Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T01:40:07.972Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Anthropogenic impacts on behavior: the pros and cons of plasticity

from Part II - Anthropogenic impacts on animal behavior and their implications for conservation and management

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2016

Daniel I. Rubenstein
Affiliation:
Princeton University, USA
Oded Berger-Tal
Affiliation:
Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel
David Saltz
Affiliation:
Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel
Get access

Summary

Many threatened and endangered species inhabit environments where people have altered the landscape and have created novel and powerful selective pressures. These often disrupt “bottom-up” factors associated with resource acquisition, change “top-down” factors involving predation or disease transmission or alter “side-ways” factors involving competitive or mutualistic interactions happening at the same trophic level. Since one of the basic precepts of behavioral ecology is that environmental conditions shape behavior, such changes should lead to changing responses by the species experiencing them. But this pre-supposes that there is enough plasticity in a species’ behavioral repertoire to cope with the environmental change. If there is, then contingent responses displayed in the past may help species cope with a changing present. If the degree of environmental change is so great or the species has a limited ability to adjust its behavior, then the species could be pushed beyond its limits to adapt, with the stress it experiences lowering its fecundity or survival, leading to its demise (Chapter 4). Alternatively, the species could be pushed into new behavioral space, revealing novel responses. These might be sufficient to cope with the environmental changes, creating new evolutionary potential, or they could be pathological, creating unintended negative consequences that may at first appear benign, but in the long run may impact the species’ viability or involve cascading effects on other species.

Recent reviews by Rubenstein (2010) and Caro and Sherman (2011, 2013) have provided insights into many dimensions of this problem. Caro and Sherman (2011) argue that species exhibit remarkable degrees of plasticity and that behavioral diversity per se should be added to the traditional list of attributes to be conserved – genes, species and ecosystems. If behaviors are not conserved, then many behavioral variants will remain latent and unexpressed, and if given enough time, will vanish from a species’ repertoire forever. This could potentially change a species’ evolutionary trajectory by reducing a species’ ability to escape from new stressors or by eliminating the possibility of conservationists seeding threatened populations with corrective behavioral variants. Such a doom and gloom scenario need not be cast in stone since dramatic changes in the past have often disrupted communities and altered selective pressures. Adaptations in the past have led to successful coping strategies and they can do so again going forward.

