Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-14T13:45:43.564Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20 - Plurilingualism and Young Children’s Perspectival Cognition

from Part Four - Language(s) and Literacy of Multilingual Children through Schooling

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 August 2022

Anat Stavans
Affiliation:
Beit Berl College, Israel
Ulrike Jessner
Affiliation:
Leopold-Franzens-Universität Innsbruck, Austria
Get access

Summary

The chapter explores how plurilingualism potentially influences young children's perspectival cognition, including understandings of false belief and implications for cooperative science inquiry.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aikenhead, G. S., & Elliott, D. (2010). An emerging decolonizing science education in Canada. Canadian Journal of Science, Mathematics and Technology Education, 10(4), 321–38, https://doi.org/1080/14926156.2010.524967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bak, T. H. (2015). Beyond a simple “yes” and “no.Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 73, 332–33, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.08.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barone, P., Corradi, G., & Gomila, A. (2019). Infants’ performance in spontaneous-response false belief tasks: A review and meta-analysis. Infant Behavior & Development, 57, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(5), 857–94, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716414000174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berguno, G., & Bowler, D. M. (2004). Communicative interactions, knowledge of a second language, and theory of mind in young children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human Development, 165(3), 293309, https://doi.org/10.3200/GNTP.165.3.293-309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122(1), 6773, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Senman, L. (2004). Executive processes in appearance–reality tasks: The role of inhibition of attention and symbolic representation. Child Development, 75(2), 562–79, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00693.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callaghan, T., Rochat, P., Lillard, A., Claux, M. L., Odden, H., Itakura, S., … Singh, S. (2005). Synchrony in the onset of mental-state reasoning: Evidence from five cultures. Psychological Science, 16, 378–84, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01544.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpendale, J. I. M., & Lewis, C. (2015). The development of social understanding. In Liben, L. S., Müller, U., & Lerner, R. M., eds., Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science: Cognitive Processes., Vol. 2, 7th ed.. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 381424, http://proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2015-15510-010&site=ehost-liveGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2013). Mindreading the self. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Lombardo, M. V, eds., Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Social Neuroscience, 3rd Ed. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 467–85, https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199692972.003.0026.Google Scholar
Clements, W. A., Rustin, C. L., & McCallum, S. (2000). Promoting the transition from implicit to explicit understanding: A training study of false belief. Developmental Science, 3(1), 8192, https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coste, D., Moore, D., & Zarate, G. (2009). Plurilingual and Pluricultural Competence. Strasbourg: Council of Europe, https://rm.coe.int/168069d29b.Google Scholar
Council of Europe (2001). Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: Learning, Teaching, Assessment. Strasbourg: Language Policy Unit, www.coe.int/t/dg4/linguistic/source/framework_en.pdf.Google Scholar
Cox, S. R., Bak, T. H., Allerhand, M., Redmond, P., Starr, J. M., Deary, I. J., & MacPherson, S. E. (2016). Bilingualism, social cognition and executive functions: A tale of chickens and eggs. Neuropsychologia, 91, 299306, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csibra, G., Hernik, M., Mascaro, O., Tatone, D., & Lengyel, M. (2016). Statistical treatment of looking-time data. Developmental Psychology, 52(4), 521–36, https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahlgren, S., Almén, H., & Dahlgren Sandberg, A. (2017) Theory of mind and executive functions in young bilingual children, The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 178(5), 303–07, https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2017.1361376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, D. (2001) Subjective, Intersubjective, Objective. Oxford: Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. (2013). Language and reasoning about beliefs. In Banaji, M. R. & Gelman, S. A., eds., Navigating the Social World: What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 96100, https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199890712.003.0018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (2014). The role of language in theory of mind development. Topics in Language Disorders, 34(4), 313–28, https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000000037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devine, R., & Hughes, C. (2017). Let’s talk: Parents’ mental talk (not mind-mindedness or mindreading capacity) predicts children’s false belief understanding. Child Development, 90, https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12990.Google ScholarPubMed
Diaz, V., & Farrar, M. J. (2018). The missing explanation of the false‐belief advantage in bilingual children: A longitudinal study. Developmental Science, 21(4), 113, http://proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2018-31287-003&site=ehost-live.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickson, S., Webber, S., & Takaro, T. D. (2014). Preparing BC for Climate Migration. Vancouver: Canadian Centre for Policy Alternatives.Google Scholar
Goetz, P. J. (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(1), 115, https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728903001007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (1992). Why the child’s theory of mind really is a theory. Mind & Language, 7(1–2), 145–71, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.1992.tb00202.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grein, M. (2008). Sociological concepts and their impact on rhetoric: Japanese language concepts. In Weigand, E., ed., Dialogue and Rhetoric. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 195207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grueneisen, S., & Tomasello, M. (2020). The development of coordination via joint expectations for shared benefits. Developmental Psychology, 56(6), 1149–56, https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. (2005). Conversation, pretense and theory of mind. In Astington, J. & Baird, J., eds., Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 7083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. (2014). False belief in infancy: A fresh look. Developmental Science, 17(5), 647–59, https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., Devine, R. T., & Wang, Z. (2018). Does parental mind-mindedness account for cross-cultural differences in preschoolers’ theory of mind? Child Development, 89(4), 1296—310, https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jessner, U. (2008). A DST model of multilingualism and the role of metalinguistic awareness. Modern Language Journal, 92(2), 270–83, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2008.00718.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerr, J., & Parent, A. (2018). The First Peoples’ Principles of Learning in teacher education: Responding to the Truth and Reconciliation Commission’s calls to action. Canadian Journal of Native Education, 40(1), 3653.Google Scholar
