Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x24gv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-10T00:43:40.729Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part I - Understanding Belief

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 November 2022

Julien Musolino
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Joseph Sommer
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Pernille Hemmer
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Cognitive Science of Belief
A Multidisciplinary Approach
, pp. 29 - 230
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Bratman, Michael (1999) Faces of intention. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, Daniel (1987) The intentional stance. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, Gilles (1994) Mental spaces. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A. (1975) The language of thought. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A. (1978) Propositional attitudes. The Monist 61, 501523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frege, Gottlob (1892/1952) On sense and reference. In Geach, P. and Black, M., (Eds.). Translations from the philosophical writings of Gottlob Frege (pp. 5578). Blackwell.Google Scholar
Grano, Thomas (2021) Attitude reports. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grano, Thomas (2022) Intention reports and eventuality abstraction in a theory of mood choice. Under review.Google Scholar
Heim, Irene & Kratzer, Angelika (1998) Semantics in generative grammar. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray (1975) On belief-contexts, Linguistic Inquiry, 6(1), 5393.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray (1980) Belief-Contexts Revisited, Linguistic Inquiry, 11(2), 395413.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray (1983) Semantics and cognition. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray (1985) Believing and intending: two sides of the same coin, Linguistic Inquiry, 16(3), 445459.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray (1990)Semantic structures. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray (2002) Foundations of language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray (2007) Language, consciousness, culture. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray (2010) The natural logic of morals and of laws, Brooklyn Law Review 75 (2), 383407.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray (2012) A User’s guide to thought and meaning. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George (1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, Michael (2019) Propositional attitude reports. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2019 Ed.), Zalta, Edward N. (Ed.), https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2019/entries/prop-attitude-reports/Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven (2007) The stuff of thought. Penguin Group.Google Scholar
Pustejovsky, James (1995) The generative lexicon. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Quine, W. v. O. ( 1956) Quantifiers and propositional attitudes. Journal of Philosophy, 53, 177187.Google Scholar
Russell, Bertrand (1905) On denoting. Mind, 14, 479493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwitzgebel, Eric (2019) “Belief”, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2019 Ed.), Zalta, Edward N. (ed.), https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2019/entries/belief/Google Scholar
Searle, John (1983) Intentionality. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stalnaker, Robert (1984) Inquiry. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stich, Stephen (1983) From folk psychology to cognitive science: the case against belief. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vendler, Zeno (1972) Res cogitans. Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, Anna (1987) English speech act verbs: a semantic dictionary. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, Ludwig (1953) Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar

References

Allport, F. H. & Lepkin, M. (1945) Wartime rumors of waste and special privilege: why some people believe them. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 40, 336.Google Scholar
Alter, A. L. & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2009) Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(3), 219235. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309341564Google Scholar
Anderson, C. A. (1983) Abstract and concrete data in the perseverance of social theories: when weak data lead to unshakeable beliefs. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19(2), 93108. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(83)90031-8Google Scholar
Andrews, K. (2012) Do apes read minds? Toward a new folk psychology. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Andrews, K. (2017) Do chimpanzees reason about belief? In Andrews, K., & Beck, J. (Eds.). The Routledge handbook of philosophy of animal minds (pp. 258268). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315742250-25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asp, E. & Tranel, D. (2013) False tagging theory. In Stuss, D. T., & Knight, R. T. (Eds.). Principles of frontal lobe function, 2nd ed. (pp. 383416). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195134971.001.0001Google Scholar
Astuti, R. & Harris, P. (2008) Understanding mortality and the life of the ancestors in rural Madagascar. Cognitive Science: a Multidisciplinary Journal, 32(4), 713740. https://doi.org/10.1080/03640210802066907Google Scholar
Atran, S. (2002) In gods we trust. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bail, C. A., Argyle, L. P., Brown, T. W. et al. (2018) Exposure to opposing views on social media can increase political polarization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(37), 92169221.Google Scholar
Baldassano, C., Chen, J., Zadbood, A., Pillow, J. W., Hasson, U., & Norman, K. A. (2017) Discovering event structure in continuous narrative perception and memory. Neuron, 95(3), 709721.Google Scholar
Banas, J. A. & Rains, S. A. (2010) A meta-analysis of research on inoculation theory. Communication Monographs, 77(3), 281311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1997) Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 3746. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8Google Scholar
Beecher, Henry K. (1955) The powerful placebo. Journal of the American Medical Association 159(7), 16021606. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.1955.02960340022006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bendaña, J. & Mandelbaum, E. (2021) The fragmentation of belief. In Kindermann, Dirk, Borgoni, Cristina, & Onofri, Andrea (Eds.). The fragmentation of mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bermúdez, J. L. (2008) Thinking without words. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bingel, U., Wanigasekera, V., Wiech, K. et al. (2011) The effect of treatment expectation on drug efficacy: imaging the analgesic benefit of the opioid remifentanil. Science translational medicine, 3(70), 714. https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.3001244Google Scholar
Block, N. (1980) Troubles with functionalism. Readings in Philosophy of Psychology, 1, 268305.Google Scholar
Bouton, M. E. (2004) Context and behavioral processes in extinctionLearning & Memory11(5), 485494. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.78804CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouton, M. E, & Bolles, R.C. (1979) Role of conditioned contextual stimuli in reinstatement of extinguished fear. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5(4), 368378. https://doi.org/10.1037//0097-7403.5.4.368Google Scholar
Bouton, M. E. & King, D. A. (1983) Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear: tests for the associative value of the context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9(3), 248265.Google Scholar
Bouton, M. E. & Ricker, S. T. (1994) Renewal of extinguished responding in a second context. Animal Learning & Behavior, 22, 317324.Google Scholar
Bouton, M. E., Westbrook, R. F., Corcoran, K. A., & Maren, S. (2006) Contextual and temporal modulation of extinction: behavioral and biological mechanisms. Biological Psychiatry, 60(4), 352360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brock, T. C. & Balloun, J. L. (1967) Behavioral receptivity to dissonant information. Journal of personality and social psychology, 6(4), 413428.Google Scholar
Brooks, D. C. & Bouton, M. E. (1993) A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates spontaneous recovery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 19(1), 7789.Google Scholar
Brownstein, M. (2015) Implicit Bias. In Zalta, E (Ed.). The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy, Spring 2015.Google Scholar
Brownstein, M. (2018) The implicit mind: cognitive architecture, the self, and ethics. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190633721.001.0001Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Buttelmann, F., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017) Great apes distinguish true from false beliefs in an interactive helping task. PLoS ONE, 12(4), 113, e0173793. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0173793Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009) Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition. 112(2), 337342 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006.Google Scholar
Brashier, N. M. & Marsh, E. J. (2020) Judging truth. Annual Review of Psychology, 71(1), 499515. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-050807CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheke, L. G. & Clayton, N. S. (2010) Mental time travel in animals. WIREs Cognitive Science, 1(6), 915930. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.59Google Scholar
Cheyette, S. & Piantadosi, S. (2017) Knowledge transfer in a probabilistic language of thought. In CogSci.Google Scholar
Cialdini, R. B., Trost, M. R., & Newsom, J. T. (1995) Preference for consistency: the development of a valid measure and the discovery of surprising behavioral implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(2), 318328. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.2.318Google Scholar
Clatterbuck, H. (2016) Darwin, Hume, Morgan, and the verae causae of psychology. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 60, 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.shpsc.2016.09.002Google Scholar
Clements, W. A. & Perner, J. (1994) Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development, 9(4), 377395. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4Google Scholar
Colloca, L. & Benedetti, F. (2007) Nocebo hyperalgesia: how anxiety is turned into pain. Current Opinions in Anaesthesiology, 20(5), 435439. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACO.0b013e3282b972fbGoogle Scholar
Compton, J. (2020) Prophylactic versus therapeutic inoculation treatments for resistance to influence. Communication Theory, 30(3), 330343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cone, J., Flaharty, K., & Ferguson, M. J. (2021) The long-term effects of new evidence on implicit impressions of other people. Psychological Science, 32(2), 173188. doi:10.1177/0956797620963559Google Scholar
Cone, J., Mann, T. C., & Ferguson, M. J. (2017). Changing our implicit minds: how, when, and why implicit evaluations can be rapidly revised. In Olson, J. M., (Ed.). Advances in experimental social psychology: vol. 56 (pp. 131199). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (1982) Rational animals. Dialectica, 36(4), 317328. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1746-8361.1982.tb01546.xGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, J., Van Dessel, P., & Moran, T. (2020) Attitudes beyond associations: On the role of propositional representations in stimulus evaluation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 61, 127183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1987) The intentional stance. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1991) Real patterns. The Journal of Philosophy, 88(1), 2751. https://doi.org/10.2307/2027085Google Scholar
DuBrow, S., Rouhani, N., Niv, Y., & Norman, K. A. (2017) Does mental context drift or shift?. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 17, 141146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ecker, U. K., Lewandowsky, S., & Apai, J. (2011). Terrorists brought down the plane! – no, actually it was a technical fault: Processing corrections of emotive information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(2), 283310.Google Scholar
Ecker, U. K., Lewandowsky, S., & Tang, D. T. (2010) Explicit warnings reduce but do not eliminate the continued influence of misinformation. Memory & Cognition, 38(8), 10871100.Google Scholar
Egan, A. (2008) Seeing and believing: perception, belief formation and the divided mind. Philosophical Studies, 140(1), 4763. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098–008-9225-1Google Scholar
Elga, A. & Rayo, A. (2021) Fragmentation and Information Access. In Kindermann, Dirk, Borgoni, Cristina, & Onofri, Andrea (Eds.). The fragmentation of mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fazio, L., Rand, D., & Pennycook, G. (2019) Repetition increases perceived truth equally for plausible and implausible statements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 17051710. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423–019-01651-4Google Scholar
Fazio, L. K., Brashier, N. M., Payne, B. K., & Marsh, E. J. (2015) Knowledge does not protect against illusory truth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(5), 9931002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000098.suppGoogle Scholar
Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956). When prophecy fails. University of Minnesota Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10030-000Google Scholar
Festinger, L., Riecken, H., & Schachter, S. (2017) When prophecy fails: a social and psychological study of a modern group that predicted the destruction of the world. Lulu Press, Inc.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1978) Propositional attitudes. The Monist, 61(4), 501523. https://doi.org/10.5840/monist197861444Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1987) Psychosemantics: the problem of meaning in the philosophy of mind. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gendler, T. (2008) Alief and belief. The Journal of Philosophy, 105(10), 634663. https://doi.org/10.5840/jphil20081051025CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, D. T. (1991) How mental systems believe. American Psychologist, 46(2), 107119. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.46.2.107Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Krull, D., & Malone, M. (1990). Unbelieving the unbelievable: some problems in the rejection of false information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(4), 601613. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.4.601Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998) Immune neglect: a source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 617638. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.617Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. & Nosek, B. (2009) Attitudinal dissociation: what does it mean? In Attitudes: insights from the new implicit measures, Petty, R., Fazio, R., & Brinol, P. (Eds.). (pp. 6582). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, N. D., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Gerstenberg, T. (2014) Concepts in a probabilistic language of thought. In The conceptual mind: new directions in the study of concepts. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, N. D., Ullman, T. D., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2011). Learning a theory of causality. Psychological Review, 118(1), 110199.Google Scholar
Gray, H. M., Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2007) Dimensions of mind perception. Science, 315(5812), 619619. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1134475Google Scholar
Gu, X., Lohrenz, T., Salas, R. et al. (2015) Belief about nicotine selectively modulates value and reward prediction error signals in smokers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(8): 25392544. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1416639112Google Scholar
Gundersen, H., Specht, K., Grüner, R., Ersland, L., & Hugdahl, K. (2008) Separating the effects of alcohol and expectancy on brain activation: an fMRI working memory study. Neuroimage, 42(4), 15871596. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.037Google Scholar
Hampton, J. A. (1995) Testing the prototype theory of concepts. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(5), 686708. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1031Google Scholar
Hasher, L., Goldstein, D., & Toppino, T. (1977) Frequency and the conference of referential validity. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behaviour, 16(1), 107112. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022–5371(77)80012-1.Google Scholar
Hawthorne, J., Rothschild, D., & Spectre, L. (2016) Belief is weak. Philosophical Studies, 173(5), 13931404. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098–015-0553-7Google Scholar
van Holst, R. J., Clark, L., Veltman, D. J., van den Brink, W., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2014) Enhanced striatal responses during expectancy coding in alcohol dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 142, 204208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.06.019Google Scholar
Howard, S., Avarguès-Weber, A., Garcia, J., Greentree, A.D., & Dyer, A. D. (2019) Numerical ordering of zero in honeybees. Science, 360(6393), 11241126. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aar4975Google Scholar
Hughes, S., Ye, Y., Van Dessel, P., & De Houwer, J. (2019) When people co-occur with good or bad events: graded effects of relational qualifiers on evaluative conditioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(2), 196208. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218781340Google Scholar
Icard, T. (2016) Subjective probability as sampling propensity. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 7(4), 863903.Google Scholar
Jacobs, K. W. & Nordan, F. M. (1979) Classification of placebo drugs: effect of color. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49(2), 367372. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1979.49.2.367Google Scholar
Jensen, G., Alkan, Y., Ferrera, V. P., & Terrace, H. S. (2018) Reward associations do not explain transitive inference performance in monkeys. Science Advances, 5(7), Article aaw2089. https://doi.org:10.7287/peerj.preprints.26889v1Google Scholar
Katz, Y., Goodman, N.D., Kersting, K., Kemp, C., & Tenenbaum, J.B. (2008). Modeling semantic cognition as logical dimensionality reduction. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 30.Google Scholar
Kemp, C., Goodman, N., & Tenenbaum, J. (2008). Learning and using relational theories. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 20, 753760.Google Scholar
Knowles, E. S. & Condon, C. A. (1999) Why people say “yes”: a dual-process theory of acquiescenceJournal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2), 379386https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.2.379Google Scholar
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016) Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354(6308), 110114. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf8110Google Scholar
Kufahl, P., Li, Z., Risinger, R. et al. (2008) Expectation modulates human brain responses to acute cocaine: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry, 63(2), 222230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.021Google Scholar
Kumkale, G. T. & Albarracín, D. (2004) The sleeper effect in persuasion: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 130(1), 143172. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.1.143Google Scholar
Kurdi, B. & Banaji, M. R. (2017) Repeated evaluative pairings and evaluative statements: how effectively do they shift implicit attitudes? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General146(2), 194213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000239Google Scholar
Kurdi, B. & Banaji, M. R. (2019) Attitude change via repeated evaluative pairings versus evaluative statements: shared and unique features. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 116(5), 681703. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000151Google Scholar
Kurdi, B. & Dunham, Y. (2020) Propositional accounts of implicit evaluation: taking stock and looking ahead. Social Cognition, 38(Supplement), s42s67. http://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2020.38.supp.s42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurdi, B. & Dunham, Y. (2021) Sensitivity of implicit evaluations to accurate and erroneous propositional inferences. Cognition, 214, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104792.Google Scholar
Lai, C. K., Skinner, A. L., Cooley, E. et al. (2016) Reducing implicit racial preferences: II. Intervention effectiveness across time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(8), 10011016. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/xge0000179Google Scholar
Lacassagne, D., Béna, J., & Corneille, O. (2022) Is Earth a perfect square? Repetition increases the perceived truth of highly implausible statements. Cognition, 223, 105052. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105052Google Scholar
Lange, M. (2002) Who’s afraid of Ceteris-Paribus laws? Or: how i Learned to stop worrying and love them. Erkenntnis 57, 407423. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021546731582Google Scholar
Lewis, D. (1980) Mad pain and Martian pain. In Block, Ned (Ed.). Readings in the philosophy of psychology (pp. 216 S.222). Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Liang, P., Jordan, M. I., & Klein, D. (2010) Learning programs: a hierarchical Bayesian approach. In Proceedings of the 27th International Conference on Machine Learning (ICML-10) (pp. 639–646).Google Scholar
Lurz, R. W. (2015) Mindreading animals: the debate over what animals know about other minds. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262016056.001.0001Google Scholar
Mandelbaum, E. (2013) Against alief. Philosophical Studies, 165(1), 197211. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098–012-9930-7Google Scholar
Mandelbaum, E. (2014) Thinking is believing. Inquiry, 57(1), 5596. https://doi.org/10.1080/0020174X.2014.858417Google Scholar
Mandelbaum, E. (2015a) Associationist theories of thought. In Zalta, Edward N. (Ed.). The stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Summer 2017 Edition. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2017/entries/associationist-thought/Google Scholar
Mandelbaum, E. (2015b) The automatic and the ballistic: modularity beyond perceptual processes. Philosophical Psychology, 28(8), 11471156. www.doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2014.950217Google Scholar
Mandelbaum, E. (2016) Attitude, inference, association: on the propositional structure of implicit bias. Noûs, 50(3), 629658. https://doi.org/10.1111/nous.12089Google Scholar
Mandelbaum, E. (2018) Seeing and conceptualizing: modularity and the shallow contents of perception. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 97(2), 267283. https://doi.org/10.1111/phpr.12368Google Scholar
Mandelbaum, E. (2019) Troubles with Bayesianism: an introduction to the psychological immune system. Mind & Language, 34(2), 141157. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12205Google Scholar
Mandelbaum, E. (Forthcoming) A psychofunctional theory of belief. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mandelbaum, E. & Quilty-Dunn, J. (2015). Believing without reason or: why liberals shouldn’t watch Fox News. The Harvard Review of Philosophy, 22, 4252. https://doi.org/10.5840/harvardreview2015226Google Scholar
Mann, T. C. & Ferguson, M. J. (2015) Can we undo our first impressions? The role of reinterpretation in reversing implicit evaluationsJournal of Personality and Social Psychology108(6), 823849. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000021Google Scholar
Mann, T. C. & Ferguson, M. J. (2017). Reversing implicit first impressions through reinterpretation after a two-day delayJournal of Experimental Social Psychology68, 122127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.06.004Google Scholar
Mann, T. C., Kurdi, B., & Banaji, M. R. (2019) How effectively can implicit evaluations be updated? Using evaluative statements after aversive repeated evaluative pairings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Advance Online Publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McHoskey, J. W. (1995) Case closed? On the John F. Kennedy assassination: biased assimilation of evidence and attitude polarization. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17(3), 395409. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp1703_7Google Scholar
McGuire, W. J. (1964). Inducing resistance to persuasion: some contemporary approaches. In Berkowitz, L (Ed.). Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 191229). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mercier, H. (2020) Not born yesterday: the science of who we trust and what we believe. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Mollica, F. & Piantadosi, S. (2015). Towards semantically rich and recursive word learning models. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Conference, 37, 16071612.Google Scholar
Nichols, S. (2021) Rational rules: towards a theory of moral learning. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nielsen, M. & Stewart, R. T. (2020) Persistent disagreement and polarization in a Bayesian setting. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 72 (1), 5178.Google Scholar
Núñez, R., Allen, M., Gao, R., Rigoli, C. M., Relaford-Doyle, J., & Semenuks, A. (2019) What happened to cognitive science?. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(8), 782791. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562–019-0626-2Google Scholar
Onishi, K. H. & Baillargeon, R. (2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science308(5719), 255258. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1107621Google Scholar
van Orman Quine, W. & Ullian, J. (1978) The web of belief. Random House.Google Scholar
Ossipov, M. H., Dussor, G. O., & Porreca, F. (2010) Central modulation of pain. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 120(11), 37793787. https://doi.org/10.1172/JCI43766Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cannon, Tyrone D. & Rand, , David, G. (2018) Prior exposure increases perceived accuracy of fake news. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(12), 18651880. www.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000465Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2015) On the reception and detection of pseudo-profound bullshitJudgment and Decision Making, 10(6), 549563.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Epstein, Z., Mosleh, M., Arechar, A. A., Eckles, D., & Rand, D. G. (2021) Shifting attention to accuracy can reduce misinformation online. Nature, 592, 16.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. (2019) Lazy, not biased: susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition, 188, 3950. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.011Google Scholar
Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. (2021) The Psychology of Fake News. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(5), 388402.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M., Gray, S. L., Mody, S., Cornero, F. M., & Carey, S. (2019) Logical reasoning by a grey parrot? A case study of the disjunctive syllogism. Behaviour, 156(5–8), 409445. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-00003528Google Scholar
Petrocelli, J. V., Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2007) Unpacking attitude certainty: attitude clarity and attitude correctness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(1), 3041. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.30Google Scholar
Phillips, J., Buckwalter, W., Cushman, F. et al. (2020) Knowledge before belief. Behavioral and Brain Science, 8(44), E140. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X20000618Google Scholar
Piantadosi, S. T., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Goodman, N. D. (2012) Bootstrapping in a language of thought: a formal model of numerical concept learning. Cognition 123(2), 199217.Google Scholar
Piantadosi, S. T., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Goodman, N. D. (2016) The logical primitives of thought: Empirical foundations for compositional cognitive modelsPsychological Review, 123(4), 392424https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039980Google Scholar
Porot, N. J. (2019) Some non-human languages of thought. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). City University of New York, Graduate Center.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. & Vonk, J. (2003) Chimpanzee minds: suspiciously human? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(4), 157160. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00053-6Google Scholar
Quilty-Dunn, J. & Mandelbaum, E. (2018a) Against dispositionalism: belief in cognitive science. Philosophical Studies, 175(9), 23532372. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098–017-0962-xGoogle Scholar
Quilty-Dunn, J. & Mandelbaum, E. (2018b) Inferential transitions. Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 96(3), 532547. https://doi.org/10.1080/00048402.2017.1358754Google Scholar
Rescorla, M. (2016) Bayesian sensorimotor psychology. Mind & Language, 31(1), 336.Google Scholar
Ritchie, K. (2016) Can semantics guide ontology? Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 94(1), 2441. https://doi.org/10.1080/00048402.2015.1045912Google Scholar
Rosati, A. G. & Santos, L. R. (2016) Spontaneous metacognition in rhesus monkeys. Psychological Science, 27(9), 11811191. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616653737Google Scholar
Ross, L. (1977) The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: distortions in the attribution process. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 173220. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065–2601(08)60357-3Google Scholar
Schachter, S. & Singer, J. E. (1962) Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69(5), 379399. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046234Google Scholar
Schwitzgebel, E. (2013) A dispositional approach to attitudes: thinking outside of the belief box. In Nottelmann, N. (Ed.). New essays on belief (pp. 7599). Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137026521_5Google Scholar
Seifert, C. (2002) The continued influence of misinformation in memory: what makes a correction effective? In Ross, B. (Ed.). Psychology of Learning and Motivation, vol. 41 (pp. 265292). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Singer, D. J., Bramson, A., Grim, P. et al. (2019) Rational social and political polarization. Philosophical Studies, 176(9), 22432267.Google Scholar
Skurnik, I., Yoon, C., Park, D. C., & Schwarz, N. (2005) How warnings about false claims become recommendations. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(4), 713724.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Couchman, J. J., & Beran, M. J. (2014). The highs and lows of theoretical interpretation in animal-metacognition research. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Metacognition, 367(1594), 12971309. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-45190-4_5Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1997) Intuitive and reflective beliefs. Mind and Language, 12(1), 6783.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., Clément, F., Heintz, C. et al. (2010) Epistemic vigilance. Mind and Language, 25 (4), 359393.Google Scholar
Stich, S. P. (1979) Do animals have beliefs? Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 57(1), 1528. https://doi.org/10.1080/00048407912341011Google Scholar
Storms, M. D. & Nisbett, R. E. (1970). Insomnia and the attribution process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16(2), 319328. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0029835Google Scholar
Tibbetts, E., Agudelo, J., Pandit, S., & Riojas, J. (2019) Transitive inference in Polistes paper wasps. Biology Letters, 15(5), Article 20190015. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2019.0015Google Scholar
Tracey, I. (2010) Getting the pain you expect: mechanisms of placebo, nocebo and reappraisal effects in humans. Nature Medicine, 16(11), 12771283. https://doi.org/10.1038/nm.2229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tracy, R. Young, S. Porot, N., & Mandelbaum, E. (under review) Disfluency Attenuates the Reception of Pseudoprofound and Postmodernist Bullshit.Google Scholar
Trouche, E., Johansson, P., Hall, L., & Mercier, H. (2016) The selective laziness of reasoning. Cognitive Science 40(8), 21222136.Google Scholar
Unkelbach, C. & Rom, S. C. (2017) A referential theory of the repetition-induced truth effect. Cognition, 160, 110126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.12.016Google Scholar
Van Dessel, P., De Houwer, J., Gast, A., Smith, C. T., & De Schryver, M. (2016) Instructing implicit processes: when instructions to approach or avoid influence implicit but not explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 63, 19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.11.002Google Scholar
Van Dessel, P., Ye, Y., & De Houwer, J. (2019) Changing deep-rooted implicit evaluation in the blink of an eye: negative verbal information shifts automatic liking of Gandhi. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10(2), 266273. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617752064Google Scholar
Van Leeuwen, N. (2014) Religious credence is not factual belief. Cognition, 133(3), 698715. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.015Google Scholar
Van Leeuwen, N. (2017) Two paradigms for religious representation: the physicist and the playground (a reply to Levy). Cognition, 164, 206211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.03.021Google Scholar
Vogel, T., Silva, R. R., Thomas, A., & Wänke, M. (2020) Truth is in the mind, but beauty is in the eye: fluency effects are moderated by a match between fluency source and judgment dimension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(8), 15871596. doi:10.1037/xge0000731Google Scholar
Vul, E., Goodman, N., Griffiths, T. L., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2014). One and done? Optimal decisions from very few samples. Cognitive Science, 38(4), 599637. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12101Google Scholar
Vul, E. & Pashler, H. (2008) Measuring the crowd within: probabilistic representations within individuals. Psychological Science, 19(7), 645647. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02136.xGoogle Scholar
Waber, R. L., Shiv, B., Carmon, Z., & Ariely, D. (2008) Commercial features of placebo and therapeutic. Journal of the American Medical Association, 299(9), 10161017. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.299.9.1016Google Scholar
Watson, A., Power, A., Brown, C., El-Deredy, W., & Jones, A. (2012) Placebo analgesia: cognitive influences on therapeutic outcome. Arthritis Research & Therapy, 14, Article 206. https://doi.org/10.1186/ar3783.Google Scholar
Weisberg, J. (2020) Belief in psyontology. Philosophers’ Imprint, 20(11), 127.Google Scholar
Webster, D. M. & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994) Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 10491062. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.67.6.1049Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M., Coulton, G. F., & Wenzlaff, R. (1985) The transparency of denial: briefing in the debriefing paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(2), 338346.Google Scholar
Whissel, C., Abramson, C. I., & Barber, K. I. (2013) The search for cognitive terminology: an analysis of comparative psychology journal titles. Behavioral Sciences, 3(1), 133142.Google Scholar
Wiklund, R. A. & Rosenbaum, S. H. (1997) Anesthesiology (part one of two). New England Journal of Medicine, 337(17), 12151219. https://doi.org/10.1056/nejm199710233371707Google Scholar
Zanna, M. & Cooper, J. (1974) Dissonance and the pill: an attribution approach to studying the arousal properties of dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29(5), 703709. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0036651Google Scholar
Zettlemoyer, L. S. & Collins, M. (2005) Learning to map sentences to logical form: Structured classification with probabilistic categorical grammars. Proceedings of the 21st Conference on Uncertainty in Artificial Intelligence, pp. 658–666.Google Scholar

