Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-pftt2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-06T17:58:14.128Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

30 - Computational Models of Emotion and Cognition-Emotion Interaction

from Part IV - Computational Modeling in Various Cognitive Fields

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 April 2023

Ron Sun
Affiliation:
Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute, New York
Get access

Summary

Recent decades have witnessed a rapid growth in computational emotion modeling. Models are being developed to enhance believability and autonomy of virtual agents and robots, and for basic research purposes, to help elucidate mechanisms mediating affective processes in biological agents.This chapter provides a comprehensive introduction and state-of-the-art overview of this emerging subdiscipline within the broader area of affective computing, focusing on models at the psychological (vs. neuroscience) level, and those that emphasize cognition emotion interactions.Following an overview of emotion research from psychology, the theoretical foundations for model design are discussed. An analytical framework is then introduced, to promote a more abstract perspective on model design and analysis, followed by a discussion of specific approaches to modeling emotion generation and emotion effects, along with examples of representative models.The chapter concludes with a discussion of model validation and evaluation, and highlights some of the open questions and key challenges.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adam, C., Herzig, A., & Longin, D. (2009). A logical formalization of the OCC theory of emotions. Synthese, 168 (2), 201248.Google Scholar
Alfonso, B., Vivancos, E., & Botti, V. J. (2014). An open architecture for affective traits in a BDI agent. Paper presented at the 6th ECTA (the 6th IJCCI).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alfonso, B., Vivancos, E., & Botti, V. (2017). Toward formal modeling of affective agents in a BDI architecture. ACM Transactions on Internet Technology (TOIT), 17, Article 5. JCR 0.705 – 71/106 Q3 T2.Google Scholar
Andre, E., Klesen, M., Gebhard, P., Allen, S., & Rist, T. (2000). Exploiting models of personality and emotions to control the behavior of animated interactive agents. In Proceedings of IWAI, Siena, Italy.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (2005). Beware the passionate robot. In Fellous, J.-M. & Arbib, M. A. (Eds.), Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot (pp. 333383). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and Personality. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1994). I Feel, Therefore I Am – I Think. In Ekman, P. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Aylett, R., Louchart, S., Dias, J., Paiva, A., & Vala, M. (2005). Fearnot! – an experiment in emergent narrative. Paper presented at Intelligent Virtual Agents 2005.Google Scholar
Aylett, R. S. (2004). Agents and affect: why embodied agents need affective systems. Paper presented at the 3rd Hellenic Conference on AI, Samos, Greece.Google Scholar
Bach, J. (2009). Principles of Synthetic Intelligence: Psi: An Architecture of Motivated Cognition. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 124.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2014). The conceptual act theory: a précis. Emotion Review, 6 (4), 292297. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073914534479er.sagepub.comGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2017). How Emotions Are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Lewis, M., & Haviland-Jones, J. M. (2016). Handbook of Emotions (4th ed.). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Bates, J., Loyall, A. B., & Reilly, W. S. (1992). Integrating reactivity, goals, and emotion in a broad agent. In Proceedings of the 14th Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Beaudoin, L., Pudlo, M., & Hyniewska, S. (2020). Mental perturbance: an integrative design-oriented concept for understanding repetitive thought, emotions and related phenomena involving a loss of control of executive functions. SFU Educational Review, 13 (1), 2958.Google Scholar
Becker-Asano, C. (2008). WASABI: Affect Simulation for Agents with Believable Interactivity. Clifton, VA: IOS Press.Google Scholar
Becker-Asano, C. (2013). WASABI for affect simulation in human-computer interaction Architecture description and example applications. Ph.D. Thesis, Bielefeld University.Google Scholar
Becker-Asano, C., Kopp, S., Pfeiffer-Leßmann, N., & Wachsmuth, I. (2008). Virtual humans growing up: from primary toward secondary emotions. KI – Künstliche Intelligenz, 1, 2327.Google Scholar
Becker-Asano, C., Stahl, P., Ragni, M., Courgeon, M., Martin, J.-C., & Nebel, B. (2013). An affective virtual agent providing embodied feedback in the paired associate task: system design and evaluation. Paper presented at IVA 2013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker-Asano, C., Meneses, E., Riesterer, N., Hue, J., Dornhege, C., & Nebel, B. (2014). The hybrid Agent MARCO: a multimodal autonomous robotic chess opponent. Paper presented at the 2nd International Conference on Human-Agent Interaction, Tsukuba, Japan.Google Scholar
Becker-Asano, C., & Wachsmuth, I. (2009). Affective computing with primary and secondary emotions in a virtual human. Paper presented at the Autonomous Agents and Multi-Agent Systems.Google Scholar
Becker-Asano, C., & Wachsmuth, I. (2010). Affective computing with primary and secondary emotions in a virtual human. Autonomous Agents and Multi-Agent Systems, 20, 3249.Google Scholar
Belavkin, R. V., & Ritter, F. E. (2004). OPTIMIST: a new conflict resolution algorithm for ACT-R. In Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Cognitive Modeling, Pittsburgh, PA.Google Scholar
Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 99 (2), 229246.Google Scholar