Kovács, Á. M. (2009). Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false-belief reasoning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 4854, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00742.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawson, D. F., Stevenson, K. T., Peterson, M. N., Carrier, S. J., Strnad, L., R., & Seekamp, E. (2019). Children can foster climate change concern among their parents. Nature Climate Change, 9, 458–62, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558–019-0463-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lecce, S., & Hughes, C. (2010). The Italian job? Comparing theory of mind performance in British and Italian children. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 747–66, https://doi.org/10.1348/026151009X479006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, K., Olson, D.R., & Torrance, N. (1999). Chinese children’s understanding of false beliefs: the role of language. Journal of Child Language, 26(1), 121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, D., Wellman, H. M., Tardif, T., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2008). Theory of mind development in Chinese children: A meta-analysis of false belief understanding across cultures and languages. Developmental Psychology, 4(2), 523–31, https://doi.org/ 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.523.Google Scholar
Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: a training study. Child Development, 74(4), 1130–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lüdi, G., & Py, B. (2009). To be or not to be … a plurilingual speaker. International Journal of Multilingualism, 6(2), 154–67, https://doi.org/10.1080/14790710902846715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsui, T., Rakoczy, H., Miura, Y., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Understanding of speaker certainty and false-belief reasoning: A comparison of Japanese and German preschoolers. Developmental Science, 12(4), 602–13, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00812.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Leekam, S. R., & de Rosnay, M. (2013). Mind-mindedness and theory of mind: Mediating roles of language and perspectival symbolic play. Child Development, https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12061.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mildenberger, M., Howe, P. D., Lachapelle, E., Stokes, L. C., Marlon, J., & Gravelle, T. (2016). The distribution of climate change public opinion in Canada. PLoS ONE, 11(8), e0159774.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, P. H., & Aloise, P. A. (1989). Young children’s understanding of the psychological causes of behavior: A review. Child Development, 60(2), 257–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–46, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, D. (2015). Paroles enfantines à propos … du plurilinguisme et des écritures des langues. Sinogrammes, textes polygraphiques et dialogues métagraphiques. In Gradoux, X., Jacquin, J., & Merminod, G., eds., Agir dans la diversité des langues. Mélanges en l’honneur d’Anne-Claude Berthoud. Louvain-la-Neuve: De Boeck, pp. 209–24.Google Scholar
Moore, D. (2018). PASTeL au musée. Plurilinguismes, AST (Art, Sciences, Technologie) et Littératies, quelles contributions pour la didactique du plurilinguisme? Mélanges CRAPEL, 38(1), 5981, www.atilf.fr/IMG/pdf/5_moore.pdf.Google Scholar
Moore, D., Hoskyn, M., & Mayo, J. (2018). Thinking language awareness at a science centre: iPads, science and early literacy development with multilingual, kindergarten children in Canada. International Journal of Bias, Identity and Diversities in Education (IJBIDE), 3(1), 4062, https://doi.org/10.4018/IJBIDE.2018010104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, T.-K., & Astington, J. W. (2014). Reassessing the bilingual advantage in theory of mind and its cognitive underpinnings. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(2), 396409, https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs?. Science, 308(5719), 255–58, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1107621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ontai, L. L., & Thompson, R. A. (2008). Attachment, parent–child discourse and theory-of-mind development. Social Development, 17(1), 4760, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00414.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds’ difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–37, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1987.tb01048.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that …”: Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39(3), 437–71, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubio-Fernández, P. (2017). Why are bilinguals better than monolinguals at false-belief tasks? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(3), 987–98, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423–016-1143-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubio-Fernández, P., & Geurts, B. (2013). How to pass the false-belief task before your fourth birthday. Psychological Science, 24(1), 2733, https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612447819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruffman, T., & Taumoepeau, M. (2017). Behaviour to beliefs. In Slaughter, V. & de Rosnay, M., eds., Theory of Mind Development in Context. Ruffman: Routledge, pp. 7186, http://proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2016-53396-005&site=ehost-live.Google Scholar
Sawatzky, A., Cunsolo, A., Jones-Bitton, A. et al. (2020). “The best scientists are the people that’s out there”: Inuit-led integrated environment and health monitoring to respond to climate change in the Circumpolar North. Climatic Change, 160, 4566, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584–019-02647-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. (2017). Early false-belief understanding. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(4), 237–49, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senju, A., Southgate, V., Snape, C., Leonard, M., & Csibra, G. (2011). Do 18-month-olds really attribute mental states to others?: A critical test. Psychological Science, 22(7), 878–80, https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611411584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tardif, T.,Wellman, H. M., & Cheung, K. M. (2004). False belief understanding in Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Child Languages, 31(4), 779800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., (2018). How children come to understand false beliefs: A shared intentionality account. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(34), 8491–98, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1804761115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2014). A Natural History of Human Thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turnbull, W., Carpendale, J., & Racine, T. (2008) Relations between mother–child talk and 3- to 5-year-old children’s understanding of belief: Beyond mental state terms to talk about the mind. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54(3), https://digitalcommons.wayne.edu/mpq/vol54/iss3/5.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (2018). Theory of mind: The state of the art. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15(6), 728–55, https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2018.1435413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–84, www.jstor.org/stable/1132444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–28, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×