References

Alloy, L. B. & Abramson, L. Y. (1979) Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 108(4) 441485.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th Ed.). APA.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1989) Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44(9), 11741184.Google Scholar
Bortolotti, L. (2009) Delusions and other irrational beliefs. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bortolotti, L. (2014) Irrationality. Polity Press.Google Scholar
Bortolotti, L. (2018) Stranger than fiction: costs and benefits of everyday confabulation. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 9(2) 227249.Google Scholar
Bortolotti, L. (2020) The epistemic innocence of irrational Beliefs. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bortolotti, L. & Antrobus, M. (2014) Costs and benefits of realism and optimism. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 28(2), 194198.Google Scholar
Bortolotti, L., Antrobus, M., & Sullivan-Bissett, E. (2019) The epistemic innocence of optimistically biased beliefs. In Balcerak Jackson, M. and Balcerak Jackson, B. (Eds.). Reasoning: new essays on theoretical and practical thinking. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bortolotti, L., Broome, M., & Mameli, M. (2014) Delusions and responsibility for action: insights from the Breivik case. Neuroethics, 7(3), 377382.Google Scholar
Bortolotti, L., Ichino, A., & Mameli, M. (2021) Conspiracy Theories and Delusions. Reti, Saperi e Linguaggi 2 (VIII), 183200.Google Scholar
Bortolotti, L. & Sullivan-Bissett, E. (2021) Is choice blindness a case of self-ignorance? Synthese, 198(6), 54375454.Google Scholar
Brewster, Z. W. & Rusche, S. N. (2012) Quantitative evidence of the continuing significance of race: tableside racism in full-service restaurants. Journal of Black Studies, 43(4), 359384.Google Scholar
Broome, M. R., Bortolotti, L., & Mameli, M. (2010) Moral responsibility and mental illness: a case study. Cambridge Quarterly of Healthcare Ethics, 19(2), 179187.Google Scholar
Brown, J. K., Waltz, J. A., Straus, G. P., McMahon, R. P., Frank, M. J., & Gold, J. M. (2013) Hypothetical decision making in schizophrenia: the role of expected value computation and “irrational” biases. Psychiatry Research 209(2), 142149.Google Scholar
Craigie, J. & Bortolotti, L. (2014) Rationality, diagnosis, and patient autonomy in psychiatry. In Sadler, John Z., Fulford, K. W. M., & van Staden, Werdie (C. W.) (Eds.). The oxford handbook of psychiatric ethics (pp. 387404) Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1996) The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 351(1346), 14131420.Google Scholar
Ellis, H. & Young, A. (1990) Accounting for delusional misidentifications. British Journal of Psychiatry, 157(2), 239248. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.157.2.239Google Scholar
Fricker, M. (2007) Epistemic injustice: power and the ethics of knowledge. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Garrett, N., Sharot, T., Faulkner, P., et al. (2014) Losing the rose tinted glasses: neural substrates of unbiased belief updating in depression. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8: 639.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. & Todd, P. M. (1999) Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hemmens, C., Miller, M., Burton, V. S. Jr., & Milner, S. (2002) The consequences of official labels: an examination of the rights lost by the mentally ill and mentally incompetent ten years later. Community Mental Health Journal, 38(2), 129140.Google Scholar
Hirstein, W. (2005) Brain fiction: self-deception and the riddle of confabulation. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jurjako, M. & Malatesti, L. (2016) Instrumental rationality in psychopathy: implications from learning tasks, Philosophical Psychology, 29( 5), 717731.Google Scholar
Kemp, R., Chua, S., McKenna, P., & David, A. (1997) Reasoning and delusionsThe British Journal of Psychiatry170(5), 398405.Google Scholar
McKay, R., Langdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (2005) “Sleights of mind”: delusions, defences, and self-deception. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10(4), 305326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellet, E., Houdé, O., Brazo, P., Mazoyer, B., Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., & Dollfus, S. (2006) When a schizophrenic deficit becomes a reasoning advantageSchizophrenia Research84(2–3), 359364.Google Scholar
Mirian, D. R., Heinrichs, W., & McDermid Vaz, S. (2011) Exploring logical reasoning abilities in schizophrenic patientsSchizophrenia Research127(1–3), 178180.Google Scholar
Miyazono, K. & Bortolotti, L. (2021) philosophy of psychology: an introduction. Polity Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. & Ross, L. (1980) Human inference: strategies and shortcomings in social judgement. Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. & Wilson, T. D. (1977) Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231259.Google Scholar
Owen, G. S., Cutting, J., & David, A. S. (2007) Are people with schizophrenia more logical than healthy volunteers? British Journal of Psychiatry191(5), 453454.Google Scholar
Pierre, J. M. (2020) Mistrust and misinformation: a two-component, socio-epistemic model of belief in conspiracy theories. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 8(2), 617641.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. (1995) Anosognosia in parietal lobe syndrome. Conscious Cognition 4(1), 2251.Google Scholar
Revsbech, R., Mortensen, E. L., Owen, G. et al. (2015) Exploring rationality in schizophreniaBritish Journal of Psychiatry Open1(1), 98103.Google Scholar
Ritunnano, R., Humpston, C., & Broome, M. (2021) Finding order within the disorder: a case study exploring the meaningfulness of delusions. BJPsych Bulletin, 46(2), 17. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjb.2020.151Google Scholar
Shepperd, J. A., Klein, W. M. P., Waters, E. A., & Weinstein, N. D. (2013) Taking stock of unrealistic optimism. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(4), 395411.Google Scholar
Stanovich, Keith E. & West, Richard F. (2000) Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5) 645665.Google Scholar
Stein, E. (1996) Without good reason: the rationality debate in philosophy and cognitive science. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sullivan-Bissett, E. (2015) Implicit bias, confabulation, and epistemic innocence. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 548560.Google Scholar
Sweeny, K., Carroll, P. J., & Shepperd, J. A. (2006) Is optimism always best? Future outlooks and preparedness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 302306.Google Scholar
Tiberius, V. (2008) The reflective life: living wisely with our limits. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2004) International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems, tenth revision (ICD-10), 2nd ed. World Health Organization.Google Scholar