Bless, H., & Fiedler, K. (2006). Mood and the regulation of information processing and behavior. In Forgas, J. P. (Ed.), Hearts and Minds: Affective Influences on Social Cognition and Behaviour (pp. 6584). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bosse, T. (2017). On computational models of emotion regulation and their applications within HCI. In Jeon, M. (Ed.), Emotions and Affect in Human Factors and Human-Computer Interaction (pp. 311337). London: Academic Press/Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosse, T., Broekens, J., Dias, J., & van der Zwaan, J. (2014). Emotion Modeling: Towards Pragmatic Computational Models of Affective Processes (LNAI 8750). Cham: Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Bosse, T., Duell, R., Memon, Z. A., Treur, J., & Wal, C. N. v. d. (2015). Agent-based modeling of emotion contagion in groups. Cognitive Computation, 7, 111136. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12559-014-9277-9Google Scholar
Bosse, T., Gerritsen, C., & Man, J. d. (2014). Agent-based simulation as a tool for the design of a virtual training environment. Paper presented at the 14th International Conference on Intelligent Agent Technology (IAT’14).Google Scholar
Bosse, T., & Zwanenburg, E. (2014). Do prospect-based emotions enhance believability of game characters? A case study in the context of a dice game. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 5 (1), 1731. https://doi.org/10.1109/T-AFFC.2013.30Google Scholar
Boukricha, H., Wachsmuth, I., Carminati, M., & Knoeferle, P. (2013). A computational model of empathy: empirical evaluation. Paper presented at the Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction (ACII).Google Scholar
Boukricha, H., & Wachsmuth, I. (2011). Empathy-based emotional alignment for a virtual human: a three-step approach. KI – Künstliche Intelligenz, 25 (3), 195204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129148.Google Scholar
Bower, G. H. (1992). How might emotions affect memory? In Christianson, S. A. (Ed.), Handbook of Emotion and Memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Breazeal, C. L. (2003). Emotion and sociable humanoid robots. International Journal of Human Computer Studies, 59 (12), 119155.Google Scholar
Breazeal, C., & Brooks, R. (2005). Robot emotion: a functional perspective. In Fellous, J.-M. & Arbib, M. A. (Eds.), Who Needs Emotions? New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Broekens, J. (2010). Modelling the experience of emotion. International Journal of Synthetic Emotions, 1 (1), 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broekens, J. (2011). Computational affective science. International Journal of Synthetic Emotions, 2 (2), 7375.Google Scholar
Broekens, J., Bosse, T., & Marsella, S. (2013). Challenges in computational modeling of affective processes. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 4 (3), 242245. https://doi.org/10.1109/T-AFFC.2013.23Google Scholar
Broekens, J., & Dai, L. (2019). A TDRL model for the emotion of regret. Paper presented at the 8th International Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction (ACII), Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Broekens, J., DeGroot, D., & Kosters, W. A. (2008). Formal models of appraisal: theory, specification, and computational model. Cognitive Systems Research, 9 (3), 173197.Google Scholar
Broekens, J., Hudlicka, E., & Bidarra, R. (2016). Emotional appraisal engines for games. In Karpouzis, C. & Yannakakis, G. (Eds.), Emotion in Games (Vol. 4, pp. 215232). Cham: Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Broekens, J., Jacobs, E., & Jonker, C. M. (2015). A reinforcement learning model of joy, distress, hope and fear. Connection Science, 27 (4), 119.Google Scholar
Brosch, T. (2013). Comment: on the role of appraisal processes in the construction of emotion. Emotion Review, 5 (4), 369373. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913489752Google Scholar
Busemeyer, J. R., Dimperio, E., & Jessup, R. K. (2007). Integrating emotional processes into decision-making models. In Gray, W. (Ed.), Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cañamero, L. (1997). A hormonal model of emotions for behavior control. Paper presented at the 4th European Conference on Artificial Life (ECAL ‘97), Brighton, UK.Google Scholar
Cañamero, L. D. (2001). Building emotional artifacts in social worlds: challenges and perspectives. Paper presented at the AAAI Fall Symposium “Emotional and Intelligent II: The Tangled Knot of Social Cognition,” Cape Cod, MA.Google Scholar
Cañamero, L., & Avila-Gracia, O. (2007). A bottom-up investigation of emotional modulation in competitive scenarios. Paper presented at the Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction.Google Scholar
Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions: a critical examination and an alternative theory. American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106124.Google Scholar
Castelfranchi, C., & Miceli, M. (2009). The cognitive-motivational compound of emotional experience. Emotion Review, 1 (3), 223231.Google Scholar
Castellanos, S., Rodriguez, L.-F., & Gutierrez-Garcia, J. O. (2019). A mechanism for biasing the appraisal process in affective agents. Cognitive Systems Research, 58, 351365.Google Scholar
Clore, G. L. (1994). Why emotions are felt? In Ekman, P. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (2013). Psychological construction in the OCC model of emotion. Emotion Review, 5 (4), 335343. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913489751er.sagepub.comGoogle Scholar
Coenen, R., & Broekens, J. (2012). Modeling emotional contagion based on experimental evidence for moderating factors. Paper presented at the Workshop on Emotional and Empathic Agents, at the 11th International Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems, Valencia, Spain.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Four ways five factors are basic. Personality & Individual Differences, 13, 653665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A., & Carvalho, G. B. (2013). The nature of feelings: evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14 (2), 143152. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3403Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York, NY: Putnam.Google Scholar
Dancy, C. L. (2013). ACT-RΦ: a cognitive architecture with physiology and affect. Biologically Inspired Cognitive Architectures, 6 (1), 4045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dastani, M., & Lorini, E. (2012). A logic of emotions: from appraisal to coping. Paper presented at the 11th International Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems.Google Scholar
