Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T02:15:58.384Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Theory and research questions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 December 2010

Pierre R. Dasen
Affiliation:
Université de Genève
Ramesh C. Mishra
Affiliation:
Banaras Hindu University, Varanasi, India
Get access

Summary

Introduction

“Put the knife to the north side of the plate, and the fork south!” To many readers of this book, this motherly instruction to her child will seem rather bizarre. Why not right and left of the plate? Because this mother is using a “geocentric” frame of spatial reference (FoR) instead of an egocentric one; in other words, she is using a large-scale orientation system (such as cardinal directions, but there are others) to talk about small-scale table-top space inside a room. While it is possible to do this in English, it is hardly ever done and comes across as strange. In some other languages, however, like Balinese, Hindi or Nepali, it would not sound strange at all; in fact, it is standard practice, and could be heard by any child old enough to set the table.

How do children learn to use a geocentric FoR? This question has never been addressed in mainstream developmental psychology, because a geocentric FoR is quite unfamiliar to Western children and psychologists alike. They may use a geocentric frame when navigating in a North American city laid out in a grid pattern, or on interstate highways, or when map reading, but not when talking about the location of objects inside a house. Yet many of the children in Bali, in India or in Nepal where we carried out our research (and in several other locations world-wide studied by Levinson, 2003, and his colleagues) use a geocentric FoR with ease.

Type
Chapter
Information
Development of Geocentric Spatial Language and Cognition
An Eco-cultural Perspective
, pp. 3 - 48
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×