References

Alba, J. W. & Hasher, L. (1983) Is memory schematic? Psychological Bulletin, 93(2), 203231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.93.2.203Google Scholar
Allred, S. R., Crawford, L. E., Duffy, S., & Smith, J. (2016) Working memory and spatial judgments: cognitive load increases the central tendency bias. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(6), 18251831. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423–016-1039-0Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1990) The adaptive character of thought. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1991) Is human cognition adaptive? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14(3), 471485.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1993) The rules of the mind. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. & Milson, R. (1989) Human memory: an adaptive perspective. Psychological Review, 96(4), 703719. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.703Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. & Schooler, L. J. (1991) Reflections of the environment in memory. Psychological Sience, 2(6), 396408.Google Scholar
Ausubel, D. P. & Fitzgerald, D. (1962) Organizer, general background, and antecedent learning variables in sequential verbal learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 53(6), 243249.Google Scholar
Bae, G.-Y., Olkkonen, M., Allred, S. R., & Flombaum, J. I. (2015) Why some colors appear more memorable than others: a model combining categories and particulars in color working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(4), 744763. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000076Google Scholar
Bangerter, A. (2000) Transformation between scientific and social representations of conception: the method of serial reproduction. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39(4), 521535.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. & Bartlett, F. C. (1932) Remembering: a study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Berens, S. C., Richards, B. A., & Horner, A. J. (2020) Dissociating memory accessibility and precision in forgetting. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(8), 866877. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0888-8Google Scholar
Brady, T. F. & Alvarez, G. A. (2011) Hierarchical encoding in visual working memory: ensemble statistics bias memory for individual items. Psychological Science, 22(3), 384392.Google Scholar
Brady, T. F., Konkle, T., Gill, J., Oliva, A., & Alvarez, G. A. (2013) Visual long-term memory has the same limit on fidelity as visual working memory. Psychological Science, 24(6), 981990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797612465439Google Scholar
Brady, T. F. & Oliva, A. (2008) Statistical learning using real-world scenes: extracting categorical regularities without conscious intent. Psychogical Science, 19, 678685 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02142.xGoogle Scholar
Brewer, W. F. & Treyens, J. C. (1981) Role of schemata in memory for places. Cognitive Psychology, 13(2), 207230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(81)90008-6Google Scholar
Brown, B. R. & Evans, S. H. (1969) Perceptual learning in pattern discrimination tasks with two and three schema categories. Psychonomic Science, 15(2), 101103. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03336223Google Scholar
de Gardelle, V. & Summerfield, C. (2011) Robust averaging during perceptual judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(32), 1334113346. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1104517108Google Scholar
Djonlagic, I., Rosenfeld, A., Shohamy, D., Myers, C., Gluck, M., & Stickgold, R. (2009) Sleep enhances category learning. Learning & Memory, 16(12), 751755. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.1634509Google Scholar
Duffy, S., Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Crawford, L. E. (2010) Category effects on stimulus estimation: Shifting and skewed frequency distributions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(2), 224230. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.2.224Google Scholar
Durrant, S. J., Cairney, S. A., McDermott, C., & Lewis, P. A. (2015) Schema-conformant memories are preferentially consolidated during REM sleep. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 122, 4150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2015.02.011Google Scholar
Durrant, S. J., Taylor, C., Cairney, S., & Lewis, P. A. (2011) Sleep-dependent consolidation of statistical learning. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 13221331. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.015Google Scholar
Ebbinghaus, H. (1885) Über das gedächtnis: untersuchungen zur experimentellen psychologie. Duncker & Humblot.Google Scholar
Estes, W.K. (1955) Statistical theory of distributed phenomena in learning. Psychological Review, 62(5), 369377.Google Scholar
Eckstein, M. P., Abbey, C. K., Pham, B. T., & Shimozaki, S. S. (2004) Perceptual learning through optimization of attentional weighting: human versus optimal Bayesian learner. Journal of Vision, 4(12), 10061019. https://doi.org/10.1167/4.12.3Google Scholar
Epstein, R. A. (2008) Parahippocampal and retrosplenial contributions to human spatial navigation. Trends in Cognitve Science, 12(10), 388396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.004Google Scholar
Feldman, N. H., Griffiths, T. L., & Morgan, J. L. (2009) The influence of categories on perception: explaining the perceptual magnet effect as optimal statistical inference. Psychological Review, 116(4), 752782. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017196Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1975) Hindsight is not equal to foresight: the effect of outcome knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1(3), 288299. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.1.3.288Google Scholar
Galleguillos, C. & Belongie, S. (2010) Context based object categorization: a critical survey. Computer Vision and Image Understanding, 114(6), 712722. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cviu.2010.02.004Google Scholar
Graesser, A. C. & Nakamura, G. V. (1982) The impact of a schema on comprehension and memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.). The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 16) (pp. 59109). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L. & Kalish, M. L. (2005) A Bayesian view of language evolution by iterated learning. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (Vol. 27, No. 27).Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L., Lieder, F., & Goodman, N. D. (2015) Rational use of cognitive resources: levels of analysis between the computational and the algorithmic. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7(2), 217229.Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L. & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2005) Structure and strength in causal induction. Cognitive Psychology, 51(4), 354384.Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L. & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2006) Optimal predictions in everyday cognition. Psychological Science, 17(9), 767773. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01780.xGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, T. L. & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007) From mere coincidences to meaningful discoveries. Cognition, 103(2), 180226.Google Scholar
Haberman, J. & Whitney, D. (2010) The visual system discounts emotional deviants when extracting average expression. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72(7), 18251838. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.7.1825Google Scholar
Hardt, O. & Pohl, R. (2003) Hindsight bias as a function of anchor distance and anchor plausibility. Memory, 11(4–5), 379394. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210244000504Google Scholar
Hemmer, P. & Steyvers, M. (2009a) Integrating episodic and semantic information in memory for natural scenes. In Taatgen, N. A. and van Rijn, H. (Eds.). Proceedings of the 31tst Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, (pp. 15571562). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Hemmer, P. & Steyvers, M. (2009b) A Bayesian account of reconstructive memory. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(1), 189202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2008.01010.xGoogle Scholar
Hemmer, P. & Persaud, K. (2014) Interaction between categorical knowledge and episodic memory across domains. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00584Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Duncan, S. (1991) Categories and particulars: prototype effects in estimating spatial location. Psychological Review, 98(3), 352376. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.98.3.352Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (2000) Why do categories affect stimulus judgment? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(2), 220241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.220Google Scholar
Kashima, Y. (2000) Maintaining cultural stereotypes in the serial reproduction of narratives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(5), 594604.Google Scholar
Kruschke, J. K. (1996) Base rates in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(1), 326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.3Google Scholar
Körding, K. P. & Wolpert, D. M. (2006). Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(7), 319326.Google Scholar
Lew, T. F. & Vul, E. (2015) Ensemble clustering in visual working memory biases location memories and reduces the Weber noise of relative positions. Journal of Vision, 15(4), 114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/15.4.10Google Scholar
Lieder, F., Griffiths, T. L., M. Huys, Q. J., & Goodman, N. D. (2017) The anchoring bias reflects rational use of cognitive resources. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 322349. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423–017-1286-8Google Scholar
Magnussen, S. & Dyrnes, S. (1994) High-fidelity perceptual long-term memory. Psychological Science, 5(2), 99102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00638.xGoogle Scholar
Marr, D. (1982) Vision: a computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. Freeman.Google Scholar
Mayer, R. E. (1979) Can advance organizers influence meaningful learning? Review of Educational Research, 49(2), 371383. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00346543049002371Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O’Reilly, R. C. (1995) Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102(3), 419457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L. & Rogers, T. T. (2003) The parallel distributed processing approach to semantic cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(4), 310322. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1076Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2007) Using the methods of experimental social psychology to study cultural evolution. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 1(2), 3538.Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1975) A framework for representing knowledge. In Winston, P. H. (Ed.). The psychology of computer vision (pp. 211277). McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Mitterer, H. & de Ruiter, J. P. (2008) Recalibrating color categories using world knowledge. Psychological Science, 19(7), 629634. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02133.xGoogle Scholar
Moscovitch, M., Rosenbaum, R. S., Gilboa, A. et al. (2005) Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: a unified account based on multiple trace theory. Journal of Anatomy, 207(1), 3566. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00421.xGoogle Scholar
Nissen, M. J. and Bullemer, P. (1987) Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19(1), 132. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8Google Scholar
Oaksford, M. & Chater, N. (1996) Rational explanation of the selection task. Psychological Review, 103(2), 381391. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.2.381Google Scholar
Orhan, A. E. & Jacobs, R. A. (2013) A probabilistic clustering theory of the organization of visual short-term memory. Psychological Review, 120(2), 297328. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031541Google Scholar
Parpart, P., Jones, M., & Love, B. C. (2018) Heuristics as Bayesian inference under extreme priors. Cognitive Psychology, 102, 127144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.11.006Google Scholar
Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007) Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(12), 976987. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2277Google Scholar
Persaud, K. & Hemmer, P. (2014) The influence of knowledge and expectations for color on episodic memory. In Bello, P., Guarini, M., McShane, M., & Scassellati, B. (Eds.). Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Persaud, K. & Hemmer, P. (2016) The dynamics of fidelity over the time course of long-term memory. Cognitive Psychology, 88, 121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.05.003Google Scholar
Popov, V., Zhang, Q., Koch, G. E., Calloway, R. C., & Coutanche, M. N. (2019) Semantic knowledge influences whether novel episodic associations are represented symmetrically or asymmetrically. Memory & Cognition, 47(8), 15671581.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. & Keele, S. W. (1968) On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77(3, Pt.1), 353363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0025953Google Scholar
Raaijmakers, J. G. & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981) Search of associative memory. Psychological Review, 88(2), 93134.Google Scholar
Richards, B. A., Xia, F., Santoro, A. et al. (2014) Patterns across multiple memories are identified over time. Nature Neuroscience, 17(7), 981986. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.3736Google Scholar
Richter, F. R., Bays, P. M., Jeyarathnarajah, P., & Simons, J. S. (2019) Flexible updating of dynamic knowledge structures. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 115.http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598–019-39468-9Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L. & McDermott, K. B. (1995) Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(4), 803814.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C. & Abelson, R. P. (1977) Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Schooler, L. J. & Anderson, J. R. (1997) The role of process in the rational analysis of memory. Cognitive Psychology, 32(3), 219250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1997.0652Google Scholar
Schulman, A. I. (1974) Memory for words recently classified. Memory & Cognition, 2(1), 4752. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03197491Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1955) A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69(1): 99118. https://doi.org/10.2307/1884852Google Scholar
Steyvers, M. & Griffiths, T. L. (2008) Rational analysis as a link between human memory and information retrieval. In Chater, N & Oaksford, M (Eds.). The probabilistic mind: prospects for Bayesian cognitive science (pp. 329349). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Steyvers, M., Griffiths, T. L., & Dennis, S. (2006) Probabilistic inference in human semantic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(7), 327334.Google Scholar
Steyvers, M. & Hemmer, P. (2012). Reconstruction from memory in naturalistic environments. In Psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 56 (pp. 125144). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Steyvers, M., Tenenbaum, J. B., Wagenmakers, E. J., & Blum, B. (2003) Inferring causal networks from observations and interventions. Cognitive Science, 27(3), 453489.Google Scholar
Sweegers, C. C. G., Coleman, G. A., van Poppel, E. A. M., Cox, R., & Talamini, L. M. (2015) Mental schemas hamper memory storage of goal-irrelevant information. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00629Google Scholar
Sweegers, C. C. G. & Talamini, L. M. (2014) Generalization from episodic memories across time: a route for semantic knowledge acquisition. Cortex, 59, 4961. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.006Google Scholar
Synodinos, N. E. (1986) Hindsight distortion: i knew it- all-along and I was sure about it. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 16(2), 107117.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E. & Crocker, J. (1981) Schematic bases of social information processing. In Higgins, E. T., Herman, C. P., & Zanna, M. P. (Eds.). Social cognition: the Ontario symposium, Vol. 1(pp. 89134). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Todorovic, D. (2010) Context effects in visual perception and their explanations. Review of Psychology 17(1), 1732.Google Scholar
Tompary, A. & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2021) Semantic influences on episodic memory distortions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(9), 18001824. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0001017Google Scholar
Tompary, A., Zhou, W., & Davachi, L. (2020) Schematic memories develop quickly, but are not expressed unless necessary. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598–020-73952-xGoogle Scholar
Torralba, A. (2003) Contextual priming for object detection. International Journal of Computer Vision 53(2), 169191. http://dxdoi.org/10.1023/A:1023052124951Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. C. (1996) The role of lexical frequency in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(4), 566585. http://dxdoi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0030Google Scholar
Tse, D., Langston, R. F., Kakeyama, M. et al. (2007) Schemas and memory consolidation. Science, 316(5821), 7682. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1135935Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1983) Elements of episodic memory. Clarendon.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. & Markowitsch, H. J. (1998) Episodic and declarative memory: role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 8(3), 198204.Google Scholar
van Kesteren, M. T., Rignanese, P, Gianferrara, P. G, Krabbendam, L, & Meeter, M (2020) Congruency and reactivation aid memory integration through reinstatement of prior knowledge. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 113.Google Scholar
van Kesteren, M. T. R., Rijpkema, M., Ruiter, D. J., & Fernandez, G. (2010) Retrieval of associative information congruent with prior knowledge is related to increased medial prefrontal activity and connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(47), 1588815894. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2674-10.2010Google Scholar
van Kesteren, M. T. R., Rijpkema, M., Ruiter, D. J., & Fernández, G. (2013) Consolidation differentially modulates schema effects on memory for items and associations. PLoS ONE, 8(2), e56155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0056155Google Scholar
Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleger, R., & Born, J. (2004) Sleep inspires insight. Nature, 427(6972), 352355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature02223Google Scholar
Wilson, S., Arora, S., Zhang, Q., & Griffiths, T.L. (2021) A rational account of anchor effects in hindsight bias. Proceedings of the 43th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (Vol. 43, No. 43).Google Scholar
Xu, J. & Griffiths, T. L. (2010) A rational analysis of the effects of memory biases on serial reproduction. Cognitive psychology, 60(2), 107126.Google Scholar
Zeng, T., Tompary, A., Schapiro, A. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. (2021) Tracking the relation between gist and item memory over the course of long-term memory consolidation. eLife 10:e65588. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.65588Google Scholar
Zhang, Q., Griffiths, T. & Norman, K. (in press). Optimal policies for free recall. Psychological Review.Google Scholar
Zhang, Q., Popov, V., Koch, G. E., Calloway, R. C., & Coutanche, M. N. (2018) Fast memory integration facilitated by schema consistency. Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 27772782). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar

References

Barsalou, L. W. (1983) Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition, 11(3), 211227.Google Scholar
Bigelow, E. J. & Piantadosi, S. T. (2016a) Inferring priors in compositional cognitive models. In Proceedings of the 38th Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Bigelow, E. J. & Piantadosi, S. T. (2016b) A large dataset of generalization patterns in the number game. Journal of Open Psychology Data, 4(1), e4e4.Google Scholar
Boole, G. (1854) An investigation of the laws of thought: on which are founded the mathematical theories of logic and probabilities. Walton and Maberly.Google Scholar
Burris, S. (2000) The laws of Boole’s thought. Preprint.Google Scholar
Chater, N. & Vitányi, P. (2007) Ideal learning of natural language: positive results about learning from positive evidence. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 51(3), 135163.Google Scholar
Denison, S. & Xu, F. (2014) The origins of probabilistic inference in human infants. Cognition, 130(3), 335347.Google Scholar
Depeweg, S., Rothkopf, C. A., & Jäkel, F. (2018) Solving bongard problems with a visual language and pragmatic reasoning. arXiv preprint arXiv:1804.04452.Google Scholar
Eckert, J., Call, J., Hermes, J., Herrmann, E., & Rakoczy, H. (2018) Intuitive statistical inferences in chimpanzees and humans follow Weber’s law. Cognition, 180, 99107.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1975) The language of thought. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. & Pylyshyn, Z. (1988) Connectionism and cognitive architecture: a critical analysis. Cognition, 28, 371.Google Scholar
Gershman, S. J. (2017) On the blessing of abstraction. SAGE.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. & Selten, R. (2002) Bounded rationality: the adaptive toolbox. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, N. D., Frank, M. C., Griffiths, T. L., Tenenbaum, J. B., Battaglia, P. W., & Hamrick, J. B. (2015) Relevant and robust: a response to Marcus and Davis (2013). Psychological Science, 26(4), 539541.Google Scholar
Goodman, N. D., Tenenbaum, J., Feldman, J., & Griffiths, T. (2008) A rational analysis of rule-based concept learning. Cognitive Science, 32(1), 108154.Google Scholar
Goodman, N. D., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Gerstenberg, T. (2015) Concepts in a probabilistic language of thought. In Margolis, E & Laurence, S (Eds.), The conceptual mind: new directions in the study of concepts. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, N. D., Ullman, T. D., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2011) Learning a theory of causality. Psychological Review, 118(1), 110119.Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L., Lieder, F., & Goodman, N. D. (2015) Rational use of cognitive resources: levels of analysis between the computational and the algorithmic. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7(2), 217229.Google Scholar
Heinze-Deml, C., Maathuis, M. H., & Meinshausen, N. (2018) Causal structure learning. Annual Review of Statistics and Its Application, 5(1), 371391.Google Scholar
Hutter, M. (2005) Universal artificial intelligence: sequential decisions based on algorithmic probability. Springer Science & Business Media.Google Scholar
Jaynes, E. (2003) Probability theory: the logic of science. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jones, M. & Love, B. C. (2011) Bayesian fundamentalism or enlightenment? On the explanatory status and theoretical contributions of Bayesian models of cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(4), 169231.Google Scholar
Katz, Y. & Springer, M. (2016) Probabilistic adaptation in changing microbial environments. PeerJ, 4, e2716.Google Scholar
Katz, Y., Springer, M., & Fontana, W. (2018) Embodying probabilistic inference in biochemical circuits. arXiv preprint arXiv:1806.10161.Google Scholar
Kemp, C. & Tenenbaum, J. (2008) The discovery of structural form. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(31), 1068710692.Google Scholar
Kemp, C., Tenenbaum, J. B., Niyogi, S., & Griffiths, T. L. (2010) A probabilistic model of theory formation. Cognition, 114(2), 165196.Google Scholar
Kidd, C., Piantadosi, S. T., & Aslin, R. (2012) The Goldilocks effect: human infants allocate attention to visual sequences that are neither too simple nor too complex. PLoS ONE 7(5), e36399.Google Scholar
Kidd, C., Piantadosi, S. T., & Aslin, R. N. (2014) The goldilocks effect in infant auditory attention. Child Development, 85(5), 17951804.Google Scholar
Knill, D. (1996) Perception as Bayesian inference. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kolmogorov, A. (1950) Foundations of the theory of probability. Chelsea Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Lake, B. M., Salakhutdinov, R., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2015). Human-level concept learning through probabilistic program induction. Science, 350(6266), 13321338.Google Scholar
Marcus, G. F. & Davis, E. (2013) How robust are probabilistic models of higher-level cognition? Psychological science, 24(12), 23512360.Google Scholar
Marcus, G. F. & Davis, E. (2015) Still searching for principles: a response to Goodman et al. (2015). Psychological Science, 26 (4), 542544.Google Scholar
Musolino, J., d’Agostino, K. L., & Piantadosi, S. T. (2019) Why we should abandon the semantic subset principle. Language Learning and Development, 15(1), 3246.Google Scholar
Nilsson, N. J. (2009). The quest for artificial intelligence. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Overlan, M. C., Jacobs, R. A., & Piantadosi, S. T. (2017) Learning abstract visual concepts via probabilistic program induction in a language of thought. Cognition, 168, 320334.Google Scholar
Piantadosi, S. T. (2011) Learning and the language of thought (Doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology).Google Scholar
Piantadosi, S. T. & Jacobs, R. (2016) Four problems solved by the probabilistic language of thought. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(1), 5459.Google Scholar
Piantadosi, S. T., Kidd, C., & Aslin, R. (2014) Rich analysis and rational models: inferring individual behavior from infant looking data. Developmental Science, 17, 321337.Google Scholar
Piantadosi, S. T., Tenenbaum, J., & Goodman, N. (2012) Bootstrapping in a language of thought: a formal model of numerical concept learning. Cognition, 123(2), 199217.Google Scholar
Piantadosi, S. T., Tenenbaum, J., & Goodman, N. (2016) The logical primitives of thought: Empirical foundations for compositional cognitive models. Psychological Review, 123(4), 392424.Google Scholar
Rakoczy, H., Clüver, A., Saucke, L. et al. (2014) Apes are intuitive statisticians. Cognition, 131(1), 6068.Google Scholar
Ramsey, F. P. (1926) Truth and probability. In Readings in formal epistemology (pp. 2145). Springer.Google Scholar
Rothe, A., Lake, B. M., & Gureckis, T. (2017) Question asking as program generation. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 1047–1056.Google Scholar
Rule, J., Tenenbaum, J., & Piantadosi, S. (2020) The child as hacker. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(11), 900915.Google Scholar
Russell, S. & Norvig, P. (2009) Artificial intelligence: a modern approach. Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Saffran, J., Aslin, R., & Newport, E. (1996) Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274(5294), 19261928.Google Scholar
Saffran, J., Johnson, E. K., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1999) Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition, 70(1), 2752.Google Scholar
Savage, L. J. (1954) The Foundations of Statistics. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1980) Multidimensional scaling, tree-fitting, and clustering. Science, 210(4468), 390398.Google Scholar
Sides, A., Osherson, D., Bonini, N., & Viale, R. (2002) On the reality of the conjunction fallacy. Memory & Cognition, 30(2), 191198.Google Scholar
Solomonoff, R. J. (1964a) A formal theory of inductive inference. Part I. Information and Control, 7(1), 122.Google Scholar
Solomonoff, R. J. (1964b) A formal theory of inductive inference. Part II. Information and control, 7(2), 224254.Google Scholar
Talbott, W. (2016) Bayesian Epistemology. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.). The stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Winter 2016 ed.). Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Tecwyn, E. C., Denison, S., Messer, E. J., & Buchsbaum, D. (2017) Intuitive probabilistic inference in capuchin monkeys. Animal Cognition, 20(2), 243256.Google Scholar
Téglás, E., Vul, E., Girotto, V., Gonzalez, M., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Bonatti, L. L. (2011) Pure Reasoning in 12-Month-Old Infants as Probabilistic Inference. Science, 27(332), 10541059.Google Scholar
Tenenbaum, J. (1999) A Bayesian framework for concept learning (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Tenenbaum, J. (2000) Rules and similarity in concept learning. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 12, 5965.Google Scholar
Tenenbaum, J., Kemp, C., Griffiths, T., & Goodman, N. (2011) How to grow a mind: statistics, structure, and abstraction. Science, 331(6022), 12791285.Google Scholar
Tijms, H. & Staats, K. (2007) Negative probabilities at work in the m/d/1 queue. Probability in the Engineering and Informational Sciences, 21(1), 6776.Google Scholar
Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1981) Judgments of and by representativeness (Tech. Rep.). Stanford University, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1983) Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90(4), 293315.Google Scholar
Ullman, T., Goodman, N., & Tenenbaum, J. (2012) Theory learning as stochastic search in the language of thought. Cognitive Development, 27(4), 455480.Google Scholar
Xu, F. & Denison, S. (2009) Statistical inference and sensitivity to sampling in 11-month-old infants. Cognition, 112(1), 97104.Google Scholar
Xu, F. & Garcia, V. (2008) Intuitive statistics by 8-month-old infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(13), 50125015.Google Scholar
Zednik, C. & Jäkel, F. (2016) Bayesian reverse-engineering considered as a research strategy for cognitive science. Synthese, 193(12), 39513985.Google Scholar