Dastani, M., & Pankov, A. (2017). Other-condemning moral emotions: anger, contempt and disgust. ACM Transactions on Internet Technologies, 17 (1), 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, R., Scherer, K., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2003). Handbook of Affective Sciences. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
de Rosis, F., Pelachaud, C., Poggi, I., Carofiglio, V., & De Carolis, B. (2003). From Greta’s mind to her face: modelling the dynamics of affective states in a conversational embodied agent. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 59 (12), 81118.Google Scholar
Derryberry, D. (1988). Emotional influences on evaluative judgments: roles of arousal, attention, and spreading activation. Motivation and Emotion, 12 (1), 2355.Google Scholar
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 225236.Google Scholar
Dias, J., Mascarenhas, S., & Paiva, A. (2014). FAtiMA modular: towards an agent architecture with a generic appraisal framework. In Bosse, T., Broekens, J., Dias, J., & van der Zwaan, J. (Eds.), Towards Pragmatic Computational Models of Affective Processes. Cham: Springer.Google Scholar
Dias, J., & Paiva, A. (2005). Feeling and reasoning: a computational model for emotional agents. In Proceedings of the 12th Portuguese Conference on Artificial Intelligence (EPIA).Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6 (3–4), 169200.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1994). All emotions are basic. In Ekman, P. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The Nature of Emotions: Fundamental Questions (pp. 1519). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Cordaro, D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emotion Review, 3 (4), 364370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Elliot, C. (1992). The affective reasoner: a process model of emotions in a multiagent system. Ph.D. Thesis, Northwestern University, Evanston.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of Affective Sciences. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
El-Nasr, M. S., Yen, J., & Ioerger, T. R. (2000). FLAME – Fuzzy logic adaptive model of emotions. Autonomous Agents and Multi-Agent Systems, 3 (3), 219257.Google Scholar
Fellous, J. M. (2004). From human emotions to robot emotions. In Proceedings of the AAAI Spring Symposium 2004: Architectures for Modeling Emotion, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA.Google Scholar
Fellous, J. M., & Arbib, M. A. (2005). Who Needs Emotions? New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fitrianie, S., Bruijnes, M., Richards, D., Abdulrahman, A., & Brinkman, W.-P. (2019). What are we measuring anyway? A literature survey of questionnaires used in studies reported in the intelligent virtual agent conferences. Paper presented at Intelligent Virtual Agent Conference (IVA), Paris, France.Google Scholar
Fontaine, J. R. J., Scherer, K., Roesch, E. B., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2007). The world of emotions is not two-dimensional. Psychological Science, 18 (12), 10501057. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02024.xGoogle Scholar
Forgas, J. (1995). Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117 (1), 3966.Google Scholar
Forgas, J. (2003). Affective influences on attitudes and judgments. In Davidson, K. R. S. R. J. & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of Affective Sciences. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (2017). Mood effects on cognition: affective influences on the content and process of information processing and behavior. In Jeon, M. (Ed.), Emotions and Affect in Human Factors and Human-Computer Interaction (pp. 89122). London: Academic Press/Elsevier.Google Scholar
Fox, A. S., Lapate, R. C., Shackman, A. J., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (2018). The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Franklin, S., Madl, T., D’Mello, S., & Snaider, J. (2014). LIDA: a systems-level architecture for cognition, emotion, and learning. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 6 (6), 1941. https://doi.org/10.1109/TAMD.2013.2277589Google Scholar
Frederickson, B., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19 (3), 313332. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930441000238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. (1993). Moods, emotion episodes, and emotions. In Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. M. (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. (2008). The psychologists’ point of view. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions (3rd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H., & Swagerman, J. (1987). Can computers feel? Theory and design of an emotional system. Cognition and Emotion, 1 (3), 235257.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H., & Scherer, K. R. (2009). Emotion definition (psychological perspectives). In Sander, D. & Scherer, K. R. (Eds.), Oxford Companion to Emotion and the Affective Sciences (pp. 142143). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13 (1), 3440.Google Scholar
Gephard, P. (2005). ALMA – a layered model of affect. In Proceedings of the Fourth International Joint Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems.Google Scholar
Gluz, J., & Jaques, P. A. (2017). A probabilistic formalization of the appraisal for the OCC event-based emotions. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 58 (1), 627664.Google Scholar
Gratch, J., & Marsella, S. (2004). A domain-independent framework for modeling emotion. Journal of Cognitive Systems Research, 5 (4), 269306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gratch, J., & Marsella, S. (2015). Appraisal models. In Calvo, R. A., D’Mello, S., Gratch, J., & Kappas, A. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Affective Computing. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gratch, J., Marsella, S., Wang, N., & Stankovic, B. (2009). Assessing the validity of appraisal-based models of emotion. In Proceedings of the 3rd Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction (ACII).Google Scholar
