Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T17:52:25.469Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Spatial and symbolic codes in the development of three-dimensional graphic representation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Maria A. Tallandini
Affiliation:
University College London and Università de Trieste
Luisa Morassi
Affiliation:
Università de Trieste
Chris Lange-Küttner
Affiliation:
London Metropolitan University
Annie Vinter
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, France
Get access

Summary

Tallandini investigates the dissociation of the spatial code and the semantic (or symbolic) code in drawing. She tested a model of drawing which assumes two pathways of attention to the real object. Children could either draw only paying attention to the surface properties (spatial code) which should enhance visually realistic drawing, or they would draw more meaningful objects (semantic code) where attention is diverted between spatial and semantic aspects, and thus the more early intellectually realistic drawing style should pervade. Tallandini asked children and adults to copy aggregates of cubes in different conditions, (1) plain and purely geometrical, (2) with features of a doll, (3) with a part (the head) rotated, (4) with the entire figure rotated. Using a 7-point Q3DS-scale, she found that the more complex the spatial aspects, i.e. object orientation and position, and the less semantic aspects, the better the quality of the level of the projective system, in both children and adults. It is concluded that semantic content, the ‘what’, interfered with the construction of spatially complex figure constructions, the ‘how’, a clear dissociation between the spatial and the semantic code in drawing.

from a historical perspective, Piaget (1977) identified two inextricable aspects of knowledge (Feldman, 2000): the ‘figurative’, which concerns an object's physical reality (e.g. colour, texture, position, size, weight, etc.) and the ‘operative’, which corresponds to the ways in which that object is understood.

