Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T14:42:30.240Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2014

Julian G. Elliott
Affiliation:
University of Durham
Elena L. Grigorenko
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Dyslexia Debate , pp. 183 - 258
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2014

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1000 Genomes Project Consortium. (2010). A map of human genome variation from population-scale sequencing. Nature, 467, 1061–1073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ackerman, P. T., Holloway, C. A., Youngdahl, P. L., & Dykman, R. A. (2001). The double-deficit theory of reading disability does not fit all. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 16, 152–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Lee, J. (2010). Kindergarten predictors of second vs. eighth grade reading comprehension impairments. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 332–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Agurs-Collins, T., Khoury, M. J., Simon-Morton, D., Olster, D. H., Harris, J. R., & Milner, J. A. (2008). Public health genomics: translating obesity genomics research into population health benefits. Obesity, 16, S85–S94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahissar, M. (2007). Dyslexia and the anchoring-deficit hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 458–465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahissar, M., & Oganian, Y. (2008). Response to Ziegler: The anchor is in the details. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 245–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahissar, M., Protopapas, A., Reid, M., & Merzenich, M. M. (2000). Auditory processing parallels reading abilities in adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 6832–6837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Al Otaiba, S., Folsom, S. J., Schatschneider, C., Wanzek., J., Greulich, L., Meadows, J., et al. (2011). Predicting first-grade reading performance from kindergarten response to Tier 1 instruction. Exceptional Children, 77, 453–470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Al Otaiba, S., & Fuchs, D. (2002). Characteristics of children who are unresponsive to early literacy intervention: A review of the literature. Remedial and Special Education, 23, 300–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Al Otaiba, S., & Fuchs, D. (2006). Who are the young children for whom best practices in reading are ineffective? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 414–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albon, E., Adi, Y., & Hyde, C. (2008). The effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of colored filters for reading disability: A systematic review. Birmingham, AL: University of Birmingham Department of Public Health and Epidemiology.Google Scholar
Aleci, C., Piana, G., Piccoli, M., & Bertolini, M. (2012). Developmental dyslexia and spatial relationship perception. Cortex, 48(4), 466–476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allor, J. H., Mathes, P. G., Roberts, J. K., Jones, F. G., & Champlin, T. M. (2010). Teaching students with moderate intellectual disabilities to read: An experimental examination of a comprehensive reading intervention. Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 45, 3–22.Google Scholar
Alloway, T. P., Bibile, V., & Lau, G. (2013). Computerized working memory training: Can it lead to gains in cognitive skills in students? Computers in Human Behavior, 29(3), 632–638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Kirkwood, H. J., & Elliott, J. G. (2009). The cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with low working memory. Child Development, 80, 606–621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Al-Yagon, M., Cavendish, W., Cornoldi, C., Fawcett, A. J., Grünke, M., Hung, L., Jiménez, J. E., Karande, S., van Kraayenoord, C. E., Lucangeli, D., Margalit, M., Montague, M., Sholapurwala, R., Sideridis, G., Tressoldi, P. E., & Vio, C. (2013). The proposed changes for DSM-5 for SLD and ADHD: International perspectives – Australia, Germany, Greece, India, Israel, Italy, Spain, Taiwan, United Kingdom, and United States. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(1), 58–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Academy of Pediatrics (Section on Ophthalmology & Council on Children with Disabilities, Ophthalmology, American Academy of Ophthalmology, American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus, & American Association of Certified Orthoptists). (2009). Learning disabilities, dyslexia, and vision. Pediatrics, 124, 837–844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Academy of Pediatrics. (1982). The doman-delacato treatment of neurologically handicapped children: A policy statement by the American Academy of Pediatrics. Pediatrics, 70, 810–812.Google Scholar
Amitay, S., Ben-Yehudah, G., Banai, K., & Ahissar, M. (2002). Disabled readers suffer from visual and auditory impairments but not from a specific magnocellular deficit. Brain, 125, 2272–2285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amitay, S., Ben-Yehudah, G., Banai, K., & Ahissar, M. (2003). Visual magnocellular deficits in dyslexia: Reply to the Editor. Brain, 126, e3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amtmann, D., Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (2007). Mixture growth models of RAN and RAS row by row: Insight into the reading system at work over time. Reading and Writing, 20, 785–813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anastasiou, G., & Kauffman, J. M. (2011). A social constructionist approach to disability: Implications for special education. Exceptional Children, 77, 367–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, K. (2000, June 18). The Reading Wars – A look at the report of the National Reading Panel, “Teaching Children to Read,” and the debate over what reading strategy works best in the classroom, Los Angeles Times, p. 5.