Gray, J. R., Schaefer, A., Braver, T. S., & Most, S. B. (2005). Affect and the resolution of cognitive control dilemmas. In Feldman-Barrett, L., Niedenthal, P. M., & Winkielman, P. (Eds.), Emotion and Consciousness. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hamann, S. (2012). Mapping discrete and dimensional emotions onto the brain: controversies and consensus. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16 (9), 458466. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.07.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hesp, C., Smith, R., Parr, T., Allen, M., Friston, K. J., & Ramstead, M. J. D. (2021). Deeply felt affect: the emergence of valence in deep active inference. Neural Computation, 33, 149. https://doi.org/10.1162/neco_a_01341Google Scholar
Hindriks, K. V., & Broekens, J. (2011). Comparing formal cognitive emotion theories. Paper presented at the Standards in Emotion Modeling, Leiden, Netherlands.Google Scholar
Hoemann, K., Devlin, M., & Barrett, L. F. (2019). Comment: emotions are abstract, conceptual categories that are learned by a predicting brain. Emotion Review, 12 (4), 253255.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (1998). Modeling emotion in symbolic cognitive architectures. In Proceedings of the AAAI Fall Symposium: Emotional and Intelligent I, Orlando, FL.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2002). This time with feeling: integrated model of trait and state effects on cognition and behavior. Applied Artificial Intelligence, 16, 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2003). Modeling effects of behavior moderators on performance: evaluation of the MAMID methodology and architecture. In Proceedings of BRIMS-12, Phoenix, AZ.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2004). Two sides of appraisal: implementing appraisal and its consequences within a cognitive architecture. In Proceedings of the AAAI Spring Symposium: Architectures for Modeling Emotion, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2007). Reasons for emotions. In Gray, W. (Ed.), Advances in Cognitive Models and Cognitive Architectures. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2008a). What are we modeling when we model emotion?. In AAAI Spring Symposium: Emotion, Personality, and Social Behavior (Vol. Technical Report SS-08-04, pp. 5259), Stanford University, CA. Menlo Park, CA: AAAI Press.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2008b). Modeling the mechanisms of emotion effects on cognition. Paper presented at the AAAI Fall Symposium on Biologically Inspired Cognitive Architectures, Arlington, VA.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2012). Guidelines for designing computational models of emotions. International Journal of Synthetic Emotions (IJSE), 2 (1), 2679.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2014a). From habits to standards: towards systematic design of emotion models and affective architectures. In Tibor Bosse, J. B., Dias, J., & van der Zwaan, J. (Eds.), Towards Pragmatic Computational Models of Affective Processes (pp. 121). Cham: Springer.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2014b). Affective BICA: challenges and open questions. Biologically Inspired Cognitive Architectures, 7, 98125.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2014c). From cognitive biases to panic: modeling the mechanisms of anxiety disorders. Paper presented at the Workshop on “Computational Modeling of Cognition-Emotion Interactions: Relevance to Mechanisms of Affective Disorders,” in conjunction with CogSci, Quebec City, Quebec, Canada.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2016). Virtual companions, coaches, and therapeutic games in psychotherapy. In Luxton, D. D. (Ed.), Artificial Intelligence in Mental Healthcare. Waltham, MA: Academic Press/Elsevier.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2017). Computational modeling of cognition‐emotion interactions: theoretical and practical relevance for behavioral healthcare. In Jeon, M. (Ed.), Emotions and Affect in Human Factors and Human-Computer Interaction (pp. 383436). London: Academic Press/Elsevier.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2019a). Modeling cognition–emotion interactions in symbolic agent architectures: examples of research and applied models. In Aldinhas Ferreira, M. I., Silva Sequeira, J., & Ventura, R. (Eds.), Cognitive Architectures (Vol. 94). Cham: Springer.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2019b). Cognitive-affective architectures as clinical case formulations. Paper presented at the ISRE, Amsterdam, Netherlands.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2020). The case for cognitive-affective architectures as affective user models in behavioral health technologies. In Schmorrow, D. & C. Fidopiastis, (Eds.), Augmented Cognition. Human Cognition and Behavior (Vol. 12197). Cham: Springer.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M. (1993). Positive affect and decision making. In Haviland, J. M. & Lewis, M. (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human Emotions. New York, NY: Plenum.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1993). Four systems for emotion activation: cognitive and noncognitive processes. Psychological Review, 100 (1), 6890.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (2009). Emotion theory and research: highlights, unanswered questions, and emerging issues. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 125.Google Scholar
Jack, R., Garrod, O. G. B., & Schyns, P. (2014). Dynamic facial expressions of emotion transmit an evolving hierarchy of signals over time. Current Biology, 24, 187192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.064Google Scholar
Jack, R., Garrod, O. G. B., Yu, H., Caldara, R., & Schyns, P. G. (2012). Facial expressions of emotion are not culturally universal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (online). www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1200155109 [last accessed July 25, 2022].Google Scholar
Jiang, H., Vidal, J. M., & Huhns, M. N. (2007). EBDI: an architecture for emotional agents. Paper presented at the 6th International Joint Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems.Google Scholar
