Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 August 2016
Introduction
Anosognosia refers to the lack of awareness, misbelief, or explicit denial of their illness that patients may show following brain damage or dysfunction. Anosognosia may involve a variety of neurological impairment of sensorimotor, visual, cognitive or behavioral functions, as well as non-neurological diseases. The term was coined by Joseph Babinski in 1914 when he reported at the meeting of the Society of Neurology of Paris on two patients who had left hemiplegia after a right hemisphere stroke but ‘were ignorant or seemed ignorant of the paralysis which affected them’ (Babinski, 1914). Babinski emphasized the absence of confusion and major intellectual disturbance. During the discussion that followed his report, he and the neurologists who attended the meeting speculated on more than a halfdozen possible mechanisms to explain anosognosia. Babinski himself considered a deliberate protection of self-esteem and self-appearance as plausible but unlikely, and he pointed to the special role of right hemisphere lesions. He also stressed the possible role of a severe sensory loss. Dejerine and Souques agreed with the latter, while Ballet pondered on an underlying disorder of anterograde memory. Meige argued for a selective functional motor amnesia superimposed on hemiplegia. Claude raised the issue of a loss of representation of the affected limbs and, in addition, related anosognosia to cases with unilateral apraxia and topoanesthesia that would nowadays suggest some sort of interhemispheric disconnection syndrome. Finally, after a second report of Babinski in 1918, Pierre Marie put forward the role of spatial neglect. More than 80 years later, explanations for anosognosia appear not to have gone far beyond the above discussion; many of these speculations are still in place.
Anosognosia had actually been observed long before Babinski's report. While Seneca made mention of a woman who ‘did not know that she was blind’ (cited in Bisiach and Geminiani, 1991), and Montaigne (1588) discussed a similar disorder in a nobleman, unawareness of blindness was first described as a neurological condition in 1885 by Von Monakow in two patients with bilateral posterior brain damage. Others (e.g., Dejerine and Viallet, 1893; Rossplimo, 1896), described similar cases a few years later. However, Anton (1898) first theorized about the mechanisms of these disorders with respect to perceptual awareness. He described a patient who denied her cortical blindness, as well as two other patients who appeared unaware of their receptive aphasia and cortical deafness (Anton, 1898).
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.