Andrews, J. S., Ben-Shachar, M., Yeatman, J. D., Flom, L. L., Luna, B., & Feldman, H. M. (2010). Reading performance correlates with white-matter properties in preterm and term children. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 52, 505–506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angrilli, A., Elbert, T., Cusumano, S., Stegagno, L., & Rockstroh, B. (2003). Temporal dynamics of linguistic processes are reorganized in aphasics’ cortex: An EEG mapping study. Neuroimage, 20, 657–666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angrilli, A., & Spironelli, C. (2005). Cortical plasticity of language measured by EEG in a case of anomic aphasia. Brain and Language, 95, 32–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archer, A. L., Gleason, M. M., & Vachon, V. L. (2003). Decoding and fluency: Foundation skills for struggling older readers. Learning Disability Quarterly, 26, 89–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnoutse, C., van Leeuwe, J., & Verhoeven, L. (2005). Early literacy from a longitudinal perspective. Educational Review and Research, 11, 253–275.Google Scholar
Arns, M., Peters, S., Breteler, R., & Verhoeven, L. (2007). Different brain activation patterns in dyslexic children: evidence from EEG power and coherence patterns for the double-deficit theory of dyslexia. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 175–190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arrow, A. W., & Tunmer, W. E. (2012). Contemporary reading acquisition theory: The conceptual basis for differentiated reading instruction. In Suggate, S. & Reese, E. (Eds.), Contemporary debates in childhood education and development (pp. 241–249). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ashton, C. (1996). In defence of discrepancy definitions of specific learning difficulties. Educational Psychology in Practice, 12, 131–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashton, C. (1997). SpLD, discrepancies and dyslexia: A response to Solity and the Stanoviches. Educational Psychology in Practice, 13, 9–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Au, A., & Lovegrove, W. (2001). Temporal processing ability in above average and average readers. Perception & Psychophysics, 63(1), 148–155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avramopoulos, D. (2010). Genetics of psychiatric disorders methods: Molecular approaches. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 33, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aylward, E., Richards, T., Berninger, V., Nagy, W., Field, K., Grimme, A., et al. (2003). Instructional treatment associated with changes in brain activation in children with dyslexia. Neurology, 61, 212–219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayres, A. J. (1963). The development of perceptual-motor abilities: A theoretical basis for treatment of dysfunction. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 17, 221–225.Google ScholarPubMed
Ayres, A. J. (1979). Sensory integration and the child. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Bacon, A. M., Parmentier, F. B., & Barr, P. (2013). Visuospatial memory in dyslexia: Evidence for strategic deficits. Memory, 21(2), 189–209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Badcock, N. A., Hogben, J. H., & Fletcher, J. F. (2008). No differential attentional blink in dyslexia after controlling for baseline sensitivity. Vision Research, 48, 1497–1502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. London: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 5–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–90). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Badian, N. A. (1997). Dyslexia and the double deficit hypothesis. Annals of Dyslexia, 47, 69–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badzakova-Trajkov, G., Hamm, J. P., & Waldie, K. E. (2005). The effects of redundant stimuli on visuospatial processing in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 43, 473–478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balow, B. (1996). Perceptual-motor activities in the treatment of severe reading disability. The Reading Teacher, 50, 88–97.Google Scholar
Banai, K., & Ahissar, M. (2004). Poor frequency discrimination probes dyslexics with particularly impaired working memory. Audiology & Neuro-otology, 9, 328–340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banai, K., & Ahissar, M. (2010). On the importance of anchoring and the consequences of its impairment in dyslexia. Dyslexia, 16, 240–257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barela, J. A., Dias, J. L., Godoi, D., Viana, A. R., & de Freitas, P. B. (2011). Postural control and automaticity in dyslexic children: The relationship between visual information and body sway. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 1814–1821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkovich, A. J., & Kuzniecky, R. I. (2000). Gray matter heterotopia. Neurology, 55, 1603–1608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barth, A. E., Denton, C. A., Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., Cirino, P. T., Francis, D. J., et al. (2010). A test of the cerebellar hypothesis of dyslexia in adequate and inadequate responders to reading intervention. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 526–536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, T. C., Luciano, M., Montgomery, G. W., Wright, M. J., & Martin, N. G. (2011). Genes for a component of the language acquisition mechanism: ROBO1 polymorphisms associated with phonological buffer deficit. Behavior Genetics, 41, 50–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beach, K. D., & O’Connor, R. E. (in press). Early response-to-intervention measures and criteria as predictors of reading disability in the beginning of third grade. Journal of Learning Disabilities.
Beaton, A. A. (1997). The relation of planum temporale asymmetry and morphology of the corpus callosum to handedness, gender, and dyslexia: A review of the evidence. Brain and Language, 60, 255–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaton, A. A. (2004). Dyslexia, reading and the brain: A sourcebook of psychological and biological research. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beattie, R. L., & Manis, F. R. (in press). The relationship between prosodic perception, phonological awareness and vocabulary in emergent literacy. Journal of Research in Reading.