Jones, H., Saunier, J., & Lourdeaux, D. (2009). Personality, emotions and physiology in a BDI agent architecture: the PEP→BDI model. In Proceedings of the 2009 IEEE/WIC/ACM International Joint Conference on Web Intelligence and Intelligent Agent Technology.Google Scholar
Junge, M., & Reisenzein, R. (2013). Indirect scaling methods for testing quantitative emotion theories. Cognition and Emotion, 27 (7), 12471275.Google Scholar
Kaptein, F., Broekens, J., Hindriks, K. V., & Neerincx, M. (2016). CAAF: a cognitive affective agent programming framework. Paper presented at IVA 2016.Google Scholar
Kleinginna, P. R., & Kleinginna, A. M. (1981). A categorized list of emotion definitions with suggestions for a consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 345379.Google Scholar
Klug, M., & Zell, A. (2013). Emotion-based human-robot-interaction. Paper presented at the IEEE 9th International Conference on Computational Cybernetics (ICCC), Tihany, Hungary.Google Scholar
Kragel, P. A., & LaBar, K. S. (2016). Decoding the nature of emotion in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 20 (6), 444455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.011Google Scholar
Kramer, N., Kopp, S., Becker-Asano, C., & Sommer, N. (2013). Smile and the world will smile with you – the effects of a virtual agent’s smile on users’ evaluation and behavior. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 71 (3), 335349.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. American Psychologist, 39 (2), 124129.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Cognitive-emotional interactions: listen to the brain. In Lane, R. D. & Nadel, L. (Eds.), Cognitive Neuroscience of Emotion. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P., & Kassam, K. S. (2015). Emotion and Decision Making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 799823 https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115043Google Scholar
Lerner, J. S., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Portrait of the angry decision maker: how appraisal tendencies shape anger’s influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 115137.Google Scholar
Leventhal, H., & Scherer, K. R. (1987). The relationship of emotion to cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 328.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. D. (2005). Bridging emotion theory and neurobiology through dynamic systems modeling. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28 (2), 194245. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x0500004xGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M., & Canamero, L. (2016). Hedonic quality or reward? A study of basic pleasure in homeostasis and decision making of a motivated autonomous robot. Adaptive Behavior, 24, 267291. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059712316666331Google Scholar
Lewis, M., & Canamero, L. (2014). An affective autonomous robot toddler to support the development of self-efficacy in diabetic children. In Proceedings of the 23rd Annual IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (IEEE RO-MAN 2014), Edinburgh, Scotland, UK.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., & Canamero, L. (2017). Robin: an autonomous robot for diabetic children. Paper presented at the UK-RAS Conference on “Robots Working for and Among Us.”Google Scholar
Lewis, M., & Canamero, L. (2019). A robot model of stress-induced compulsive behavior. Paper presented at the 8th ACII, Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (2008). Handbook of Emotions (3rd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Feldman-Barrett, L. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 35 (3), 121143. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11000446Google Scholar
Lisetti, C., & Gmytrasiewicz, P. (2002). Can rational agents afford to be affectless? Applied Artificial Intelligence, 16(7–8), 577609.Google Scholar
Lowe, R., Almer, A., & Balkenius, C. (2019). Bridging connectionism and relational cognition through bi-directional affective-associative processing. Open Information Science, 3, 235260. https://doi.org/10.1515/opis-2019-0017Google Scholar
Lowe, R., & Billing, E. (2017). Affective-associative two-process theory: a neural network investigation of adaptive behaviour in differential outcomes training. Adaptive Behavior, 25 (1), 523.Google Scholar
Lowe, R., Dodig-Crnkovic, G., & Almer, A. (2017). Predictive regulation in affective and adaptive behaviour: an allostatic-cybernetics perspective. In Advanced Research on Biologically Inspired Cognitive Architectures (pp. 149176). Clifton, VA: IGI Global.Google Scholar
Lowe, R., & Kyriazov, K. (2014). Utilizing emotions in autonomous robots: an enactive approach. In Bosse, T., Broekens, J., Dias, J., & van der Zwaan, J. (Eds.), Emotion Modeling: Towards Pragmatic Computational Models of Affective Processes (Vol. 8750, pp. 7698). Cham: Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Loyall, A. B. (1997). Believable agents: building interactive personalities. Ph.D. Thesis, CMU, Pittsburgh.Google Scholar
Luxton, D. D., & Hudlicka, E. (2021). Intelligent virtual agents in healthcare: ethics and application considerations. In Jotterand, F. I., Ienca, M., & Liang, M. (Eds.), Ethics of Artificial Intelligence in Brain and Mental Health. Cham: Springer Nature.Google Scholar
Macedo, L., Cardoso, A., Reisenzein, R., Lorini, E., & Castelfranchi, C. (2009). Artificial surprise. In Vallverdú, J. & Casacuberta, D. (Eds.), Handbook of Research on Synthetic Emotions and Sociable Robotics: New Applications in Affective Computing and Artificial Intelligence. Hershey, PA: IGI Global.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (2012). Cognitive bias modification approaches to anxiety. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 189217.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1984). Mind and Body: The Psychology of Emotion and Stress. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Marinier, R., & Laird, J. (2004). Toward a comprehensive computational model of emotions and feelings. In Proceedings of International Conference on Cognitive Modeling, Pittsburgh, PA.Google Scholar