References

Carey, S. (1985Conceptual change in childhood. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1991Knowledge acquisition: enrichment or conceptual change? In Carey, S., & Gelman, R. (Eds.). The Jean Piaget Symposium series. The epigenesis of mind: essays on biology and cognition (pp. 257291). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Needham, R. (1972) Belief, language and experience. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Recanati, F. (1997) Can we believe what we do not understand? Mind & Language12(1), 84100.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1975) Rethinking symbolism. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1982/1985) Apparently irrational beliefs. In Lukes, S., & Hollis, M. (Eds.). Rationality and relativism (pp. 149180). Blackwell. Revised version in D. Sperber (1985), On anthropological knowledge. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1990) The epidemiology of beliefs. In Fraser, C., & Gaskell, G. (Eds.). The social psychological study of widespread beliefs (pp. 2544). Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1994a) Understanding verbal understanding. In Khalfa, J. (Ed.). What is intelligence? (pp. 179198) Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1994b) The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. In Hirschfeld, L. A., & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.). Mapping the mind: domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996) Explaining culture: a naturalistic approach. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1981) Irony and the use-mention distinction. In Cole, P. (Ed.). Radical pragmatics. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1986/1995) Relevance: communication and cognition. Blackwell. (2nd revised ed. 1995)Google Scholar
Wilford, J. N. (1981) The mapmakers. Alfred Knopf.Google Scholar
Wilson, D., & Sperber, D. (1992) On verbal irony. Lingua, 87(S1–2), 5376.Google Scholar

References

Bago, B. & De Neys, W. (2017) Fast logic? Examining the time course assumption of dual process theory. Cognition, 158, 90109.Google Scholar
Bago, B. & De Neys, W. (2020) Advancing the specification of dual process models of higher cognition: a critical test of the hybrid model view. Thinking & Reasoning, 26(1), 130.Google Scholar
Banks, A. P. & Hope, C. (2014) Heuristic and analytic processes in reasoning: an event-related potential study of belief bias. Psychophysiology, 51(3), 290297. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12169Google Scholar
Barrett, J. L. (2004) Why would anyone believe in God? AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. L. & Lanman, J. A. (2008) The science of religious beliefs. Religion, 38(2), 109124.Google Scholar
Beller, S. (2010) Deontic reasoning reviewed: psychological questions, empirical findings, and current theories. Cognitive Processing, 11(2), 123132. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-009-0265-zGoogle Scholar
Bonnefon, J. F. (2011) Le raisonneur et ses modèles: Un changement de paradigme dans la psychologie du raisonnement. Presses Universitaires de Grenoble.Google Scholar
Cohen, L. J. (1981) Can human irrationality be experimentally demonstrated? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4(03), 317331. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00009092Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (1999) Toward an evolutionary taxonomy of treatable conditions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(3), 453464. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.108.3.453Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (2004) Knowing thyself: the evolutionary psychology of moral reasoning and moral sentiments. The Ruffin Series of the Society for Business Ethics, 4, 93128. https://doi.org/10.5840/ruffinx200447Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., Barrett, H. C., & Tooby, J. (2010) Adaptive specializations, social exchange, and the evolution of human intelligence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(2), 90079014. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914623107Google Scholar
De Neys, W. (2006) Dual processing in reasoning: two systems but one reasoner. Psychological Science, 17(5), 428433.Google Scholar
De Neys, W. (2012) Bias and conflict: a case for logical intuitions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(1), 2838. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611429354Google Scholar
De Neys, W. (2014) Conflict detection, dual processes, and logical intuitions: some clarifications. Thinking & Reasoning, 20(2), 169187.Google Scholar
De Neys, W. (2017) Bias, conflict, and fast logic: towards a hybrid dual process future? In De Neys, W. (Ed.). Dual process theory 2.0 (pp. 4765). Routledge.Google Scholar
De Neys, W. & Bonnefon, J. F. (2013) The “whys” and “whens” of individual differences in thinking biases. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(4), 172178.Google Scholar
De Neys, W. & Glumicic, T. (2008) Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of thinking. Cognition, 106(3), 12481299.Google Scholar
De Neys, W., Moyens, E., & Vansteenwegen, D. (2010) Feeling we’re biased: autonomic arousal and reasoning conflict. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(2), 208216.Google Scholar
Dominowski, R. L. (1995) Content effects in Wason’s selection task. In Newstead, S. E., & Evans, J. S. B. T. (Eds.). Perspectives on thinking and reasoning: essays in honour of Peter Wason, (pp. 4165). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. (2002) Logic and human reasoning: an assessment of the deduction paradigm. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 978996. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.978Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. (2007) Hypothetical thinking: dual processes in reasoning and judgement. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. (2008) Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255278.Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. (2011) Dual-process theories of reasoning: contemporary issues and developmental applications. Developmental Review, 31(2), 86102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.007Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. & Curtis-Holmes, J. (2005) Rapid responding increases belief bias: evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 11(4), 382389.Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B., Handley, S. J., Neilens, H., & Over, D. (2010) The influence of cognitive ability and instructional set on causal conditional inference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(5), 892909.Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B., Newstead, S. E., & Byrne, R. M. (1993) Human reasoning: the psychology of deduction. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. T. (2005) Deductive reasoning. In Holyoak, K. J., & Morrison, R. G. (Eds.). The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 169184). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fiddick, L., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000) No interpretation without representation: the role of domain-specific representations and inferences in the Wason selection task. Cognition, 77(1), 179. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00085-8Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2008) An evolutionarily informed education science. Educational Psychologist, 43(4), 179195. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520802392133Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. & Berch, D. (2015) Evolutionary approaches to understanding children’s academic achievement. In Scott, R. A. & Kosslyn, S. M. (Eds.). Emerging trends in the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 110). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118900772.etrds0123Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. & Berch, D. (2016) Evolution and children’s cognitive and academic development. In Geary, D. C. & Berch, D. (Eds.). Evolution and children’s cognitive and academic development (pp. 217249). Springer International. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-29986-0_9Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. & Bjorklund, D. F. (2000) Evolutionary developmental psychology. Child Development, 71(1), 5765. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00118Google Scholar
Gelin, M., Bonin, P., Méot, A., & Bugaiska, A. (2018) Do animacy effects persist in memory for context? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(4), 965974. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1307866Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. & Hug, K. (1992) Domain-specific reasoning: social contracts, cheating, and perspective change. Cognition, 43(2), 127171. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90060-UGoogle Scholar
Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (Eds.) (2002) Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Glenberg, A. M., Goldberg, A. B., & Zhu, X. (2011) Improving early reading comprehension using embodied CAI. Instructional Science, 39(1), 2739. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-009-9096-7Google Scholar
Griggs, R. A. & Cox, J. R. (1982) The elusive thematic-materials effect in Wason’s selection task. British Journal of Psychology, 73(3), 407420. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1982.tb01823.xGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001) The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgement. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814834. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.%20108.4.814Google Scholar
Haigh, M. & Bonnefon, J. F. (2015) Conditional sentences create a blind spot in theory of mind during narrative comprehension. Acta Psychologica, 160, 194201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.05.009Google Scholar
Hanson, R. (1997) Are beliefs like clothes? George Mason University. https://mason.gmu.edu/~rhanson/belieflikeclothes.htmlGoogle Scholar
Haselton, M. G. & Buss, D. M. (2000) Error management theory: a new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 8191. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.78.1.81Google Scholar
Haselton, M. G., Buss, D. M., & DeKay, W. T. (1998) A theory of errors in cross-sex mindreading. Paper presented at the Human Behavior and Evolution Society Meeting, Davis, CA.Google Scholar
Kahan, D. M. (2015) The politically motivated reasoning paradigm, part 1: what politically motivated reasoning is and how to measure it. Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences, 116.Google Scholar
Kahan, D. M., Peters, E., Dawson, E. C., & Slovic, P. (2017) Motivated numeracy and enlightened self-government. Behavioural Public Policy, 1(1), 5486.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. & Frederick, S. (2005) A model of heuristic judgment. In Holyoak, K. J. & Morrison, R. G. (Eds.). The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 267293). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Slovic, S. P., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.) (1982) Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kirschner, F., Paas, F., & Kirschner, P. A. (2011) Task complexity as a driver for collaborative learning efficiency: the collective working-memory effect. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(4), 615624. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1730Google Scholar
Klauer, K. C. & Singmann, H. (2013) Does logic feel good? Testing for intuitive detection of logicality in syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(4), 12651273.Google Scholar
Leding, J. K. (2018) The animacy advantage in memory: manipulations of levels of processing and survival processing. American Journal of Psychology, 131(3), 273281. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.131.3.0273Google Scholar
Lespiau, F. & Tricot, A. (2018) Primary knowledge enhances performance and motivation in reasoning. Learning and Instruction, 56, 1019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.02.007Google Scholar
Lespiau, F. & Tricot, A. (2019) Using primary knowledge: an efficient way to motivate students and promote the learning of formal reasoning. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 915938. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09482-4Google Scholar
Manktelow, K. I. & Over, D. E. (1991) Social roles and utilities in reasoning with deontic conditionals. Cognition, 39(2), 85105. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(91)90039-7Google Scholar
Mercier, H. & Sperber, D. (2009) Intuitive and reflective inferences. In In two minds: dual processes and beyond. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Morsanyi, K. & Handley, S. J. (2008) How smart do you need to be to get it wrong? The role of cognitive capacity in the development of heuristic-based judgment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99(1), 1836. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2007.08.003Google Scholar
Morsanyi, K. & Handley, S. J. (2012) Logic feels so good—I like it! Evidence for intuitive detection of logicality in syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(3), 596616. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026099Google Scholar
Nairne, J. S. (2016) Adaptive memory: fitness-relevant “tunings” help drive learning and remembering. In Geary, C. D. & Berch, B. D. (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on child development and education (pp. 251269). Springer International.Google Scholar
Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N. S., & Thompson, S. R. (2008) Adaptive memory: the comparative value of survival processing. Psychological Science, 19(2), 176180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02064.xGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, H. & Kawaguchi, J. (2016) People like logical truth: testing the intuitive detection of logical value in basic propositions. PLOS ONE, 11(12), 121. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169166Google Scholar
Paas, F. & Ayres, P. (2014) Cognitive load theory: a broader view on the role of memory in learning and education. Educational Psychology Review, 26(2), 191195. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-014-9263-5Google Scholar
Paas, F. & Sweller, J. (2012) An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 24(1), 2745. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-011-9179-2Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Fugelsang, J. A., & Koehler, D. J. (2015) What makes us think? A three-stage dual-process model of analytic engagement. Cognitive Psychology, 80, 3472.Google Scholar
Ping, R. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010) Gesturing saves cognitive resources when talking about nonpresent objects. Cognitive Science, 34(4), 602619. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01102.xGoogle Scholar
Rizeq, J., Flora, D. B., & Toplak, M. E. (2020) An examination of the underlying dimensional structure of three domains of contaminated mindware: paranormal beliefs, conspiracy beliefs, and anti-science attitudes. Thinking & Reasoning, 27(2), 187211.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1982) Models of bounded rationality. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S. & Kinzler, K. D. (2007) Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10(1), 8996. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00569.xGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (1997) Intuitive and reflective beliefs. Mind & Language, 12(1), 6783.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K.E. (2009) What intelligence tests miss. The psychology of rational thought. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2011) Rationality and the reflective mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. & West, R. F. (2000) Individual differences in reasoning: implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(05), 701717.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. & West, R. F. (2003) Evolutionary versus instrumental goals: How evolutionary psychology misconceives human rationality. In Over, D. E. (Ed.). Evolution and the psychology of thinking: the debate (pp. 171230). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Stupple, E. J. & Ball, L. J. (2008) Belief–logic conflict resolution in syllogistic reasoning: inspection-time evidence for a parallel-process model. Thinking & Reasoning, 14(2), 168181. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546780701739782Google Scholar
Stupple, E. J. & Waterhouse, E. F. (2009) Negations in syllogistic reasoning: evidence for a heuristic–analytic conflict. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 15331541. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210902785674Google Scholar
Sweller, J. (2004) Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture. Instructional Science, 32(1), 931. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021808.72598.4dGoogle Scholar
Sweller, J. (2008) Instructional implications of David C. Geary’s evolutionary educational psychology. Educational Psychologist, 43(4), 214216. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520802392208Google Scholar
Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011) Cognitive load theory (vol. 1). Springer.Google Scholar
Sweller, J. & Sweller, S. (2006) Natural information processing systems. Evolutionary Psychology, 4(1), 434458. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470490600400135Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1995) Mapping the evolved functional organization of mind and brain. In Gazzaniga, M. S., Bizzi, E., & Caramazza, A. (Eds.). The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 11851197). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Trémolière, B., De Neys, W., & Bonnefon, J. F. (2014) The grim reasoner: analytical reasoning under mortality salience. Thinking & Reasoning, 20(3), 333351.Google Scholar
Trémolière, B., De Neys, W., & Bonnefon, J. F. (2017) Reasoning and moral judgment: A common experimental toolbox. In The Routledge international handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 575589). Routledge.Google Scholar
Trémolière, B., Gagnon, M. È., & Blanchette, I. (2016) Cognitive load mediates the effect of emotion on analytical thinking. Experimental Psychology, 63(6), 343350.Google Scholar
Tricot, A. & Roussel, S. (2016) Quelles connaissances de la langue orale est-il nécessaire d’enseigner? Une contribution évolutionniste. In Grandaty, M., & Lafontaine, L. (Eds.). L’enseignement de l’oral à l’école, Les Dossiers des sciences de l’éducation, no. 36 (pp. 7594). Presses Universitaires du Midi.Google Scholar
Trippas, D., Handley, S. J., & Verde, M. F. (2013) The SDT model of belief bias: complexity, time, and cognitive ability mediate the effects of believability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(5), 13931402.Google Scholar
Trippas, D., Handley, S. J., Verde, M. F., & Morsanyi, K. (2016) Logic brightens my day: evidence for implicit sensitivity to logical validity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(9), 14481457.Google Scholar
Valiña, M. D. & Martín, M. (2016) The influence of semantic and pragmatic factors in Wason’s selection task: state of the art. Psychology, 7, 925940. https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2016.76094Google Scholar
Van Gog, T., Paas, F., Marcus, N., Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (2009) The mirror neuron system and observational learning: implications for the effectiveness of dynamic visualizations. Educational Psychology Review, 21(1), 2130. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-008-9094-3Google Scholar
Wason, P. C. (1966) Reasoning. In Foss, B. (Ed.). New horizons in psychology (pp. 135151). Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Youssef, A., Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (2012) Using general problem-solving strategies to generate ideas in order to solve geography problems. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 872877. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2888Google Scholar