Beattie, R. L., & Manis, F. R. (2012). Rise time perception in children with reading and combined reading and language difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(3), 200–209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaulieu, C., Plewes, C., Paulson, L. A., Roy, D., Snook, L., Concha, L., et al. (2005). Imaging brain connectivity in children with diverse reading ability. Neuroimage, 25(4), 1266–1271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begeny, J. C., Mitchell, R. C., Whitehouse, M. H., Samuels, F. H., & Stage, S. A. (2011). Effects of the HELPS reading fluency program when implemented by classroom teachers with low-performing second-grade students. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 26, 122–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bejerano, G., Lowe, C. B., Ahituv, N., King, B., Siepel, A., Salama, S. R., et al. (2006). A distal enhancer and an ultraconserved exon are derived from a novel retroposon. Nature, 441, 87–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, T. K. (1990). Rapid sequential processing in dyslexic and ordinary readers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 71, 1155–1159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellocchi, S., Muneaux, M., Bastien-Toniazzo, M., & Ducrot, S. (2013). I can read it in your eyes: What eye movements tell us about visuo-attentional processes in developmental dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 452–460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benassi, M., Simonelli, L., Giovagnoli, S., & Bolzani, R. (2010). Coherence motion perception in developmental dyslexia: A meta-analysis of behavioral studies. Dyslexia, 16, 341–357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beneventi, H., Tønnessen, F. E., Ersland, L., & Hugdahl, K. (2010). Working memory deficit in dyslexia: Behavioral and fMRI evidence. International Journal of Neuroscience, 120, 51–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benner, G. J., Nelson, J. R., Stage, S. A., & Ralston, N. C. (2011). The influence of fidelity of implementation on the reading outcomes of middle school students experiencing reading difficulties. Remedial and Special Education, 32, 79–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Shachar, M., Dougherty, R. F., & Wandell, B. A. (2007). White matter pathways in reading. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17, 258–270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-Yehudah, G., & Ahissar, M. (2004). Sequential spatial frequency discrimination is consistently impaired among adult dyslexics. Vision Research, 44, 1047–1063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berent, I, Vaknin-Nusbaum, V., Balaban, E., & Galaburda, A. M. (2012). Dyslexia impairs speech recognition but can spare phonological competence. PLoS ONE, 7, e44875.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkeley, S., Bender, W. N., Peaster, L. G., & Saunders, L. (2009). Implementation of response to intervention. A snapshot of progress. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 85–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berlin, R. (1887). Eine besondre Art der Wortblindheit.
Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (1994). Redefining learning disabilities: Moving beyond aptitude–achievement discrepancies to failure to respond to validated treatment protocols. In Lyon, G. R. (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 163–183). Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Nagy, W., & Carlisle, J. (2010). Growth in phonological, orthographic, and morphological awareness in grades 1 to 6. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 39, 141–163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berninger, V. W., Nielsen, K. H., Abbott, R. D., Wijsman, E., & Raskind, W. (2008). Gender differences in severity of writing and reading disabilities. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 151–172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berninger, V. W., Raskind, W., Richards, T., Abbott, R., & Stock, P. (2008). A multidisciplinary approach to understanding developmental dyslexia within working-memory architecture: Genotypes, phenotypes, brain, and instruction. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 707–744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Betjemann, R. S., Willcutt, E. G., Olson, R. K., Keenan, J. M., DeFries, J. C., & Wadsworth, S. J. (2008). Word reading and reading comprehension: Stability, overlap and independence. Reading and Writing, 21, 539–558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biancarosa, G., & Snow, C. E. (2006). Reading next: A vision for action and research in middle and high school literacy: A report to the Carnegie Corporation of New York (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellent Education.Google Scholar
Bianco, M., Bressoux, P., Doyen, A., Lambert, E., Lima, L., Pellenq, C., et al. (2010). Early training in oral comprehension and phonological skills: Results of a three-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 211–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. (2006). Dyslexia: What's the problem? [Comment/Reply]. Developmental science, 9, 256–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Uncommon understanding. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (2001). Genetic influences on language impairment and literacy problems in children: Same or different? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 189–198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M. (2002). Cerebellar abnormalities in developmental dyslexia: Cause, correlate or consequence?Cortex, 38, 491–498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). Curing dyslexia and ADHD by training motor coordination: Miracle or myth?Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 43, 653–655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M. (2013). Research Review: Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 2012 – Neuroscientific studies of intervention for language impairment in children: interpretive and methodological problems. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 54(3), 247–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders, and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 1027–1050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., Mcdonald, D., Bird, S., & Hayiou-Thomas, M. E. (2009). Children who read accurately despite language impairment: Who are they and how do they do it? Child Development, 80, 593–605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130, 858–886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blachman, B. A. (2000). Phonological awareness. In Kamil, M. L., Mosenthal, P. B., Pearson, P. D., & Barr, R. (Eds.), Handbook of word recognition research (Vol. III, pp. 483–502). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Blachman, B. A., Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D., Clonan, S. M., Shaywitz, B. A., et al. (2004). Effects of intensive reading remediation for second and third graders and a 1-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 444–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, J., & Hoeft, F. (2012). Prediction of children's reading skills: Understanding the interplay among environment, brain, and behavior. In Benasich, A. A. & Fitch, R. H. (Eds.), Developmental dyslexia: Early precursors, neurobehavioral markers, and biological substrates (pp. 191–207). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Black, J. M., Tanaka, H., Stanley, L., Nagamine, M., Zakerani, N., et al. (2012). Maternal history of reading difficulty is associated with reduced language-related gray matter in beginning readers. Neuroimage, 59(3), 3021–3032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackmore, S. (1999). The meme machine. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Blau, V., Reithler, J., van Atteveldt, N., Seitz, J., Gerretsen, P., Goebel, R., et al. (2010). Deviant processing of letters and speech sounds as proximate cause of reading failure: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of dyslexic children. Brain, 133, 868–879.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blau, V., van Atteveldt, N., Ekkebus, M., Goebel, R., & Blomert, L. (2009). Reduced neural integration of letters and speech sounds links phonological and reading deficits in adult dyslexia. Current Biology, 19, 503–508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blythe, P. (1992). A physical approach to resolving specific learning difficulties. Chester: Institute for Neuro-Physiological Psychology.Google Scholar
Boden, C., & Giaschi, D. (2007). M-Stream deficits and reading-related visual processes in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 346–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boets, B., De Smedt, B., Cleuren, L., Vandewalle, E., Wouters, J., & Ghesquire, P. (2010). Towards a further characterization of phonological and literacy problems in Dutch-speaking children with dyslexia. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 5–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boets, B., Ghesquière, P., van Wieringen, A., & Wouters, J. (2007). Speech perception in preschoolers at family risk for dyslexia: Relations with low-level auditory processing and phonological ability. Brain and Language, 101, 19–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boets, B., Vandermosten, M., Poelmans, H., Luts, H., Wouters, J., & Ghesquière, P. (2011). Preschool impairments in auditory processing and speech perception uniquely predict future reading problems. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 560–570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boets, B., Wouters, J., van Wieringen, A., & Ghesquière, P. (2007). Auditory processing, speech perception and phonological ability in preschool children at high-risk of dyslexia: A longitudinal study of the auditory temporal processing theory. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1608–1620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogliotti, C., Serniclaes, W., Messaoud-Galusi, S., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2008). Discrimination of speech sounds by children with dyslexia: Comparisons with chronological age and reading level. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101, 137–155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonifacci, P., & Snowling, M. J. (2008). Speed of processing and reading disability: A cross-linguistic investigation of dyslexia and borderline intellectual functioning. Cognition, 107, 999–1017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, J. R., Bebko, G., Burman, D. D., & Bitan, T. (2007). Children with reading disorder show modality independent brain abnormalities during semantic tasks. Neuropsychologia, 45, 775–783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosse, M. L., Tainturier, M. J., & Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104, 198–230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosse, M. L., & Valdois, S. (2009). Influence of the visual attention span on child reading performance: A cross-sectional study. Journal of Research in Reading, 32, 230–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, P. G., Sunseth, K., & Golden, J. (1999). The route between rapid naming and reading progress. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 31–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 5, 69–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read – A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brambati, S. M., Termine, C., Ruffino, M., Danna, M., Lanzi, G., Stella, G., et al. (2006). Neuropsychological deficits and neural dysfunction in familial dyslexia. Brain Research, 1, 174–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brambati, S. M., Termine, C., Ruffino, M., Stella, G., Fazio, F., Cappa, S. F., et al. (2004). Regional reductions of gray matter volume in familial dyslexia. Neurology, 63, 742–745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bramlett, R. K., Murphy, J. J., Johnson, J., & Wallingsford, L. (2002). Contemporary practices in school psychology: A national survey of roles and referral problems. Psychology in the Schools, 39, 327–335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Branum-Martin, L., Fletcher, J. M., & Stuebing, K. K. (2013). Classification and identification of reading and math disabilities: The special case of comorbidity. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(6), 490–499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brem, S., Bach, S., Kucian, K., Guttorm, T. K., Martin, E., Lyytinen, H., et al. (2010). Brain sensitivity to print emerges when children learn letter–speech sound correspondences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 7939–7944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breteler, M. H. M., Arns, M., Peters, S., Giepmans, I., & Verhoeven, J. (2010). Improvements in spelling after QEEG-based neurofeedback in dyslexia: A randomized controlled treatment study. Applied Psychophysiological Biofeedback, 35, 5–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breznitz, Z., & Misra, M. (2003). Speed of processing of the visual–orthographic and auditory–phonological systems in adult dyslexics: The contribution of “asynchrony” to word recognition deficits. Brain & Language, 85, 486–502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
British Psychological Society. (1999). Dyslexia, literacy and psychological assessment: Report by a Working Party of the Division of Educational and Child Psychology of the British Psychological Society. Leicester: British Psychological Society.Google Scholar
Brkanac, Z., Chapman, N. H., Igo, R. P. J., Matsushita, M. M., Nielsen, K., Berninger, V. W., et al. (2008). Genome scan of a nonword repetition phenotype in families with dyslexia: Evidence for multiple loci. Behavior Genetics, 38, 462–475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, G. (1984). The teaching of silent reading to beginners. In Brooks, G. & Pugh, A. K. (Eds.), Studies in the history of reading (pp. 85–96). London and Reading: Centre for the Teaching of Reading, University of Reading and U.K. Reading Association.Google Scholar
Brooks, G. (2007). What works for pupils with literacy difficulties? The effectiveness of intervention schemes. London: Department for Education and Skills.Google Scholar
Brown, W. E., Eliez, S., Menon, V., Rumsey, J. M., White, C. D., & Reiss, A. L. (2001). Preliminary evidence of widespread morphological variations of the brain in dyslexia. Neurology, 56, 781–783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown Waesche, J. S., Schatschneider, C., Maner, J. K., Ahmed, Y., & Wagner, R. K. (2011). Examining agreement and longitudinal stability among traditional and RTI-based definitions of reading disability using the affected-status agreement statistic. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44, 296–307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruder, J., Leppänen, P. H. T., Bartling, J., Csépe, V., Démonet, J., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2011). Children with dyslexia reveal abnormal native language representations: Evidence from a study of mismatch negativity. Psychophysiology, 48, 1107–1118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruno, J. L., Lu, Z. L., & Manis, F. R. (2013). Phonological processing is uniquely associated with neuro-metabolic concentration. NeuroImage, 67, 175–181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunswick, N., McCrory, E., Price, C. J., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics: A search for Wernicke's Wortschatz?Brain, 122, 1901–1917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, P. E., & Bradley, L. (1985). Children's reading problems: Psychology and education. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bryant, P. E., Maclean, L., Bradley, L., & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection, and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bull, L. (2009). Survey of complementary and alternative therapies used by children with specific learning difficulties (dyslexia). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44(2), 224–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buonincontri, R., Bache, I., Silahtaroglu, A., Elbro, C., Veber Nielsen, A.-M., Ullmann, R., et al. (2011). A cohort of balanced reciprocal translocations associated with dyslexia: identification of two putative candidate genes at DYX1. Behavior Genetics, 41, 125–133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burden, R. (2008). Is dyslexia necessarily associated with negative feelings of self-worth? A review and implications for future research. Dyslexia, 14, 188–196.Google ScholarPubMed
Burnett, J. R. (1998, October 19). Phonics controversy in Texas [Radio broadcast]. Washington, DC: National Public Radio.
Burt, C. (1937). The backward child. London: University of London Press.Google Scholar
Butterworth, B., & Kovas, Y. (2013). Understanding neurocognitive developmental disorders can improve education for all. Science, 340, 300–305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Büttner, G., & Hasselhorn, M. (2011). Learning disabilities: Debates on definitions, causes, subtypes, and responses. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 58, 75–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B. (2011). Evaluating the role of phonological factors in early literacy development: Insights from experimental and behavior-genetic studies. In Brady, S. A., Braze, D., & Fowler, C. A. (Eds.), Explaining individual differences in reading: Theory and evidence (pp. 175–195). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, B., Coventry, W. L., Olson, R. K., Hulslander, J., Wadsworth, S., Defries, J., et al. (2008). A behaviour-genetic analysis of orthographic learning, spelling and decoding. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 8–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B., Coventry, W. L., Olson, R. K., Samuelsson, S., Corley, R., Willcutt, E. G., et al. (2009). Genetic and environmental influences on aspects of literacy and language in early childhood: Continuity and change from preschool to Grade 2. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22, 219–236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, B., Delaland, C., Fielding-Barnsley, R., & Quain, P. (2002). Longitudinal twins study of early reading development in three countries: Preliminary results. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 49–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calfee, R. C., & Drum, P. (1986). Research on teaching reading. In Whittock, M. C. (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (pp. 804–849). New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Calfee, R. C., & Norman, K. A. (1998). Psychological perspectives on the early reading wars: The case of phonological awareness. Teachers College Record, 100, 242–274.Google Scholar