Marinier, R. P., & Laird, J. E. (2006). A cognitive architecture theory of comprehension and appraisal. Paper presented at ACE 2006, Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Marinier, R. P., Laird, J., & Lewis, R. L. (2009). A computational unification of cognitive behavior and emotion. Cognitive Systems Research, 10 (1), 4869.Google Scholar
Martinez-Miranda, J., Breso, A., & Garcia-Gomez, J. M. (2014). Modelling two emotion regulation strategies as key features of therapeutic empathy. In Bosse, T., Broekens, J., Dias, J., & van der Zwaan, J. (Eds.), Emotion Modeling: Towards Pragmatic Computational Models of Affective Processes (Vol. 8750, pp. 115133). Cham: Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Matthews, G. A., & Harley, T. A. (1993). Effects of extraversion and self-report arousal on semantic priming: a connectionist approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65 (4), 735756.Google Scholar
McCarthy, J. (1995). Making robots conscious of their mental states. Paper presented at the AAAI Spring Symposium, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA.Google Scholar
McQuiggan, S. W., & Lester, J. C. (2007). Modeling and evaluating empathy in embodied companion agents. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 65 (4), 348360. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2006.11.015Google Scholar
Mehrabian, A. (1995). Framework for a comprehensive description and measurement of emotional states. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 121, 339361.Google Scholar
Mellers, B. A., Ho, K., & Ritov, I. (1997). Decision affect theory: emotional reactions to the outcomes of risky options. Psychological Science, 8, 423429. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00455Google Scholar
Meyer, J. J. C. (2006). Reasoning about emotional agents. International Journal of Intelligent Systems, 21 (6), 601619.Google Scholar
Mineka, S., Rafael, E., & Yovel, I. (2003). Cognitive biases in emotional disorders: information processing and social-cognitive perspectives. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of Affective Science. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1986). The Society of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (2006). The Emotion Machine: Commonsense Thinking, Artificial Intelligence, and the Future of the Human Mind. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Mobbs, D., Adolphs, R., Fanselow, M. S., et al. (2019). Viewpoints: approaches to defining and investigating fear. Nature Neuroscience, 22 (8), 12051216. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0456-6Google Scholar
Moutoussis, M., Shahar, N., Hauser, T. U., & Dolan, R. J. (2017). Computation in psychotherapy, or how computational psychiatry can aid learning-based psychological therapies. Computational Psychiatry, 2, 5073. https://doi.org/10.1162/cpsy_a_00014Google Scholar
Neto, A. F. B., & da Silva, F. S. C. (2012). A computer architecture for intelligent agents with personality and emotions. In Zacarias, M. & Oliveira, J. V. (Eds.), Human-Computer Interaction: The Agency Perspective (pp. 263285). Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
Newell, A. (1990). Unified Theories of Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 5158.Google Scholar
Ochs, M., Sadek, D., & Pelachaud, C. (2012). A formal model of emotions for an empathic rational dialog agent. Autonomous Agents and Multi-Agent Systems, 24, 410440. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10458–010-9156-zGoogle Scholar
Ojha, S., Vitale, J., & Williams, M.-A. (2020). Computational emotion models: a thematic review. International Journal of Social Robotics (online). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-020-00713-1Google Scholar
Ortony, A., & Turner, T. J. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions? Psychological Review, 97 (3), 315331.Google Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The Cognitive Structure of Emotions. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ortony, A., Norman, D., & Revelle, W. (2005). Affect and proto-affect in effective functioning. In Fellous, J.-M. & Arbib, M. A. (Eds.), Who Needs Emotions? New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Osuna, E., Rodriguez, L.-F., Gutierrez-Garcia, J. O., & Castro, L. A. (2020). Development of computational models of emotions: a software engineering perspective. Cognitive Systems Research, 60, 119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2019.11.001Google Scholar
Paiva, A., Dias, J., Sobral, D., et al. (2004). Caring for agents and agents that care: building empathic relations with synthetic agents. Paper presented at the International Joint Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems, New York.Google Scholar
Panskepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panskepp, J., & Watt, D. (2011). What is basic about basic emotions? Lasting lessons from affective neuroscience. Emotion Review, 3 (4), 387396. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911410741Google Scholar
Pessoa, L., & McMenamin, B. (2017). Dynamic networks in the emotional brain. Neuroscientist, 23 (4), 383396. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858416671936Google Scholar
Pfeifer, R. (1994). The fungus eater approach to emotion: a view from artificial intelligence. Cognitive Studies, 1, 4257.Google Scholar
Phelps, E. (2006). The interaction of emotion and cognition: the relation between the human amygdala and cognitive awareness. In Hassin, R. R., Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), The New Unconscious (pp. 6076). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Picard, R. (1997). Affective Computing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Plutchik, R. (1984). Emotions: a general psychoevolutionary theory. In Scherer, K. R. & Ekman, P. (Eds.), Approaches to Emotion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Popescu, A., Broekens, J., & Someren, M. v. (2014). GAMYGDALA: an emotion engine for games. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 5 (1), 3244.Google Scholar
