Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T23:00:09.677Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Graphic syntax and representational development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Annie Vinter
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, France
Delphine Picard
Affiliation:
University of Montpellier
Viviane Fernandes
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, Dijon
Chris Lange-Küttner
Affiliation:
London Metropolitan University
Annie Vinter
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, France
Get access

Summary

This chapter focuses specifically on the relationships between syntax and cognitive development, particularly representational development. Vinter, Picard and Fernandes promote the take-home message that changes in drawing behaviour during development result from changes in the size of the cognitive units or mental representations used to plan behaviour, and in the capacity to manage part–whole relationships. This hypothesis is first illustrated by reviewing studies in which children's adherence to the graphic rules when they copy elementary or complex figures is assessed. The authors also examine children's syntactical behaviour at a more global level, characterizing the entire drawing sequences built by children when they produce a drawing. Children's graphic strategies appear to reflect how they conceive of the patterns they reproduce. Task constraints (meaning given to the pattern, type of primes used to enhance specific strategies) contribute to modify their syntactical behaviour, but not uniformly throughout development. A three-step developmental model outlined in the first section of the chapter finds further support in studies dealing with procedural and representational flexibility. Finally, the authors report an original perspective on studying syntactical drawing behaviour, where it is shown that this behaviour can be incidentally modified through directed practice in children as well as in adults. By the way, the results reported by Vinter, Picard and Fernandes reveal the extent to which syntax constitutes a flexible component of drawing behaviour.