Type
Chapter
Information
Conservation Behavior
Applying Behavioral Ecology to Wildlife Conservation and Management
, pp. 121 - 146
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alós, J., Palmer, M., Trías, P., Díaz-Gil, C. and Arlinghaus, R. 2014. Recreational angling intensity correlates with alteration of vulnerability to fishing in a carnivorous coastal fish species. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences, 72:1–9.Google Scholar
Andrewartha, H.G. and Birch, L.C. 1964. The Distribution and Abundance of Animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Barnard, C.J. and Sibly, R.M. 1981. Producers and scroungers: a general model and its application to captive flocks of house sparrows. Animal Behaviour, 29:543–550.Google Scholar
Bejder, L, Samuels, A., Whitehead, H., Finn, H. and Allen, S. 2009. Impact assessment research: use and misuses of habituation, sensitization and tolerance in describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 395:177–185.Google Scholar
Berger-Tal, O., Nathan, J., Meron, E. and Saltz, D. 2014. The exploration–exploitation dilemma: a multidisciplinary framework. PloS one, 9:e95693Google Scholar
Cameron, E.Z., Setsaas, T.H. and Linklater, W.L. 2009. Social bonds between unrelated females increase reproductive success in feral horses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106:13850–13853.Google Scholar
Caro, T. and Sherman, P.W. 2011. Endangered species and a threatened discipline: behavioural ecology. Trends in Ecology &Evolution, 26:111–118.Google Scholar
Caro, T. and Sherman, P.W. 2013. Eighteen reasons animal behaviourists avoid involvement in conservation. Animal Behaviour, 85:305–312.Google Scholar
Cockburn, A. and Russell, A.F. 2011. Cooperative breeding: a question of climate?Current Biology, 21:R195–R197.Google Scholar
Davies, N.B., Krebs, J.R. and West, S.A. 2012. An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons.
Dukas, R. 1998. Evolutionary ecology of learning. In Dukas, R. (Ed.), Cognitive Ecology, pp. 129–174. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Dukas, R. 2004. Male fruit flies learn to avoid interspecific courtship. Behavioral Ecology, 15:695–698.Google Scholar
Dukas, R. 2010. Insect social learning. In Breed, M. and Moore, I. (Eds.) Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior, pp. 176–179, Oxford: Academic Press.
Dukas, R. 2013. Effects of learning on evolution: robustness, innovation and speciation. Animal Behaviour, 85:1023–1030.Google Scholar
Dukas, R. and Dukas, L. 2012. Learning about prospective mates in male fruit flies: effects of acceptance and rejection. Animal Behaviour. 84:1427–1434.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R.I. 1992. Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 22:469–493.Google Scholar
Ellis, J.E. and Swift, D.M. 1988. Stability of African pastoral ecosystems: alternate paradigms and implications for development. Journal of Range Management Archives, 41:450–459.Google Scholar
Farine, D.R., Aplin, L.M., Sheldon, B.C. and Hoppitt, W. 2015. Interspecific social networks promote information transmission in wild songbirds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282:20142804.Google Scholar
Franceschini, M.D., Rubenstein, D.I., Low, B. and Romero, L.M. 2008. Fecal glucocorticoid metabolite analysis as an indicator of stress during translocation and acclimation in an endangered large mammal, the Grevy's zebra. Animal Conservation 11:263–269.Google Scholar
Fischhoff, I.R., Sundaresan, S.R., Cordingley, J. and Rubenstein, D.I. 2007. Habitat use and movements of plains zebra (Equus burchelli) in response to predation danger from lions. Behavioral Ecology, 18:725–729.Google Scholar
Gates, J.E. and Gysel, L.W. 1978. Avian nest dispersion and fledging success in field-forest ecotones. Ecology, 59:871–883.Google Scholar
Gause, G.F. 1932. Experimental studies on the struggle for existence I. Mixed population of two species of yeast. Journal of Experimental Biology, 9:389–402.Google Scholar
Gilroy, J.J., Anderson, G.Q.A., Grice, P.V. and Sutherland, W.J. 2011. Identifying mismatches between habitat selection and habitat quality in a ground‐nesting farmland bird. Animal Conservation, 14:620–629.Google Scholar
Giraldeau, L.A. and Lefebvre, L. 1987. Scrounging prevents cultural transmission of food-finding behaviour in pigeons. Animal Behaviour, 35:387–394.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J.K.et al. 2012. Behavioral plasticity in honey bees is associated with differences in brain microRNA transcriptome. Gene Brain and Behavior, 11:660–670.Google Scholar
Grieco, F., van Noordwijk, A.J. and Visser, M.E. 2002. Evidence for the effect of learning on timing of reproduction in blue tits. Science, 296:136–138.Google Scholar
Greenwood, P.J. and Harvey, P.H. 1982. The natal and breeding dispersal of birds. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 13:1–21.Google Scholar
Guttal, V. and Couzin, I.D. 2011. Leadership, collective motion and the evolution of migratory strategies. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 4:294–298.Google Scholar
