Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T00:43:25.629Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - The interaction of biomechanical and cognitive constraints in the production of children's drawing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Gregory Braswell
Affiliation:
Illinois State University
Karl Rosengren
Affiliation:
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
Chris Lange-Küttner
Affiliation:
London Metropolitan University
Annie Vinter
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, France
Get access

Summary

Research investigating the process of drawing has often been overshadowed by a focus on the end product of this process. However, it is important to acknowledge that various factors that shape the process also impact on the final drawing outcome. In this chapter, Braswell and Rosengren examine how biomechanical, cognitive and contextual factors shape how children and adults draw. For example, the physiology and structure of the fingers, hands and arms often influences the direction in which strokes are produced on a page. Numerous studies demonstrate the effect of handedness on drawing horizontal lines, for instance. Other research has demonstrated that cognitive factors shape where drawers begin figures and the direction in which strokes are produced. In some cases, drawers rely on relatively stable, procedural representations to guide the sequencing and placement of strokes for entire images. Contextual factors, such as writing systems, also play important roles in determining how individuals construct a drawing. They discuss these factors and how they interact using the TASC-based approach, which views development as driven by constraints internal and external to the individual.

drawing is a complex skill that emerges during the second year of life, changes significantly over the course of childhood, and involves both higher-order symbolic processes and a motor system capable of producing a desired representation in the real world. Much of the research on children's drawings has focused on the cognitive aspects of children's drawing, often with an emphasis on the final product of a drawing episode.

