya). Comptes Rendus de l'Academie des Sciences, Series IIA – Earth and Planetary Science 332/2: 137–44.
Seyfarth, R.M., Cheney, D.L. and Marler, P.. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: Evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210: 801–3.
Shapiro, L. (2010). Embodied Cognition. London: Routledge.
Shipman, P. (2001). The Man who Found the Missing Link: The Extraordinary Life of Eugène Dubois. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Shinohara, K. (1999). Epistemology of Space and Time. Kwansei, Japan: Kwansei Gakuin University Press.
Shumaker, R.W., Walkup, K.R. and Beck, B.B.. (2011). Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals. Baltimore, MA: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Siffre, M. (1964). Beyond Time. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Sinha, C. (1982). Representational development and the structure of action. In Butterworth, P. and Light, G. (Eds.), Social Cognition: Studies of the Development of Understanding, pp. 137–62. Brighton: Harvester Press.
Sinha, C. (1988). Language and Representation: A Socio-Naturalistic Approach to Human Development. Hove: Harvester Press.
Sinha, C., da Silva Sinha, V., Zinken, J. and Sampaio, W.. (2011). When time is not space: The social and linguistic construction of time intervals and temporal event relations in an Amazonian culture. Language and Cognition, 3/1: 137–69.
Sørensen, B. (2009). Energy use by Eem Neanderthals. Journal of Archaeological Science, 36: 2201–5.
Sperber, D. and Wilson, D.. (1996). Relevance: Communication and Cognition (second edition). Oxford: Blackwell.
Spivey, M., Tyler, M., Richardson, D. and Young, E.. (2000). Eye movements during comprehension of spoken scene descriptions. In Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 487–92. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Spoor, F., Leakey, M.G., Gathogo, P.N., Brown, F.H., Antón, S.C., McDougall, I., Kiarie, C., Manthi, F.K. and Leakey, L.N.. (2007). Implications of new early Homo fossils from Ileret, east of Lake Turkana, Kenya. Nature, 448/7154: 688–91.
Stanfield, R.A. and Zwaan, R.A.. (2001). The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological Science, 12: 153–6.
Stanovich, K. (2011). Rationality and the Reflective Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Stern, J.T. Jr. and Susman, R.L.. (1983). The locomotor anatomy of Australopithecus afarensis. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 60/3: 279–317.
Stockwell, P. (2002). Cognitive Poetics: An Introduction. London: Routledge.
Sweetser, E. (1988). Grammaticalization and semantic bleaching. In Axmaker, S., Jaisser, A. and Singmaster, H. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 14th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, pp. 389–405. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.
Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a Cognitive Semantics (two volumes). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tattersall, T. (2012). Masters of the Planet: The Search for Our Human Origins. London: Palgrave Macmillan.
Taylor, J. (2002). Cognitive Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Taylor, J. (2003). Linguistic Categorization (third edition). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Taylor, J. (2012). The Mental Corpus: How Language is Represented in the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Taylor, L. and Zwaan, R. A.. (2009). Action in cognition: The case of language. Language and Cognition, 1/1: 45–58.
Thierry, G., Athanasopoulos, P., Wiggett, A., Dering, B. and Kuipers, J-R.. (2009). Unconscious effects of language-specific terminology on preattentive color perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 106/11: 4567–70.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Harvard, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a Language. Harvard, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M. (2008). The Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tomasello, M. (2011). Human culture in evolutionary perspective. In Gelfand, M. (Ed.), Advances in Culture and Psychology (pp. 5–51). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Tomasello, M. (2014). A Natural History of Human Thinking. Harvard, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M. and Call, J.. (1997). Primate Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Tomkins, S. (1998). The Origins of Humankind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Bullowa, M. (Ed.), Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication, pp. 321–47. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Turing, A.M. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind, 59: 433–60.
Turner, F. and Pöppel, E.. (1983). The neural lyre: Poetic meter, the brain and time. Poetry, 142/5: 277–309.
Turner, M. (1996). The Literary Mind: The Origins of Thought and Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Turner, M. (2014). The Origin of Ideas: Blending, Creativity and the Human Spark. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Tyler, A. (2012). Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Learning: Theoretical Basics and Experimental Evidence. London: Routledge.
Tyler, A. and Evans, V.. (2001). Reconsidering prepositional polysemy networks: The case of over. Language, 77/4: 724–65.
