Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T12:47:29.279Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - The more who die, the less we care: psychic numbing and genocide

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2014

Daniel Västfjäll
Affiliation:
Linköping University
Adam Oliver
Affiliation:
London School of Economics and Political Science
Get access

Summary

A defining element of catastrophes is the magnitude of their harmful consequences. To help society prevent or mitigate damage from catastrophes, immense effort and technological sophistication are often employed to assess and communicate the size and scope of potential or actual losses. This effort assumes that people can understand the resulting numbers and act on them appropriately.

However, recent behavioural research casts doubt on this fundamental assumption. Many people do not understand large numbers. Indeed, large numbers have been found to lack meaning and to be underweighted in decisions unless they convey affect (feeling). As a result, there is a paradox that rational models of decision-making fail to represent. On the one hand, we respond strongly to aid a single individual in need. On the other hand, we often fail to prevent mass tragedies – such as genocide – or take appropriate measures to reduce potential losses from natural disasters. This might seem irrational but we think this occurs, in part, because as numbers get larger and larger, we become insensitive; numbers fail to trigger the emotion or feeling necessary to motivate action.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Associated Press (1994). 38,000 shoes stand for loss in lethal year. The Register-Guard, 21 September: 6A.
Batson, C. D., O’Quin, K., Fultz, J., Vanderplas, M. and Isen, A. (1983). Self-Reported Distress and Empathy and Egoistic versus Altruistic Motivation for Helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45: 706–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clements, D. H. and Sarama, J. (2007). Early Childhood Mathematics Learning. In Lester, J. F. K. (ed.), Second Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning. New York: Information Age, pp. 461–555.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (1997). The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (2001). Précis of The Number Sense. Mind and Language 16: 16–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dillard, A. (1999). For the Time Being. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Farmer, P. (2005). Never Again? Reflections on Human Values and Human Rights. Tanner Lectures on Human Values, Salt Lake City, Utah ().
Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S. and Spelke, E. (2004). Core Systems of Number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 307–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fetherstonhaugh, D., Slovic, P., Johnson, S. M. and Friedrich, J. (1997). Insensitivity to the Value of Human Life: A Study of Psychophysical Numbing. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 14: 283–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001). The Emotional Dog and its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment. Psychological Review 108: 814–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Just, R. (2008). The Truth Will Not Set You Free: Everything We Know about Darfur and Everything We’re Not Doing about It. New Republic, August: 36–47.
Kingsolver, B. (1995). High Tide in Tucson. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Kogut, T. and Ritov, I. (2005). The ‘Identified Victim’ Effect: An Identified Group, or Just a Single Individual? Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 18: 157–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kristof, N. D. (2007). Save the Darfur puppy. New York Times, 10 May.
Libertus, M. E. and Brannon, E. M. (2009). Behavioral and Neural Basis for Number Sense in Infancy. Current Directions in Psychological Science 18: 346–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lifton, R. J. (1967). Death in Life: Survivors of Hiroshima. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Peters, E., Slovic, P., Västfjäll, D. and Mertz, C. K. (2008). Intuitive Numbers Guide Decisions. Judgment and Decision Making 3: 619–35.Google Scholar
Power, S. (2003). A Problem from Hell: America and the Age of Genocide. New York: Harper Perennial.Google Scholar
Schroeder, P. and Schroeder-Hildebrand, D. (2004). Six Million Paper Clips: The Making of a Children’s Holocaust Museum. Minneapolis, MN: Kar-Ben.Google Scholar
Slovic, P. (2007). ‘If I look at the mass I will never act’: Psychic Numbing and Genocide. Judgment and Decision Making 2: 79–95.Google Scholar
Slovic, P. (2010). The More Who Die, the Less We Care. In Michel-Kerjan, E. and Slovic, P. (eds.), The Irrational Economist: Making Decisions in a Dangerous World. New York: Public Affairs Press, pp. 30–40.Google Scholar
Slovic, P. and Västfjäll, D. (2010). Affect, Moral Intuition, and Risk. Psychological Inquiry: An International Journal for the Advancement of Psychological Theory 21: 387–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P., Zionts, D., Woods, A. K., Goodman, R. and Jinks, D. (2013). Psychic Numbing and Mass Atrocity. In Shafir, E. (ed.), The Behavioral Foundations of Public Policy. Princeton University Press, pp. 126–42.Google Scholar
Small, D. A., Loewenstein, G. and Slovic, P. (2007). Sympathy and Callousness: The Impact of Deliberative Thought on Donations to Identifiable and Statistical Victims. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 102: 143–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. and Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal Construal. Psychological Review 110: 403–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
UN General Assembly (1948). Convention on the Prevention and Punishment of the Crime of Genocide ().
UN General Assembly (2005). Resolution adopted by the General Assembly: 60/1. 2005 World Summit Outcome ().
Västfjäll, D., Peters, E. and Slovic, P. (2010). Compassion Fatigue: Donations and Affect are Greatest for a Single Child in Need (manuscript in preparation).
Vendantam, S. (2010). The Hidden Brain: How Our Unconscious Minds Elect Presidents, Control Markets, Wage Wars, and Save Our Lives. New York: Spiegel & Grau.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×