Prendinger, H., & Ishizuka, M. (2004). Life-Like Characters: Tools, Affective Functions, and Application. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2004). Gut Reactions: A Perceptual Theory of Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rao, A. (2009). AgentSpeak(L): BDI agents speak out in a logical computable language. Paper presented at the European Workshop on Modelling Autonomous Agents in a Multi-Agent World.Google Scholar
Rao, A. S., & Georgeoff, M. P. (1995). BDI agents: from theory to practice. In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Multi-Agent Systems (ICMAS).Google Scholar
Reilly, W. S. N. (2006). Modeling what happens between emotional antecedents and emotional consequents. In Proceedings of ACE 2006, Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Reilly, W. S. R. (1996). Believable social and emotional agents. Ph.D. Thesis, CMU, Pittsburgh.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (2001). Appraisal processes conceptualized from a schema-theoretic perspective: contributions to a process analysis of emotions. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal Processes in Emotion: Theory, Methods, Research. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (2009). A theory of emotions as metarepresentational states of mind. Cognitive Systems Research, 10, 620. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2008.03.001Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (2012). What is an emotion in the belief-desire theory of emotion? In Paglieri, F., Tummolini, L., Falcone, R., & Miceli, M. (Eds.), The Goals of Cognition: Essays in Honor of Cristiano Castelfranchi. London: College Publications.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (2019). Cognition and emotion: a plea for theory. Cognition and Emotion, 33 (1), 109118. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1568968Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., Hildebrandt, A., & Weber, H. (2020). Personality and emotion. In Corr, P. J. & Matthews, G. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Personality Psychology (2nd ed.) (pp. 8199). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., Hudlicka, E., Dastani, M., et al. (2013). Computational modeling of emotion: toward improving the inter- and intradisciplinary exchange. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 4 (3), 246266.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., & Junge, M. (2006). Uberraschung, Enttauschung und Erleichterung: Emotionsintensitat als Funktion von subjektiver Wahrscheinlichkeit und Erwunschtheit [Surprise, disappointment and relief: emotion intensity as function of subjective probability and desirability]. Paper presented at the 45th Congress of the German Psychological Association, Nuremburg, Germany.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., & Junge, M. (2012). Language and emotion from the perspective of the computational belief-desire theory of emotion. Dynamicity in Emotion Concepts, 27, 3759.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., & Stephan, A. (2014). More on James and the physical basis of emotion. Emotion Review, 6 (1), 3546.Google Scholar
Ritter, F. E., & Avramides, M. N. (2000). Steps Towards Including Behavior Moderators in Human Performance Models in Synthetic Environments. Technical Report No. ACS 2000-1. May 19, 2000. School of information sciences and technology, The Pennsylvania State University.Google Scholar
Ritter, F. E., Reifers, A. L., Klein, L. C., & Schoelles, M. J. (2007). Lessons from defining theories of stress for cognitive architectures. In Gray, W. (Ed.), Advances in Cognitive Models and Cognitive Architectures. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rodriguez, L.-F., & Ramos, F. (2014). Development of computational models of emotions for autonomous agents: a review. Cognitive Computation, 6, 351375. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12559–013-9244-xGoogle Scholar
Roseman, I. J., & Smith, C. A. (2001). Appraisal theory: overview, assumptions, varieties, controversies. In Scherer, A. S. K. R. & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal Processes in Emotion: Theory, Methods, Research. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Russell, J. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110 (1), 145172.Google Scholar
Russell, J., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76 (5), 805819.Google Scholar
Russell, J., & Mehrabian, A. (1977). Evidence for a three-factor theory of emotions. Journal of Research on Personality, 11, 273294.Google Scholar
Samsonovich, A. V. (2020). Socially emotional brain-inspired cognitive architecture framework for artificial intelligence. Cognitive Systems Research, 60, 5776. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2019.12.002Google Scholar
Sanchez-Lopez, Y., & Cerezo, E. (2019). Designing emotional BDI agents: good practices and open questions. The Knowledge Engineering Review, 34 (26), 133.Google Scholar
Sander, D., & Scherer, S. (Eds.). (2009). Oxford Companion to Emotion and the Affective Sciences. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2018). Are LeDoux’s survival circuits basic emotions under a different name? Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 24, 7582. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.06.001Google Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2021). Handbook of Emotion Theory. Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A., & de Sousa, R. (2018). Emotion. In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2018/entries/emotion/ [last accessed July 25, 2022].Google Scholar
Scarantino, A., & Griffiths, P. (2011). Don’t give up on basic emotions. Emotion Review, 3 (4), 444454. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911410745Google Scholar
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379399.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. (2009). Emotions are emergent processes: they require a dynamic computational architecture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 34593474. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0141Google Scholar