Tyler, A. and Evans, V.. (2003). The Semantics of English Prepositions: Spatial Scenes, Embodied Experience, and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ungar, P. (2004). Dental topography and diets of Australopithecus afarensis and earlyHomo. Journal of Human Evolution, 46: 605–22.
Van Schaik, C., Fox, E. and Sitompul, A.. (1996). Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans. Naturwissenschaften, 83: 186–8.
Vandeloise, C. (1994). Methodology and analyses of the preposition in. Cognitive Linguistics, 5/2: 157–84.
Varela, F. (1999). Present-time consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6/2-3: 111–40.
Varela, F., Thompson, E. and Rosch, E.. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Verri, G., Barkai, R., Bordeanu, V., Gopher, A., Hass, M. and Kaufman, A.. (2004). Flint mining in prehistory recorded by in situ-produced cosmogenic10Be. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 101: 7880–4.
Vigliocco, G., Meteyard, L., Andrews, M. and Kousta, S.. (2009). Toward a theory of semantic representation. Language and Cognition, 1/2: 219–48.
Villa, P. and Roebroeks, W.. (2014). Neanderthal demise: An archaeological analysis of the modern human superiority complex. PLoS ONE, 9/4: e96424.
Watson, J. (1972). Smiling, cooing, and ‘the game’. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18: 323–40.
Watts, I. (2009). Red ochre, body painting, and language: Interpreting the Blombos ochre. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (Eds.), The Cradle of Language, pp. 62–92. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Watts, D. and Mitani, J.C.. (2002). Hunting and meat sharing by Chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 23: 1–28.
Wertheimer, M. (1923 [1938]). Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt II, inPsycologische Forschung, 4, 301–350. Published as: Laws of Organization in Perceptual Forms, in Ellis, W. (1938). A source book of Gestalt psychology, pp. 71–88. London: Routledge.
White, T.D., Asfaw, B., Beyene, Y., Haile-Selassie, Y., Lovejoy, C.O., Suwa, G. and WoldeGabriel, G.. (2009). Ardipithecus ramidus and the paleobiology of early hominids. Science, 326 /5949: 75–86.
White, T.D., Suwa, G., Simpson, S. and Asfaw, B.. (2000). Jaws and teeth of Australopithecus afarensis from Maka, Middle Awash, Ethiopia. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 1111: 45–68.
Whitehead, H. (2003). Sperm Whales: Social Evolution in the Ocean. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Whorf, B.L. (1956). Language, Thought and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wiener, M., Turkeltaub, P., and Coslett, H.B.. (2010). The image of time: A voxel-wise meta-analysis. Neuroimage 49: 1728–40.
Wildman, D.E., Uddin, M., Guozhen, L., Grossman, L.I. and Goodman, M.. (2003). Implications of Natural Selection in Shaping 99.4% Nonsynonymous DNA Identity between Humans and Chimpanzees: Enlarging Genus Homo. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 100/12: 7181–8.
Wilkins, J.Schoville, B.J., Brown, K.S. and Chazan, M.. (2012). Evidence for early hafted hunting technology. Science, 338/6109: 942–6.
Williams, J.A. (2012). Lewis Carroll and the private life of words. Review of English Studies, 64/266: 651–71.
Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358: 749–50.
Wynn, K. (1995). Origins of numerical knowledge. Mathematical Cognition, 1: 35–60.
Yu, N. (2001). The Contemporary Theory of Metaphor: A Perspective from Chinese. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Zbikowski, L. (2001). Conceptualizing Music: Cognitive Structure, Theory, and Analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zilhão, J., Angelucci, D.E., Badal-Garcia, E., d'Errico, F., Daniel, F., and Dayet, L.. (2010). Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 107: 1023–8.
Zilhão, J. and Trinkaus, E.. (2003). Social implications. In ‘portrait of the artist as a child'. In Zilhão, J. and Trinkaus, E. (Eds.), The Gravettian Human Skeleton From the Abrigo Do Lagar Velho and its Archaeological Context, pp. 519–41. Lisbon, Portugal: Instituto Português de Arqueologia.
Zwaan, R.A. (2004). The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In Ross, B.H. (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, pp. 35–62. New York: Academic Press.
Zwaan, R.A. and Yaxley, R.H.. (2003). Hemispheric differences in semantic-relatedness judgments. Cognition, 87: 79–86.

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Book summary page views

Total views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between #date#. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.