Callens, M., Tops, W., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Cognitive profile of students who enter higher education with an indication of dyslexia. PLoS One, 7(6), e38081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callens, M., Whitney, C., Tops, W., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). No deficiency in left-to-right processing of words in dyslexia but evidence for enhanced visual crowding., The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(9), 1803–1817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, T. (2011). From aphasia to dyslexia, a fragment of a genealogy: An analysis of the formation of a “medical diagnosis”. Health Sociology Review, 20, 450–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campuzano, L., Dynarski, M., Agodini, R., & Rall, K. (2009). Effectiveness of reading and mathematics software products: Findings from two student cohorts (NCEE 2009–4041). Washington, DC: Institute of Education Sciences.Google Scholar
Canivez, G. L. (2013). Psychometric versus actuarial interpretation of intelligence and related aptitude batteries. In Saklofske, D. H., Reynolds, C. R., & Schwean, V. L. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of child psychological assessment (pp. 84–112). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cao, F., Bitan, T., Chou, T.-L., Burman, D. D., & Booth, J. R. (2006). Deficient orthographic and phonological representations in children with dyslexia revealed by brain activation patterns. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 1041–1050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardona, G., Borràs, R., Peris, E., & Castañé, M. (2010). A placebo-controlled trial of tinted lenses in adolescents with good and poor academic performance: reading accuracy and speed. Journal of Optometry, 3(2), 94–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Cornoldi, C., & De Beni, R. (2009). Role of working memory in explaining the performance of individuals with specific reading comprehension difficulties: A meta-analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 246–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. M., Snowling, M. J., Stevenson, J., & Hulme, C. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 39, 913–923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casanova, M. F., Araque, J., Giedd, J., & Rumsey, J. M. (2004). Reduced brain size and gyrification in the brains of dyslexic patients. Journal of Child Neurology, 19, 275–281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassar, M., Trieman, R., Moats, L., Pollo, T. C., & Kessler, B. (2005). How do the spellings of children with dyslexia compare with those of nondyslexic children? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 27–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castles, A., & Coultheart, M. (1996). Cognitive correlates of developmental surface dyslexia: A single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 25–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castles, A., & Coultheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read?Cognition, 91, 77–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catts, H. W. (1991). Early identification of dyslexia: Evidence from a follow-up study of speech-language impaired children. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 163–177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catts, H. W. (1993). The relationship between speech-language impairments and reading disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36, 948–958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catts, H. W. (2012). Comments on the dyslexia and visual attention study. Retrieved May 18, 2012, from .
Catts, H. W., & Adlof, S. (2011). Phonological and other language deficits associated with dyslexia. In Brady, S. A., Braze, D., & Fowler, C. A. (Eds.), Explaining individual differences in reading: Theory and evidence (pp. 137–151). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., Hogan, T. P., & Weismer, S. E. (2005). Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 48, 1378–1396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catts, H. W., Compton, D., Tomblin, J. B., & Bridges, M. S. (2012). Prevalence and nature of late-emerging poor readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(1), 166–181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 1142–1157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catts, H. W., Gillispie, M., Leonard, L., Kail, R. V., & Miller, C. A. (2002). The role of speed of processing, rapid naming, and phonological awareness in reading achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 509–524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., & Fey, M. E. (2003). Subgrouping poor readers on the basis of individual differences in reading-related abilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 151–164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catts, H. W., & Kamhi, A. (1999). Language and reading disabilities. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Catts, H. W., & Kamhi, A. G. (Eds.). (2005). Language and reading disabilities. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Catts, H. W., Petscher, Y., Schatschneider, C., Bridges, M. S., & Mendoza, K. (2009). Floor effects associated with universal screening and their impact on the early identification of reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 163–176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chacko, A., Bedard, A. C., Marks, D. J., Feirsen, N., Uderman, J. Z., et al. (in press). A randomized clinical trial of Cogmed Working Memory Training in school‐age children with ADHD: A replication in a diverse sample using a control condition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.