Type
Chapter
Information
Drawing and the Non-Verbal Mind
A Life-Span Perspective
, pp. 139 - 158
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akshoomoff, N. A. and Stiles, J. (1995a). Developmental trends in visuospatial analysis and planning: I. Copying a complex figure. Neuropsychology, 9, 364–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akshoomoff, N. A. and Stiles, J. (1995b). Developmental trends in visuospatial analysis and planning: Ⅱ. Memory for a complex figure. Neuropsychology, 9, 378–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlow, C. M., Jolley, R. P., White, D. G. and Galbraith, D. (2003). Rigidity in children's drawings and its relation with representational change. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 124–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, M., Beaumont, A. and Jennett, M. (1985). Some children do sometimes what they have been told to do: task demands and verbal instructions on children's drawings. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 176–87). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Berti, A. E. and Freeman, N. H. (1997). Representational change in resources for pictorial innovations: a three-component analysis. Cognitive Development, 12, 501–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bozeat, S., Ralph, Lambon M. A., Graham, K. S., Patterson, K., Wilkin, H., Rowland, J., Rogers, T. T. and Hodges, J. R. (2003). A duck with four legs: investigating the structure of conceptual knowledge using picture drawing in semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 27–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, G. and Moore, S. (1984). Prior visual inspection and object naming: two factors that enhance hidden-feature inclusion in young children's drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2, 371–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. and Diamond, R. (1977). From piecemeal to configurational representation of faces. Science, 195, 312–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, M. V. (1992). Children's drawings. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Davies, A. M. (1983). Contextual sensitivity in young children's drawings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 478–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dukette, D. and Stiles, J. (1996). Children's analysis of hierarchical patterns: evidence from a similarity judgment task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 103–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodnow, J. and Levine, R. A. (1973). The grammar of action: sequence and syntax in children's copying behaviour. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 82–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingram, N. (1985). Three into two won't go: symbolic and spatial coding processes in young children's drawings. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 231–47). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1990). Constraints on representational change: evidence from children's drawing. Cognition, 34, 57–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1999). Taking development seriously. Human Development, 42, 325–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, Kemler D. G. (1984). The effect of intention on what concepts are acquired. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 23, 734–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemler Nelson, D. G. (1989). The nature and occurrence of holistic processing. In Shepp, B. E. and Ballesteros, S. (eds.), Object perception (pp. 357–86). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lange-Küttner, C. (1997). Development of size modification of human figure drawings in spatial axes systems of varying complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 264–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lange-Küttner, C. (2000). The role of object violations in the development of visual analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 90, 3–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lange-Küttner, C. (2004). More evidence on size modification in spatial axes systems of varying complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 171–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, C., Russell, C. and Berridge, D. (1993). When is a mug not a mug? Effects of content, naming, and instructions on children's drawings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 291–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marot, V. and Vinter, A. (2003). Rôle de la complexité et de l'orientation d'une figure géométrique sur l'application des règles de production graphique chez l'enfant et l'adulte [The role of complexity and orientation of a geometric figure for the application of graphic production rules in the child and adult]. Archives de Psychologie, 70, 175–92.Google Scholar
Marteniuk, R. G., McKenzie, C. L., Jeannerod, M., Athenes, S. and Dugas, C. (1987). Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 365–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mounoud, P. (1988). The ontogenesis of different types of thought: language and motor behaviours as non-specific manifestations. In Weiskrantz, L. (ed.), Thought without language (pp. 25–45). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Mounoud, P. (1996). A recursive transformation of central cognitive mechanisms: the shift from partial to whole representation. In Sameroff, P. and Haith, M. (eds.), Reason and responsibility: the passage through childhood (pp. 85–110). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholls, A. and Kennedy, J. M. (1992). Drawing development: from similarity of features to direction. Child Development, 63, 227–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nihei, Y. (1983). Developmental changes in covert principles for the organization of strokes in drawing and handwriting. Acta Psychologica, 54, 846–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A. and Lieblich, A. (1976). The grammar of action: phrase structure in children's copying. Child Development, 47, 846–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perruchet, P. and Vinter, A. (2002). The self-organizing consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 297–330.Google ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. (1956). The child's conception of space. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Picard, D. and Durand, K. (2005). Are young children's drawings canonically biased?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 48–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (1999). Representational flexibility in children's drawing: effects of age and verbal instructions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 605–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (2005). Development of graphic formulas for the depiction of familiar objects. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 418–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (2006). Decomposing and connecting object representations in 5- to 9-year-old children's drawing behaviour. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 529–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (2007). Relationship between procedural rigidity and inter-representational change in children's drawing behaviour. Child Development, 74, 521–41.Google Scholar
Phillips, W. A., Inall, M. and Lauder, E. (1985). On the discovery, storage and use of graphic descriptions. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 122–34). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schyns, P. G. and Rodet, L. (1997). Categorization creates functional features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 681–96.Google Scholar
Simner, M. L. (1981). The grammar of action and children's printing. Developmental Psychology, 20, 136–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spensley, F. and Taylor, J. (1999). The development of cognitive flexibility: evidence from children's drawings. Human Development, 42, 300–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiles, J. (1995). The early use and development of graphic formulas: two case study reports of graphic formula production by 2- to 3-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 18, 127–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutton, P. J. and Rose, D. H. (1998). The role of strategic visual attention in children's drawing development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 87–107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tada, W. L. and Stiles, J. (1996). Developmental change in children's analysis of spatial patterns. Developmental Psychology, 3, 951–70.Google Scholar
Sommers, P. (1984). Drawing and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinter, A. (1990). Manual imitations and reaching behaviors: an illustration of action control in infancy. In Bard, C., Fleury, M. and Hay, L. (eds.), Development of eye–hand coordination across the life span (pp. 157–87). Columbia, SC: University of South Carolina Press.Google Scholar
Vinter, A. (1994). Hierarchy among graphic production rules: a developmental approach. In Faure, C., Keuss, P., Lorette, G. and Vinter, A. (eds.), Advances in handwriting and drawing: a multidisciplinary approach (pp. 275–88). Paris: Europia.Google Scholar
Vinter, A. (1999). How meaning modifies drawing behaviour in children. Child Development, 70, 33–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinter, A. and Detable, C. (2003). Implicit learning in children and adolescents with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 94–107.2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vinter, A. and Marot, V. (2007). The development of context sensitivity in children's graphic copying strategies. Developmental Psychology, 43, 94–110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vinter, A. and Meulenbroek, R. (1993). The role of manual dominance and visual feedback in circular drawing movements. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 25, 11–37.Google Scholar
Vinter, A. and Perruchet, P. (1999). Isolating unconscious influences: the neutral parameter procedure. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52a, 857–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinter, A. and Perruchet, P. (2000). Implicit learning in children is not related to age: evidence from drawing behaviour. Child Development, 71, 1223–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vinter, A. and Perruchet, P. (2002). Implicit motor learning through observational training in adults and children. Memory and Cognition, 30, 256–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vinter, A. and Picard, D. (1996). Drawing behaviour in children reflects internal representational changes. In Simner, M., Leedham, G. and Thomassen, A. (eds.), Handwriting and drawing research (pp. 171–85). Amsterdam: IOS Press.Google Scholar
Zhi, Z., Thomas, G. V. and Robinson, E. J. (1997). Constraints on representational changes: drawing a man with two heads. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 275–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×