References

Barrett, J. L. (1999) Theological correctness: cognitive constraint and the study of religion. Method & Theory in the Study of Religion, 11(4), 325339.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. L. & Keil, F. C. (1996) Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: anthropomorphism in God concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 31(3), 219247.Google Scholar
Baumard, N. & Boyer, P. (2013) Religious beliefs as reflective elaborations on intuitions: a modified dual-process model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(4), 295300.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. & Liénard, P. (2006) Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(6), 595613.Google Scholar
Coury, C. (1967) The basic principles of medicine in the primitive mind. Medical History, 11(2), 111127.Google Scholar
Darley, J. M. & Latané, B. (1968) Bystander intervention in emergencies: diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8(4p1), 377383.Google Scholar
de Figueiredo, A., Simas, C., Karafillakis, E., Paterson, P., & Larson, H. J. (2020) Mapping global trends in vaccine confidence and investigating barriers to vaccine uptake: a large-scale retrospective temporal modelling study. The Lancet, 396(10255), 898908.Google Scholar
DiFonzo, N. & Bordia, P. (2007) Rumor psychology: social and organizational approaches. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Diggory, J. C. (1956) Some consequences of proximity to a disease threat. Sociometry, 19(1), 4753.Google Scholar
Douglas, K. M. (2021) Are conspiracy theories harmless? The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 24, E13.Google Scholar
Durbach, N. (2000) “They might as well brand us”: working-class resistance to compulsory vaccination in Victorian England. Social History of Medicine, 13(1), 4563.Google Scholar
Horowitz, D. L. (2001) The deadly ethnic riot. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Hortensius, R. & de Gelder, B. (2018) From empathy to apathy: the bystander effect revisited. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(4), 249256.Google Scholar
Kay, J. (2011) Among the Truthers: a journey through America’s growing conspiracist underground. Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Kim, J. W. & Kim, E. (2019) Identifying the effect of political rumor diffusion using variations in survey timing. Quarterly Journal of Political Science, 14(3), 293311.Google Scholar
Leeson, P. T. (2012) Ordeals. The Journal of Law and Economics, 55(3), 691714.Google Scholar
McKay, R. T. & Dennett, D. C. (2009) The evolution of misbelief. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(06), 493510.Google Scholar
Mercier, H. (2020a) Not born yesterday: the science of who we trust and what we believe. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Mercier, H. (2020b) The cultural evolution of oaths, ordeals, and lie detectors. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 20(3–4), 159187.Google Scholar
Mercier, H. & Boyer, P. (2020) Truth-making institutions: from divination, ordeals and oaths to judicial torture and rules of evidence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 2(3), 259267.Google Scholar
Miton, H., Claidière, N., & Mercier, H. (2015) Universal cognitive mechanisms explain the cultural success of bloodletting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 303312.Google Scholar
Miton, H. & Mercier, H. (2015) Cognitive obstacles to pro-vaccination beliefs. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 19(11), 633636.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. (2021) Research note: examining false beliefs about voter fraud in the wake of the 2020 Presidential Election. Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Review. https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-51Google Scholar
Petersen, M. B. (2020) The evolutionary psychology of mass mobilization: how disinformation and demagogues coordinate rather than manipulate. Current Opinion in Psychology, 35, 7175.Google Scholar
Rappaport, R. A. & Rappaport, R. A. R. (1999) Ritual and religion in the making of humanity (Vol. 110). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Regehr, C., Goldberg, G., & Hughes, J. (2002) Exposure to human tragedy, empathy, and trauma in ambulance paramedics. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 72(4), 505513.Google Scholar
Santoro, D. & Kumar, M. (2018) Speaking truth to power – a theory of whistleblowing (Vol. 6). Springer.Google Scholar
Shibutani, T. (1966) Improvised news. A sociological study of rumor. Bobbs-Merrill Company.Google Scholar
Siegler, A. J., Mbwambo, J. K., McCarty, F. A., & DiClemente, R. J. (2012) Condoms “contain worms” and “cause HIV” in Tanzania: Negative Condom Beliefs Scale development and implications for HIV prevention. Social Science & Medicine, 75(9), 16851691. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.07.010Google Scholar
Silverman, D., Kaltenthaler, K., & Dagher, M. (2021) Seeing is disbelieving: the depths and limits of factual misinformation in war. International Studies Quarterly, 65(3), 798810.Google Scholar
Smith, M. J., Ellenberg, S. S., Bell, L. M., & Rubin, D. M. (2008) Media coverage of the measles-mumps-rubella vaccine and autism controversy and its relationship to MMR immunization rates in the United States. Pediatrics, 121(4), e836e843.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996) Explaining culture: a naturalistic approach. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1997) Intuitive and reflective beliefs. Mind and Language, 12(1), 6783.Google Scholar
Szegőfi, Á. (unpublished document) Blood Libels as Evolved Coalition Signals.Google Scholar
Torrey, N. L. (1961) Les philosophes. the philosophers of the enlightenment and modern democracy. Capricorn Books.Google Scholar
Uscinski, J. E. (2020) Conspiracy theories: a primer. Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.Google Scholar