Scherer, K. (2012). Neuroscience findings are consistent with appraisal theories of emotion; but does the brain “respect” constructionism? Behavioral Brain Sciences, 35 (3), 163164. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11001750Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: a component process approach. In Scherer, K. R. & Ekman, P. (Eds.), Approaches to Emotion (pp. 293318). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2001a). Appraisal considered as a process of multilevel sequential checking. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal Processes in Emotion: Theory, Methods, Research. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2001b). The nature and study of appraisal: a review of the issues. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal Processes in Emotion: Theory, Methods, Research. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2005). Unconscious process in emotion: the bulk of the iceberg. In Barrett, L. F., Niedenthal, P. M., & Winkielman, P. (Eds.), Emotion and Consciousness. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., & Ellgring, H. (2007). Are facial expressions of emotion produced by categorical affect programs or dynamically driven by appraisal? Emotion, 7 (1), 113130.Google Scholar
Scherer, K., & Moors, A. (2019). The emotion process: event appraisal and component differentiation. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 719745. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev- psych-122216-011854Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (2001). Appraisal Processes in Emotion: Theory, Methods, Research. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scheutz, M., & Sloman, A. (2001). Affect and agent control: experiments with simple affective states. In Proceedings of IAT-01.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1988). How do I feel about it? The information function of affective states. In Fiedler, K. & Forgas, J. P. (Eds.), Affect, Cognition, and Social Behavior (pp. 4462). Toronto: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2003). Mood as information: 20 years later. Psychological Inquiry, 14 (34), 296303.Google Scholar
Sloman, A. (2001). Beyond shallow models of emotion. Cognitive Processing, 2 (1), 177198.Google Scholar
Sloman, A. (2004). What are emotion theories about? In AAAI Spring Symposium: Architectures for Modeling Emotion. Stanford University, CA: AAAI Press.Google Scholar
Sloman, A., Chrisley, R., & Scheutz, M. (2005). The architectural basis of affective states and processes. In Fellous, J.-M. & Arbib, M. A. (Eds.), Who Needs Emotions? New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sloman, A., & Croucher, M. (1981). Why robots will have emotions? Paper presented at the 7th International Conference on Artificial Intelligence (IJCAI).Google Scholar
Sloman, A., & Logan, B. (1999). Building cognitively rich agents using the Sim_agent toolkit. Communications of the Association for Computing Machinery, 43 (2), 7177.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Kirby, L. (2000). Consequences require antecedents: toward a process model of emotion elicitation. In Forgas, J. P. (Ed.), Feeling and Thinking: The Role of Affect in Social Cognition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2001). Toward delivering on the promise of appraisal theory. In Scherer, A. S. K. R. & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal Processes in Emotion. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, R., Lane, R. D., Nadel, L., & Moutoussis, M. (2020). A computational neuroscience perspective on the change process in psychotherapy. In Lane, R. D. & Nadel, L. (Eds.), Neuroscience of Enduring Change. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, R., Parr, T., & Friston, K. J. (2019). Simulating emotions: an active inference model of emotional state inference and emotion concept learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 10 , 2844. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02844Google Scholar
Staller, A., & Petta, P. (1998). Towards a tractable appraisal-based architecture for situated cognizers. Paper presented at the Grounding Emotions in Adaptive Systems Workshop, at the 5th International Conference of the Society for Adaptive Behaviour (SAB’98). Zurich, Switzerland.Google Scholar
Steunebrink, B. R., Dastani, M., & Meyer, J.-J. C. (2009). The OCC model revisited. Paper presented at the 4th Workshop on Emotion and Computing: Current Research and Future Impact, Paderborn, Germany.Google Scholar
Steunebrink, B. R., Dastani, M., & Meyer, J. J. C. (2012). A formal model of emotion triggers: an approach for BDI Agents. Synthese, 185 (1), 83129.Google Scholar
Sun, R., Wilson, N., & Lynch, M. (2016). Emotion: a unified mechanistic interpretation from a cognitive architecture. Cognitive Computation, 8 (8), 114.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S., & McCarter, R. (1964). What and where are the primary affects? Some evidence for a theory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 18, 119158.Google Scholar
Trappl, R., Petta, P., & Payr, S. (2003). Emotions in Humans and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tsai, J., Bowring, E., Marsella, S., & Tambe, M. (2013). Empirical evaluation of computational fear contagion models in crowd dispersions. Autonomous Agents and Multi-Agent Systems, 27, 200217. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10458-013-9220-6Google Scholar
Turner, T. J., & Ortony, A. (1992). Basic emotions: can conflicting criteria converge? Psychological Review, 99 (3), 566571.Google Scholar
Turrini, P., Meyer, J.-J. C., & Castelfranchi, C. (2007). Rational agents that blush. In Paiva, A., Prada, R., & Picard, R. (Eds.), Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Velasquez, J. D. (1997). Modeling emotions and other motivations in synthetic agents. In Proceedings of AAAI-97 (pp. 10–15).Google Scholar
Velásquez, J. D. (1999). An emotion-based approach to robotics. In Proceedings of IROS.Google Scholar
Vernon, D., Lowe, R., Thill, S., & Ziemke, T. (2015). Embodied cognition and circular causality: on the role of constitutive autonomy in the reciprocal coupling of perception and action. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01660Google Scholar
Wright, I., Sloman, A., & Beaudoin, L. (1995). Towards a design-based analysis of emotional episodes. Philosophy, Psychiatry & Psychology, 3 (2), 101126.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39 (2), 117123.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×