Horn, H.S. and Rubenstein, D. I. 1984. Behavioural adaptations and life history. In Krebs, J.R. and Davies, N.B. (Eds.), Behavioural Ecology, pp. 279–300. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Huang, C.F. and Sih, A. 1991. Experimental studies on direct and indirect interactions in three trophic-level stream systems. Oecologia, 85:530–536.Google Scholar
Jerison, H. 2012. Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. New York: Academic Press.
Jetz, W. and Rubenstein, D.R. 2011. Environmental uncertainty and the global biogeography of cooperative breeding in birds. Current Biology, 21:72–78.Google Scholar
Klingel, H. 1975. Social organization and reproduction in equids. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility, Supplement 23:7–11.Google Scholar
Kudo, H. and Dunbar, R.I.M. 2001. Neocortex size and social network size in primates. Animal Behaviour, 62:711–722.Google Scholar
Lahiri, M., Tantipathananandh, C., Warungu, R., Rubenstein, D.I. and Berger-Wolf, T.Y. 2011. Biometric animal databases from field photographs: identification of individual zebra in the wild. Proceedings of the ACM International Conference on Multimedia Retrieval (ICMR 2011), Trento, Italy.
Ledón-Rettig, C.C., Richards, C.L. and Martin, L.B. 2013. A place for behavior in ecological epigenetics. Behavioral Ecology, 24:329–330.Google Scholar
Lee, W.Y., Choe, J.C. and Jablonski, P.G. 2011. Wild birds recognize individual humans: experiments on magpies, Pica pica.Animal Cognition, 14:817–825.Google Scholar
Lefebvre, L., Reader, S.M., and Sol, D. 2004. Brains, innovations and evolution in birds and primates. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 63:233–246.Google Scholar
Linklater, W.L., Cameron, E.Z., Minot, E.O. and Stafford, K.J. 1999. Stallion harassment and the mating system of horses. Animal Behaviour, 58:295–306.Google Scholar
Lorenzen, E.D., Nogués-Bravo, D., Orlando, L., Weinstock, J., Binladen, J., Marske, K.A. and Cooper, A. 2011. Species-specific responses of Late Quaternary megafauna to climate and humans. Nature, 479:359–364.Google Scholar
Low, B., Sundaresan, S.R., Fischhoff, I.R. and Rubenstein, D.I. 2009. Partnering with local communities to identify conservation priorities for endangered Grevy's zebra. Biological Conservation, 142:1548–1555.Google Scholar
Macdonald, D.W. and Johnson, D.D.P. 2015. Patchwork planet: the resource dispersion hypothesis, society, and the ecology of life. Journal of Zoology. 295:75–107.Google Scholar
Macphail, E.M. and Barlow, H.B. 1985. Vertebrate intelligence: the null hypothesis [and discussion]. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 308:37–51.Google Scholar
Madosky, J.M., Rubenstein, D.I., Howard, J.J. and Stuska, S. 2010. The effects of immuno-contraception on harem fidelity in a feral horse (Equus caballus) population. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 128:50–56.Google Scholar
Manor, R. and Saltz, D. 2005. Impact of human nuisance disturbance on vigilance and group size of a social ungulate. Ecological Applications 13:1830–1834.Google Scholar
Marchand, P., Garel, M., Bourgoin, G., Dubray, D., Maillard, D. and Loison, A. 2014. Impacts of tourism and hunting on a large herbivore's spatio-temporal behavior in and around a French protected area. Biological Conservation, 177:1–11.Google Scholar
McGhee, K.E., Pintor, L.M. and Bell, A.M. 2013. Reciprocal behavioral plasticity and behavioral types during predator–prey interactions. American Naturalist, 182:704–717.Google Scholar
Morand-Ferron, J. and Quinn, J.L. 2011. Larger groups of passerines are more efficient problem solvers in the wild. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108:15898–15903.Google Scholar
Morris, D. 1957. “Typical intensity” and its relation to the problem of ritualisation. Behaviour, 11:1–12.Google Scholar
Nuñez, C.M.V., Adelman, J.S. and Rubenstein, D.I. 2010. Immunocontraception in wild horses (Equus caballus) extends reproductive cycling beyond the normal breeding season. PLoS ONE. 5(10): e13635.Google Scholar
Rubenstein, D.I. 1980. On the evolution of alternative mating strategies. In Stadden, J.E.R. (Ed.), Limits to Action: The Allocation of Individual Behaviour, pp. 65–100. New York: Academic Press.
Rubenstein, D.I. 1986. Ecology and sociality in horses and zebras. In Rubenstein, D.I. and Wrangham, R.W. (Eds.), Ecological Aspects of Social Evolution, pp. 282–302. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Rubenstein, D.I. 1991. The greenhouse effect and changes in animal behavior: effects on social structure and life-history strategies. In Peters, R. (Ed.), Consequences of Global Warming for Biodiversity, pp. 180–192. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Rubenstein, D.I. 1994. The ecology of female social behavior in horses, zebras, and asses. In Jarman, P. and Rossiter, A. (Eds.), Animal Societies: Individuals, Interactions, and Organization, pp. 13–28. Kyoto: Kyoto University Press.
Rubenstein, D.I. 2010. Ecology, social behavior, and conservation in zebras. In Macedo, R. (Ed.), Advances in the Study Behavior: Behavioral Ecology of Tropical Animals, Vol. 42, pp. 231–258. Oxford: Elsevier Press.