Type
Chapter
Information
Drawing and the Non-Verbal Mind
A Life-Span Perspective
, pp. 123 - 138
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barlow, C. M., Jolley, R. P., White, D. G. and Galbraith, D. (2003). Rigidity in children's drawings and its relation with representational change. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 124–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blöte, A. W. and Heijden, P. G. M. (1988). A follow-up study on writing posture and writing movement in young children. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 14, 57–74.Google Scholar
Blöte, A. W., Zielstra, E. M. and Zoetewey, M. W. (1987). Writing posture and writing movement in kindergarten. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 13, 323–41.Google Scholar
Braswell, G. S. and Rosengren, K. S. (2000). Decreasing variability in the development of graphic production. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 153–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braswell, G. S. and Rosengren, K. S. (2002). Interactions between cognitive and biomechanical factors in drawing development: a glimpse into the role of handedness. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 581–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braswell, G. S., Rosengren, K. S. and Pierroutsakos, S. L. (2007). Task constraints on preschool children's grip configurations during drawing. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 216–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connolly, K. and Elliott, J. (1972). The evolution and ontogeny of hand function. In Jones, N. Blurton (ed.), Ethological studies of child behaviour (pp. 329–84). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cox, M. (1992). Children's drawings. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Gesell, A. and Ames, L. B. (1946). The development of directionality in drawing. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 68, 45–61.Google ScholarPubMed
Glenn, S. M., Bradshaw, K. and Sharp, M. (1995). Handedness and the direction and sequencing in children's drawings of people. Educational Psychology, 15, 11–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodnow, J. (1977). Children's drawing. London: Fontana/Open Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodnow, J. J., Friedman, S. L., Bernbaum, M. and Lehman, E. B. (1973). Direction and sequence in copying: the effect of learning to write in English and Hebrew. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 4, 263–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodnow, J. J. and Levine, R. A. (1973). ‘The grammar of action': sequence and syntax in children's copying. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 82–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greer, T. and Lockman, J. J. (1998). Using writing instruments: invariances in young children and adults. Child Development, 69, 888–902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halverson, H. M. (1931). An experimental study of prehension in infants by means of systematic cinema records. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 10, 107–286.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1990). Constraints on representational change: evidence from children's drawing. Cognition, 34, 57–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kellogg, R. (1969). Analyzing children's art. Palo Alto, CA: Mayfield.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M., Heldmeyer, K. H. and Locklear, E. P. (1977). Children's drawings as data about internal representations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 191–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieblich, A., Ninio, A. and Kugelmass, S. (1975). Developmental trends in directionality of drawing in Jewish and Arab Israeli children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 6, 504–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martlew, M. (1992). Pen grips: their relationship to letter/word formation and literacy knowledge in children starting school. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 23, 165–85.Google Scholar
Meulenbroek, R. G. J., Vinter, A. and Mounoud, P. (1993). Development of start rotation principle in circle production. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 307–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newell, K. M. (1986). Constraints on the development of coordination. In Wade, W. and Whiting, H. T. A. (eds.), Motor development in children: aspects of coordination and control (pp. 341–60). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhof.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, W. A., Hobbs, S. B. and Pratt, F. R. (1978). Intellectual realism in children's drawings of cubes. Cognition, 6, 15–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, W. A., Inall, M. and Lauder, E. (1985). On the discovery, storage and use of graphic descriptions. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 122–34). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (2005). Development of graphic formulas for the depiction of familiar objects. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 418–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (2006). Decomposing and connecting object representations in 5- to 9-year-old children's drawing behaviour. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 529–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenbaum, D. A. (2005). The Cinderella of psychology: the neglect of motor control in the science of mental life and behaviour. American Psychologist, 60, 308–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenbloom, L. and Horton, M. E. (1971). The maturation of fine prehension in young children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 13, 3–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosengren, K. S. and Braswell, G. S. (2001). Variability in children's reasoning. In Reese, H. W. and Kail, R. (eds.), Advances in child development and behaviour (pp. 1–40). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rosengren, K. S. and Braswell, G. S. (2003). Constraints and the development of children's drawing and writing skills. In Savelsbergh, G. J. P., Davids, K., Kamp, J. and Bennett, S. (eds.), Development of movement coordination in children: applications in the fields of ergonomics, health sciences, and sport (pp. 56–74). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Rosengren, K. S., DeGuzman, D. and Pierroutsakos, S. L. (1995). Why can't Johnny draw a square square? Task constraints on children's grip configurations during drawing. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, Indiana.
Rosengren, K. S. and Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2000). A TASC based approach to perceptual motor development. Paper presented at the Motor Development Research Consortium, Bowling Green, Ohio.
Rosengren, K. S., Savelsbergh, G. J. P. and Kamp, J. (2003). Development and learning: a TASC-based perspective on the acquisition of perceptual-motor behaviors. Infant Behaviour and Development, 26, 473–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saida, Y. and Miyashita, M. (1979). Development of fine motor skill in children: manipulation of a pencil in young children ages 2 to 6 years old. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 5, 104–13.Google Scholar
Sasson, R., Nimmo-Smith, I. and Wing, A. M. (1986). An analysis of children's penholds. In Kao, H. S. R., Galen, G. P. and Hoosian, R. (eds.), Graphonomics: contemporary research in handwriting (pp. 93–106). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Scheirs, J. G. M. (1990). Relationships between the direction of movements and handedness in children. Neuropsychologia, 28, 742–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds: the process of change in children's thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stiles, J. (1995). The early use and development of graphic formulas: two case study reports of graphic formula production by 2- to 3-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 18, 127–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E. and Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thomassen, A. J. W. M. (1992). Interaction of cognitive and biomechanical factors in the organization of graphic movements. In Stelmach, G. E. and Requin, J. (eds.), Tutorials in motor behaviour (Vol. 2, pp. 249–61). Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier.Google Scholar
Thomassen, A. J. W. M., Meulenbroek, R. G. J. and Hoofs, M. P. E. (1992). Economy and anticipation in graphic stroke sequences. Human Movement Science, 11, 71–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomassen, A. J. W. M. and Teulings, H. H. M. (1979). The development of directional preference in writing movements. Visible Language, 13, 299–313.Google Scholar
Thomassen, A. J. W. M. and Teulings, H. H. M. (1983). The development of handwriting. In Martlew, M. (ed.), The psychology of written language (pp. 179–213). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Thomassen, A. J. W. M., Tibosch, H. J. C. M. and Maarse, F. J. (1989). The effect of context on stroke direction and stroke order in handwriting. In Plamondon, R., Suen, C. Y. and Simner, M. L. (eds.), Computer recognition and human production of handwriting (pp. 213–30). Singapore: World Scientific.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Roon, D., Van der Kamp, J. and Steenbergen, B. (2003). Constraints in children's learning to use spoons. In Savelsbergh, G., Davids, K., Kamp, J. and Bennett, S. J. (eds.), Development of movement co-ordination in children: applications in the fields of ergonomics, health sciences and sport (pp. 75–93). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Van Sommers, P. (1983). The conservatism of children's drawing strategies: at what level does stability persist? In Rogers, D. and Sloboda, J. A. (eds.), The acquisition of symbolic skills (pp. 65–70). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Sommers, P. (1984). Drawings and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Vinter, A. (1999). How meaning modifies drawing behaviour in children. Child Development, 70, 33–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willats, J. (1977). How children learn to draw realistic pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 367–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, T. H. and Kao, H. S. R. (1991). The development of drawing principles in Chinese. In Wann, J., Wing, A. M. and S⊘vik, N. (eds.), Development of graphic skills (pp. 93–112). New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Zhi, Z., Thomas, G. V. and Robinson, E. J. (1997). Constraints on representational change: drawing a man with two heads. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 275–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziviani, J. (1982). Children's prehension while writing – a pilot investigation. British Journal of Occupational Therapy, 45, 306–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziviani, J. (1983). Qualitative changes in dynamic tripod grip between seven and 14 years of age. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 25, 535–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Ziviani, J. and Elkins, J. (1986). Effect of pencil grip on handwriting speed and legibility. Educational Review, 38, 247–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×