Type
Chapter
Information
Drawing and the Non-Verbal Mind
A Life-Span Perspective
, pp. 217 - 238
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arnheim, R. (1974). Art and visual perception: a psychology of the creative eye. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, M. D. and Light, P. H. (1976). Symbolism and intellectual realism in children's drawings. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, 198–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremner, J. G. and Batten, A. (1991). Sensitivity to viewpoint in children's drawings of objects and relations between objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 52, 375–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, J. G., Morse, R., Hughes, S. and Andreasen, G. (2000). Relations between drawing cubes and copying line diagrams of cubes in 7 to 10-years-old children. Child Development, 71, 621–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cannoni, E. (2003). Il disegno dei bambini [The drawings of children]. Rome: Carocci.Google Scholar
Case, R. and Okamoto, Y. (1996). The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children's thought. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD), 61, 1–2.Google Scholar
Chen, M. J. and Cook, M. (1984). Representational drawings of solid objects by young children. Perception, 13, 377–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, M. V. and Perara, J. (1998). Children's observational drawings. A nine-point scale for scoring drawings of a cube. Educational Psychology, 18, 309–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, A. M. (1985). The canonical bias: young children's drawing of familiar objects. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 202–13). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Feldman, D. H. (2000). Figurative and operative processes in the development of artistic talent. Human Development, 43, 60–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, N. H. (1980). Strategies of representation in young children: analysis of spatial skills and drawing processes. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.) (1985). Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Golomb, C. (2002). Child arts in context: a cultural and comparative perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gombrich, E. H. (1960). Art and Illusion: a study in the psychology of pictorial representation. London: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Goodnow, J. J. (1977). Children's drawing. London: Open Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingram, N. (1985). Three into two won't go: symbolic and spatial coding processes in young children's drawings. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 231–47). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lange-Küttner, C. (2000). The role of object violation in the development of visual analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 1299–1310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lange-Küttner, C., Kerzmann, A. and Heckhausen, J. (2002). The emergence of visually realistic contour in the drawing of the human figure. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 439–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, M. and Bremner, J. G. (1987). The representation of depth in children's drawings of a table. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39A, 479–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luquet, G. H. (1927). Le dessin enfantin [Children's drawings]. Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.Google Scholar
Morassi, L. and Tallandini, M. A. (2006). Graphic representation: a model of interaction between the spatial and the symbolic component. Paper presented at the XIVth Symposium of Perception and Cognition. Trieste, Italy.
Morra, S., Moizo, C. and Scopesi, A. (1988). Working memory (or the M operator) and the planning of children's drawings. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 41–73.Google Scholar
Nicholls, A. L. and Kennedy, J. M. (1992). Drawing development: from similarity of features to direction. Child Development, 63, 227–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicholls, A. L. and Kennedy, J. M. (1995). Foreshortening in cube drawings by children and adults. Perception, 24, 1443–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1977). The role of action in the development of thinking. In Overton, F. W. and Gallagher, J. MacCarthy (eds.), Knowledge and Development. Vol. 1, Advances in Research and Theory (pp. 17–42). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Phillips, W. A., Hobbs, S. B. and Pratt, F. R. (1978). Intellectual realism in children's drawings of cubes. Cognition, 6, 15–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rakover, S. S. (2002). Featural vs configurational information in faces: a conceptual and empirical analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 1–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reith, E. (1990). Development of representational awareness and performance in drawing production. Archives de Psychologie, 58, 369–79.Google Scholar
Rothi, L. J., Ochipa, C. and Heilman, K. M. (1991). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 443–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheppard, E., Ropar, D. and Mitchell, P. (2005). The impact of meaning and dimensionality on the accuracy of children's copying. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 365–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tallandini, M. A. and Morassi, L. (2002). Modalità di rappresentazione proiettiva nel disegno infantile [Types of projective representation in children's drawing]. In Di Stefano, G. and Vianello, R. (eds.), Psicologia dello sviluppo e problemi educativi [Developmental psychology and educational issues]. Florence: Giunti.Google Scholar
Tallandini, M. A. and Morassi, L. (2005). The relation between spatial and symbolic components in children's drawing. Paper presented at the 12th European Conference on Developmental Psychology, 24–28 August.
Tallandini, M. A., Morassi, L. and Gerbino, W. (1999). The development of representation of tridimensional objects and the relation between spatial and symbolic code. Biennal Meeting of Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD), Albuquerque, New Mexico.Google Scholar
Tallandini, M. A. and Valentini, P. (1990). Lo sviluppo del disegno infantile: teorie studiali [The development of drawing in children: theoretical studies]. Età Evolutiva, 37, 92–105.Google Scholar
Tallandini, M. A. and Valentini, P. (1991). Strategie rappresentative nel disegno infantile di un oggetto sociale. [Representational strategies in the drawing of a social object in children]. In Stefano, G. Di and Tallandini, M.A (eds.), Meccanismi e processi di sviluppo: l'interpretazione postpiagetiana [Mechanisms and processes of development: a post-Piagetian perspective]. Milan: Cortina.Google Scholar
Tallandini, M. A. and Varesano, E. (2003). Il processo produttivo nella rappresentazione grafica [The production processes in graphic representations]. Età Evolutiva, 76, 64–71.Google Scholar
Tessari, A. and Rumiati, R. I. (2004). The strategic control of multiple routes in imitation to actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 1107–16.Google ScholarPubMed
Toomela, A. (1999). Drawing development: stages in the representation of a cube and a cylinder. Child Development, 70, 1141–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toomela, A. (2002). Drawing as a verbally mediated activity: a study of relationships between verbal, motor, and visuo-spatial skills and drawing in children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26, 234–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommers, P. (1984). Drawing and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinter, A. (1999). How meaning modifies drawing behaviour in children. Child Development, 70, 33–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willats, J. (1977). How children learn to draw realistic pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 367–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willats, J. (1981). What do the marks in the picture stand for? The child's acquisition of systems on transformation and denotation. Review of Research in Visual Arts Education, 13, 18–33.Google Scholar
Willats, J. (1985). Drawing systems revisited: the role of denotation systems in children's figure drawings. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 78–100). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Willats, J. (2003). Optical laws or symbolic rules? The dual nature of pictorial systems. In Hecht, H. and Schwartz, R. (eds.), Looking into pictures: an interdisciplinary approach to pictorial space (pp. 125–43). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Willats, J. (2005). Making sense of children's drawing. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×