Rubenstein, D.I. 2011. Family equidae (Horses and relatives). In Wilson, D.E. and Mittermeier, R.A. (Eds.), Handbook of Mammals of the World, Vol. 2, Hoofed Mammals, pp. 106–143. Barcelona: Lynx Edicions.
Rubenstein, D.I., Barnett, R.J., Ridgely, R.S. and Klopfer, P.H. 1977. Adaptive advantages of mixed species feeding flocks in Costa Rica. Ibis, 119:10–21.Google Scholar
Rubenstein, D.I. and Hack, M. 2004. Natural and sexual selection and the evolution of multi-level societies: insights from zebras with comparisons to primates. In Kappeler, P. and van Schaik, C.P. (Eds.), Sexual Selection in Primates: New and Comparative Perspectives, pp. 266–279. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Rubenstein, D.I., Sundaresan, S., Fischhoff, I. and Saltz, D. 2007. Social networks in wild asses: comparing patterns and processes among populations. In Stubbe, A., Kaczensky, P., Wesche, K., Samjaa, R. and Stubbe, M. (Eds.), Exploration into the Biological Resources of Mongolia, Vol. 10, pp.159–176. Halle: Martin-Luther-University Halle-Wittenberg.
Rubenstein, D.I.and Nuñez, C. 2009. Sociality and reproductive skew in horses and zebras. In Hager, R.and Jones, C.B. (Eds.), Reproductive Skew in Vertebrates: Proximate and Ultimate Causes, pp. 196–226. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Rubenstein, D.I. and Hack, M.A. 2013. Migration. In Levin, S.A. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Biodiversity, Volume 5, pp. 309–320. Waltham: Academic Press.
Rubenstein, D.I., Sundaresan, S.R., Fischhoff, I.R., Tantipathananandh, C. and Berger-Wolf, T.Y. 2015. Similar but different: dynamic social network analysis highlights fundamental differences between the fission-fusion societies of two equid species, the Onager and Grevy's zebra. PLoS ONE, 10:e0138645.Google Scholar
Rubenstein, D.I., Cao, Q. and Chui, J. 2016. Equids and ecological niches: behavioral and life history variations on a common theme. In Ransom, J. & Kaczensky, P. (Eds.), Wild Equids: Ecology, Conservation, and Management. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.
Rubenstein, D. R. and Lovette, I.J. 2007. Temporal environmental variability drives the evolution of cooperative breeding in birds. Current Biology, 17:1414–1419.Google Scholar
Saltz, D., Rowen, M. and Rubenstein, D.I. 2000. The effect of space-use patterns of reintroduced Asiatic wild ass on effective population size. Conservation Biology, 14:1852–1861.Google Scholar
Saltz, D., Rubenstein, D.I. and White, G.C. 2006. The impact of increased environmental stochasticity, due to climate change on the dynamics of Asiatic wild ass. Conservation Biology, 20:1402–1409.Google Scholar
Schlaepfer, M.A., Runge, M.C. and Sherman, P.W. 2002. Ecological and evolutionary traps. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 17:474–480.Google Scholar
Shultz, S. and Dunbar, R.I.M. 2010. Species differences in executive function correlate with hippocampus volume and neocortex ration across nonhuman primates. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 124:252–260.Google Scholar
Slabbekoorn, H. 2013. Songs of the city: noise-dependent spectral plasticity in the acoustic phenotype of urban birds. Animal Behaviour, 85:1089–1099.Google Scholar
Snell-Rood, E.C. 2013. An overview of the evolutionary causes and consequences of behavioural plasticity. Animal Behaviour, 85:1004–1011.Google Scholar
Smith, J.M. 1982. Evolution and the Theory of Games. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sih, A. 2013. Understanding variation in behavioural responses to human-induced rapid environmental change: a conceptual overview. Animal Behaviour, 85:1077–1088.Google Scholar
Sih, A. and Bell, A.M. 2008. Insights for behavioral ecology from behavioral syndromes. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 38:227–281.Google Scholar
Sol, D., Duncan, R.P., Blackburn, T.M., Cassey, P. and Lefebvre, L. 2005. Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102:5460–5465.Google Scholar
Sol, D., Székely, T., Liker, A. and Lefebvre, L. 2007. Big-brained birds survive better in nature. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274:763–769.Google Scholar
Sol, D., Bacher, S., Reader, S.M. and Lefebvre, L. 2008. Brain size predicts the success of mammal species introduced into novel environments. American Naturalist, 172:S63–S71.Google Scholar
Sundaresan, S.R., Fischhoff, I.R., Dushoff, J. and Rubenstein, D.I. 2007. Network metrics reveal differences in social organization between two fission-fusion species, Grevy's zebra and onager. Oecologia, 151:140–149.Google Scholar
Szyf, M. 2011. The early life social environment and DNA methylation: DNA methylation mediating the long-term impact of social environments early in life. Epigenetics, 6:971–978.Google Scholar
Zhang, Y., Cao, Q.S., Rubenstein, D.I., Zang, S., Songer, M., Leimgruber, P. et al. 2015. Water use patterns of sympatric Przewalski's horse and khulan: interspecific comparison reveals niche differences. PLoS ONE, 10:e0132094.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×