References

Acerbi, A. (2019) Cultural evolution in the digital age. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Acerbi, A. & Tehrani, J. J. (2018) Did Einstein really say that? Testing content versus context in the cultural selection of quotations. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 18(3–4), 293311.Google Scholar
Altay, S., de Araujo, E., & Mercier, H. (2020) “If this account is true, it is most enormously wonderful”: interestingness-if-true and the sharing of true and false news. Digital Journalism, 10(3), 373394. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/tdfh5.Google Scholar
Arceneaux, K., Gravelle, T. B., Osmundsen, M., Petersen, M. B., Reifler, J., & Scotto, T. J. (2021). Some people just want to watch the world burn: the prevalence, psychology and politics of the “Need for Chaos.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1822), 20200147. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0147Google Scholar
Atran, S. & Henrich, J. (2010) The evolution of religion: how cognitive byproducts, adaptive learning heuristics, ritual displays, and group competition generate deep commitments to prosocial religions. Biological Theory, 5(1), 1830.Google Scholar
Atran, S., Medin, D., Ross, N. et al. (2002) Folkecology, cultural epidemiology, and the spirit of the commons: a garden experiment in the Maya lowlands, 1991–2001. Current Anthropology, 43(3), 421450.Google Scholar
Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992) The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C. (2020) Deciding what to observe: thoughts for a post-WEIRD generation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 445453.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. L. (2000) Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(1), 2934.Google Scholar
Barsbai, T., Lukas, D., & Pondorfer, A. (2021) Local convergence of behavior across species. Science, 371(6526), 292295.Google Scholar
Bendixen, T. (2019) Sense or non-sense? A critical discussion of a recent evolutionary–cognitive approach to “folk-economic beliefs”. Evolution, Mind and Behaviour, 17(1), 2947.Google Scholar
Bendixen, T. (2020) How cultural evolution can inform the science of science communication – and vice versa. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 7(1), 135. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-020-00634-4Google Scholar
Bendixen, T. & Purzycki, B. G. (2021a) Competing forces account for the stability and evolution of religious beliefs. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 31(4), 307312. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2020.1844969.Google Scholar
Bendixen, T. & Purzycki, B. G. (2021b) Peering into the minds of gods: what cross-cultural variation in gods’ concerns can tell us about the evolution of religion. Journal for the Cognitive Science of Religion, 5(2), 142165.Google Scholar
Bendixen, T., Apicella, C. L., Atkinson, Q. et al. (forthcoming) Appealing to the minds of gods: Religious beliefs and appeals correspond to features of local social ecologies. Religion, Brain & Behavior. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/tjn3eGoogle Scholar
Bentzen, J. S. (2019) Acts of God? Religiosity and natural disasters across subnational world districts. The Economic Journal, 129(622), 22952321.Google Scholar
Berl, R. E. W., Samarasinghe, A. N., Roberts, S. G., Jordan, F. M., & Gavin, M. C. (2021). Prestige and content biases together shape the cultural transmission of narratives. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 3, 122. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2021.37Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J., Brown, G. R., Richardson, R. C., & Laland, K. N. (2011) Darwin in mind: new opportunities for evolutionary psychology. PLOS Biology, 9(7), e1001109.Google Scholar
Botero, C. A., Gardner, B., Kirby, K. R., Bulbulia, J., Gavin, M. C., & Gray, R. D. (2014) The ecology of religious beliefs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(47), 1678416789.Google Scholar
Boudry, M. (2020) The truth is (still) out there: On the epistemology and cultural dynamics of conspiracy beliefs. http://philsci-archive.pitt.edu/17699/.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. (2017) A different kind of animal: how culture transformed our species. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1992) Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13(3), 171195.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2001) Religion explained: the evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. & Petersen, M. B. (2018) Folk-economic beliefs: an evolutionary cognitive model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, e158.Google Scholar
Brotherton, R. (2015) Suspicious minds: why we believe conspiracy theories. Bloomsbury Sigma.Google Scholar
Buskell, A., Enquist, M., & Jansson, F. (2019) A systems approach to cultural evolution. Palgrave Communications, 5(1), 115.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2019) Evolutionary psychology: the new science of the mind, 6th ed. Routledge.Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Feldman, M. W. (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Chudek, M., Zhao, W. & Henrich, J. (2013) Culture-gene coevolution, large-scale cooperation, and the shaping of human social psychology. In Sterelny, K., Richard, J., Brett, C., & Ben, F. (Eds.). Cooperation and its evolution, life and mind: philosophical issues in biology and psychology (pp. 425457). The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Confer, J. C., Easton, J. A., Fleischman, D. S. et al. (2010) Evolutionary psychology: controversies, questions, prospects, and limitations. The American Psychologist, 65(2), 110126.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. (1989) The logic of social exchange: has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31(3), 187276.Google Scholar
Creanza, N., Kolodny, O., & Feldman, M. W. (2017) Cultural evolutionary theory: how culture evolves and why it matters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 77827789.Google Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Callan, M. J., Dawtry, R. J., & Harvey, A. J. (2016) Someone is pulling the strings: hypersensitive agency detection and belief in conspiracy theories. Thinking & Reasoning, 22(1), 5777.Google Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., & Cichocka, A. (2017) The psychology of conspiracy theories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 538542.Google Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Uscinski, J. E., Sutton, R. M. et al. (2019) Understanding conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 40(S1), 335.Google Scholar
Fogarty, L. & Creanza, N. (2017) The niche construction of cultural complexity: interactions between innovations, population size and the environment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1735), 20160428.Google Scholar
Frankenhuis, W. E. & Ploeger, A. (2007) Evolutionary psychology versus Fodor: arguments for and against the massive modularity hypothesis. Philosophical Psychology, 20(6), 687710.Google Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2006) Evolutionary foundations of cultural variation: evoked culture and mate preferences. Psychological Inquiry, 17(2), 7595.Google Scholar
Gervais, W. M. & Henrich, J. (2010) The Zeus problem: why representational content biases cannot explain faith in gods. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 10(3–4), 383389.Google Scholar
Gervais, W. M., Najle, M. B., & Caluori, N. (2019) The origins of religious disbelief: A dual inheritance approach. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(7), 13691379. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/e29rt.Google Scholar
Goldfinch, A. (2015). Rethinking evolutionary psychology. Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Gray, R. D., Heaney, M., & Fairhall, S. (2003) Evolutionary psychology and the challenge of adaptive expansion. In Sterelny, K., & Fitness, J., J. (Eds.). From mating to mentality: evaluating evolutionary psychology (pp. 247268). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L., Kalish, M. L., & Lewandowsky, S. (2008) Theoretical and empirical evidence for the impact of inductive biases on cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 35033514.Google Scholar
Guthrie, S. E. (1995) Faces in the clouds: anew theory of religion. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hagen, E. H. (2015) Evolutionary psychology and its critics. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.). The handbook of evolutionary psychology. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Bauer, M., Cassar, A., Chytilov’a, J., & Purzycki, B. G. (2019) War increases religiosity. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(2), 129135.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. & McElreath, R. (2003) The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 12(3), 123135.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2019). Precis of cognitive gadgets: the cultural evolution of thinking. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, e169.Google Scholar
Hill, K. & Boyd, R. (2021) Behavioral convergence in humans and animals. Science, 371(6526), 235236.Google Scholar
Jansson, F., Aguilar, E., Acerbi, A., & Enquist, M. (2021) Modelling cultural systems and selective filters. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1828), 20200045. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0045.Google Scholar
Jensen, J. (2019) What is religion? 2nd ed. Routledge.Google Scholar
Johnson, D. D. P. (2016) God is watching you: how the fear of god makes us human. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kahan, D. (2010) Fixing the communications failure. Nature, 463(7279), 296297.Google Scholar
Kahan, D. & Landrum, A. R. (2017) A tale of two vaccines – and their science communication environments. In Jamieson, K. H., Kahan, D., & Scheufele, D. (Eds.). The Oxford handbook of the science of science communication. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kahan, D. M. (2015) What is the “science of science communication”? SSRN Scholarly Paper ID 2562025, Social Science Research Network, Rochester, NY.Google Scholar
Kaplan, H. S., Hooper, P. L., & Gurven, M. (2009) The evolutionary and ecological roots of human social organization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1533), 32893299.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y., Bain, P. G., & Perfors, A. (2019). The psychology of cultural dynamics: what is it, what do we know, and what is yet to be known? Annual Review of Psychology, 70(1), 499529. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103112Google Scholar
Kendal, R. L., Boogert, N. J., Rendell, L., Laland, K. N., Webster, M., & Jones, P. L. (2018) Social learning strategies: bridge-building between fields. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(7), 651665.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. (2017) Darwin’s unfinished symphony: how culture made the human mind. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Leman, P. J. & Cinnirella, M. (2013) Beliefs in conspiracy theories and the need for cognitive closure. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 378.Google Scholar
Lifshitz, M., van Elk, M., & Luhrmann, T. M. (2019) Absorption and spiritual experience: a review of evidence and potential mechanisms. Consciousness and Cognition, 73, 102760.Google Scholar
Luhrmann, T. M., Weisman, K., Aulino, F. et al. (2021) Sensing the presence of gods and spirits across cultures and faiths. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(5) e2016649118.Google Scholar
Lumsden, C. J. & Wilson, E. O. (1981) Genes, mind, and culture: the coevolutionary process. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Mauritsen, A. L., Bendixen, T., & Christensen, H. R. (2022). Does a Pandemic Increase Religiosity in a Secular Nation? A Longitudinal Examination. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/qsgejGoogle Scholar
McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2001) Birds of a feather: homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27(1), 415444.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2011) Cultural evolution: how Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2019) Cultural evolution and cultural psychology. In Kitayama, S., & Cohen, D. (Eds.). Handbook of Cultural Psychology, 2nd ed. (pp. 144162). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Chang, L., Dall, S. R. X., & Thornton, A. (2016) The evolution of individual and cultural variation in social learning. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 31(3), 215225.Google Scholar
Miller, J. M. (2020) Do COVID-19 conspiracy theory beliefs form a monological belief system? Canadian Journal of Political Science/Revue Canadienne de Science Politique, 53(2), 319326.Google Scholar
Miton, H., Claidi’ere, N., & Mercier, H. (2015) Universal cognitive mechanisms explain the cultural success of bloodletting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 303312.Google Scholar
Nichols, S. (2004) Is religion what we want? Motivation and the cultural transmission of religious representations. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4(2), 347371.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A. F., Gervais, W. M. et al. (2016) The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, E1.Google Scholar
Odling-Smee, J. & Laland, K. N. (2011) Ecological inheritance and cultural inheritance: what are they and how do they differ? Biological Theory, 6(3), 220230.Google Scholar
Peoples, H. C., Duda, P., & Marlowe, F. W. (2016) Hunter-gatherers and the origins of religion. Human Nature, 27(3), 261282.Google Scholar
Pietraszewski, D. & Wertz, A. E. (2022). Why evolutionary psychology should abandon modularity. Perspectives on Psychological Science : A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 17(2), 465490. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691621997113Google Scholar
Prooijen, J.-W. (2018) The psychology of conspiracy theories. Routledge.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. (2011) Tyvan cher eezi and the socioecological constraints of supernatural agents’ minds. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1(1), 3145.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. (2013) The minds of gods: a comparative study of supernatural agency. Cognition, 129(1), 163179.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. (2016) The evolution of gods’ minds in the Tyva Republic. Current Anthropology, 57(S13), S88S104.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Finkel, D. N., Shaver, J., Wales, N., Cohen, A. B., & Sosis, R. (2012) What does god know? Supernatural agents’ access to socially strategic and non-strategic information. Cognitive Science, 36(5), 846869.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. & McKay, R. (2022). Moralistic Gods and Social Complexity: A Brief History of the Problem. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/vqjk2Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. & McNamara, R. A. (2016) An ecological theory of gods’ minds. In De Cruz, H., & Nichols, R., R. (Eds.). Cognitive science of religion and its philosophical implications (pp. 143167). Continuum.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. (2005) Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rodriguez, N., Bollen, J., & Ahn, Y.-Y. (2016) Collective dynamics of belief evolution under cognitive coherence and social conformity. PLOS One, 11(11), e0165910.Google Scholar
Roes, F. L. & Raymond, M. (2003) Belief in moralizing gods. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(2), 126135.Google Scholar
Rossano, M. J. (2007) Supernaturalizing social life: religion and the evolution of human cooperation. Human Nature, 18(3), 272294.Google Scholar
Schjoedt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Geertz, A. W., & Roepstorff, A. (2009) Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 199207.Google Scholar
Shapiro, L. & Epstein, W. (1998) Evolutionary theory meets cognitive psychology: a more selective perspective. Mind & Language, 13(2), 171194.Google Scholar
Sibley, C. G. & Bulbulia, J. (2012). Faith after an Earthquake: A Longitudinal Study of Religion and Perceived Health before and after the 2011 Christchurch New Zealand Earthquake. PLOS ONE, 7(12), e49648. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049648Google Scholar
Skoggard, I., Ember, C. R., Pitek, E., Jackson, J. C., & Carolus, C. (2020) Resource stress predicts changes in religious belief and increases in sharing behavior. Human Nature, 31(3), 249271.Google Scholar
Smith, E. A. (2000) Three styles in the evolutionary analysis of human behavior. In Cronk, L., Chagnon, N., Irons, & W. (Eds.). Adaptation and human behavior: an anthropological perspective (pp. 2746). Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Smolla, M., Jansson, F., Lehmann, L. et al. (2021) Underappreciated features of cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1828), 20200259. https://osf.io/preprints/socarxiv/3xvdw/Google Scholar
Snarey, J. (1996) The natural environment’s impact upon religious ethics: a cross-cultural study. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 35(2), 8596.Google Scholar
Sosis, R. & Bulbulia, J. (2011) The behavioral ecology of religion: the benefits and costs of one evolutionary approach. Religion, 41(3), 341362.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996) Explaining culture: a naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., Cl’ement, F., Heintz, C. et al. (2010) Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25(4), 359393.Google Scholar
Sørensen, J. (2004) Religion, evolution and an immunology of cultural systems. Evolution and Cognition, 10(1), 6173.Google Scholar
Stubbersfield, J. M. (2021). Conspiracy Theories: A Cultural Evolution Theory approach. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/hqkjuGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J. (2020) Evolutionary psychology as the crystalizing core of a unified modern social science. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 14(4),390403.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19136). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tylor, E. B. (1920) Primitive culture: researches into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, language, art, and custom. Murray.Google Scholar
Uscinski, J. E. (2020) Conspiracy theories: aprimer. Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.Google Scholar
van Mulukom, V., Pummerer, L. J., Alper, S. et al. (2022). Antecedents and consequences of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs: a systematic review. Social Science & Medicine, 301, 114912. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.114912Google Scholar
Varnum, M. E. W. & Grossmann, I. (2017). Cultural change: the how and the why. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 956972. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617699971Google Scholar
Willard, A. K. & Cingl, L. (2017) Testing theories of secularization and religious belief in the Czech Republic and Slovakia. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(5), 604615.Google Scholar
Willard, A. K., Henrich, J., & Norenzayan, A. (2016) Memory and belief in the transmission of counterintuitive content. Human Nature, 27(3), 221243.Google Scholar
Williams, D. (2021) Socially adaptive belief. Mind & Language, 36(3), 333354. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12294.Google Scholar
Wood, M. J., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2012) Dead and alive: beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(6), 767773.Google Scholar
Yeh, D. J., Fogarty, L., & Kandler, A. (2018) Cultural linkage: the influence of package transmission on cultural dynamics. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286(1916), 20191951.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×