Chaix, Y., Albaret, J., Brassard, C., Cheuret, E., DeCastelnau, P., Benesteau, J., et al. (2007). Motor impairment in dyslexia: The influence of attention disorders. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 11, 368–374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chall, J. S. (1996). Learning to read: The great debate (3rd ed.). Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Chall, J. S. (2000). The academic achievement challenge. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Chan, D. W., Ho, C. S. H., Tsang, S. M., Lee, S. H., & Chung, K. K. H. (2007). Prevalence, gender ratio and gender differences in reading-related cognitive abilities among Chinese children with dyslexia in Hong Kong. Educational Studies, 33, 249–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, A. C. K., & Slavin, R. E. (2012). How features of educational technology applications affect student reading outcomes: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 7, 198–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, C. H., Wood, A. C., Paloyelis, Y., Arias-Vasquez, A., Buitelaar, J. K., et al. (2012). Aetiology for the covariation between combined type ADHD and reading difficulties in a family study: The role of IQ. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(8), 864–873.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chhabildas, N., Pennington, B. F., & Wilcutt, E. G. (2001). A comparison of the neuropsychological profiles of the DSM-IV subtypes of ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 529–540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiappe, P., Stringer, R., Siegel, L. S., & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). Why the timing deficit hypothesis does not explain reading disability in adults. Reading and Writing, 15, 73–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiarello, C., Lombardino, L. J., Kacinik, N. A., Otto, R., & Leonard, C. M. (2006). Neuroanatomical and behavioral asymmetry in an adult compensated dyslexic. Brain & Language, 98, 169–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chirkina, G. V., & Grigorenko, E. L. (in press). Tracking citations: A science detective story. Journal of Learning Disabilities.
Chiu, M. M., & McBride-Chang, C. (2006). Gender, context, and reading: A comparison of students in 43 countries. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 331–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cho, E., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., & Bouton, B. (in press). Examining the predictive validity of a dynamic assessment of decoding to forecast response to Tier 2 intervention. Journal of Learning Disabilities.
Christo, C., Davis, J. M., & Brock, S. E. (2009). Identifying, assessing, and treating dyslexia at school. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Christopher, M. E., Miyake, A., Keenan, J. M., Pennington, B., DeFries, J. C., Wadsworth, S. J., Willcutt, E., & Olson, R. K. (2012). Predicting word reading and comprehension with executive function and speed measures across development: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(3), 470–488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cirino, P. T., Pollard-Durodola, S. D., Foorman, B. R., Carlson, C. D., & Francis, D. J. (2007). Teacher characteristics, classroom instruction, and student literacy and language outcomes in bilingual kindergartners. The Elementary School Journal, 107, 341–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clamp, M., Fry, B., Kamal, M., Xie, X., Cuff, J., Lin, M. F., et al. (2007). Distinguishing protein-coding and noncoding genes in the human genome. PNAS, 104, 19428–19433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. B., & Uhry, J. K. (1955). Dyslexia: Theory and practice of remedial instruction (Vol. Ba). Baltimore, MD: York.Google Scholar
Clay, M. M. (1985). The early detection of reading difficulties (3rd ed.). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Clemens, N. H., Shapiro, E. S., Wu, J., Taylor, A. B., & Caskie, G. L. (in press). Monitoring early first-grade reading progress: A comparison of two measures. Journal of Learning Disabilities.
Coffield, M., Riddick, B., Barmby, P., & O’Neill, J. (2008). Dyslexia friendly primary schools: What can we learn from asking the pupils? In Reid, G., Fawcett, A., Manis, F., & Siegel, L. (Eds.), Sage handbook of dyslexia (pp. 356–368). London: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen-Mimran, R., & Sapir, S. (2007). Deficits in working memory in young adults with reading disabilities. Journal of Communication Disorders, 40(2), 168–183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coles, G. (1998). Reading lessons: The debate over literacy. New York: Hill and Wang.Google Scholar
Collis, N. L., Kohnen, S., & Kinoshita, S. (2013). The role of visual spatial attention in adult developmental dyslexia. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(2), 245–260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coltheart, M. (1981). Disorders of reading and their implications for models of normal reading. Visible Language, 15, 245–286.Google Scholar
Compton, D. L. (2003). Modeling the relationship between growth in rapid naming speed and growth in decoding skill in first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 225–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Compton, D. L., & Carlisle, J. F. (1994). Speed of word recognition as a distinguishing characteristic of reading disabilities. Educational Psychology Review, 6, 115–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Compton, D. L., DeFries, J. C., & Olson, R. K. (2001). Are RAN and phonological awareness deficits additive in children with reading disabilities? Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research & Practice, 7, 125–149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., Bouton, B., Gilbert, J. K., Barquero, L. A., et al. (2010). Selecting at-risk first-grade readers for early intervention: eliminating false positives and exploring the promise of a two-stage gated screening process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 327–340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., & Bryant, J. D. (2006). Selecting at-risk readers in first grade for early intervention: A two-year longitudinal study of decision rules and procedures. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 394–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., Elleman, A. M., & Gilbert, J. K. (2008). Tracking children who fly below the radar screen: Latent transition modeling of students with late-emerging reading disability. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 329–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Compton, D. L., Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Lambert, W., & Hamlett, C. (2012). The cognitive and academic profiles of reading and mathematics learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45, 79–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compton, D. L., Gilbert, J. K., Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., Cho, E., Barquero, L. A., & Bouton, B. D. (2012). Accelerating chronically unresponsive children to Tier 3 instruction: What level of data is necessary to ensure selection accuracy? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45, 204–216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conlon, E. G. (2012). Visual discomfort and reading. In Stein, J. & Kapoula, Z. (Eds.), Visual aspects of dyslexia (pp. 79–90). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conlon, E. G., Lilleskaret, G., Wright, C. M., & Power, G. F. (2012). The influence of contrast on coherent motion processing in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 50, 1672–1681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conlon, E. G., Lovegrove, W., Chekaluk, E., & Pattison, P. (1999). Measuring visual discomfort. Visual Cognition, 6, 637–663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conlon, E. G., Sanders, M. A., & Wright, C. M. (2009). Relationships between global motion and global form processing, practice, cognitive and visual processing in adults with dyslexia or visual discomfort. Neuropsychologia, 47, 907–915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conlon, E. G., Wright, C. M., Norris, K., & Chekaluk, E. (2011). Does a sensory processing deficit explain counting accuracy on rapid visual sequencing tasks in adults with and without dyslexia? Brain and Cognition, 76, 197–205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conners, F. A., Atwell, J. A., Rosenquist, C. J., & Sligh, A. C. (2001). Abilities underlying decoding differences in children with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 45, 292–299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connor, C. M. (2010). Child characteristics-instruction interactions: Implications for students’ literacy skills development in the early grades. In Neuman, S. B. & Dickinson, D. K. (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy (3rd ed., pp. 256–278). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., Fishman, B., Crowe, E., Al Otaiba, S., & Schatschneider, C. (2013). A longitudinal cluster-randomized controlled study on the accumulating effects of individualized literacy instruction on students' reading from first through third grade. Psychological Science, 24(8), 1408–1419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., & Katch, E. L. (2004). Beyond the reading wars: Exploring the effect of child–instruction interaction on growth in early reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 305–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., Fishman, B. J., Schatschneider, C., & Underwood, P. (2007). Algorithm-guided individualized reading instruction. Science, 315, 464–465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connor, C. M., Piasta, S. B., Fishman, B., Glasney, S., Schatschneider, C., Crowe, E., et al. (2009). Individualizing student instruction precisely: Effects of child by instruction interactions on first graders’ literacy development. Child Development, 80, 77–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooke, A. (2001). Critical response to “Dyslexia, Literacy and Psychological Assessment (Report by a Working Party of the Division of Educational and Child Psychology of the British Psychological Society)”: A view from the chalk face. Dyslexia, 7, 47–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, G. M., Coe, B. P., Girirajan, S., Rosenfeld, J. A., Vu, T. H., Baker, C., et al. (2011). A copy number variation morbidity map of developmental delay. Nature Genetics, 43, 838–846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corina, D. P., Richards, T. L., Serafini, S., Richards, A. L., Steury, K., Abbott, R. D., et al. (2001). fMRI auditory language differences between dyslexic and able reading children. Neuroreport, 12, 1195–1201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornelissen, P. L., Richardson, A., Mason, A., Fowler, S., & Stein, J. F. (1995). Contrast sensitivity and coherent motion detection measured at photopic luminance levels in dyslexics and controls. Vision Research, 35, 1483–1494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corriveau, K. H., Goswami, U., & Thomson, J. M. (2010). Auditory processing and early literacy skills in a preschool and kindergarten population. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 369–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coventry, W. L., Byrne, B., Olson, R. K., Corley, R., & Samuelsson, S. (2011). Dynamic and static assessment of phonological awareness in preschool: a behavior-genetic study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44, 322–329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Covington, M. V. (1992). Making the grade: A self-worth perspective on motivation and school reform. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N., & Alloway, T. P. (2008). The development of working memory. In Cowan, N. (Ed.), Development of memory in childhood (pp. 303–342). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Crisfield, J. (1996). The dyslexia handbook. London: British Dyslexia Association.Google Scholar
Crombie, M., & Reid, G. (2009). The role of early identification research: Models from research and practice. In Reid, G. (Ed.), The Routledge companion to dyslexia (pp. 71–79). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. J. (1975). Beyond the two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 30, 116–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cronbach, L. J., & Snow, R. E. (1977). Aptitudes and instructional methods: A handbook for research on interactions. Irvington: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1998). The impact of print exposure on word recognition. In Metsala, J. L. & Ehri, L. C. (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 235–262). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Curtis, M. (2004). Adolescents who struggle with word identification: Research and practice. In Jetton, T. L. & Dole, J. A. (Eds.), Adolescent literacy research and practice (pp. 119–134). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Cutting, L. E., & Denckla, M. B. (2001). The relationship of rapid serial naming and word reading in normally developing readers: An exploratory model. Reading and Writing, 14, 673–705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cyhlarova, E., Bell, J. G., Dick, J. R., Mackinley, E. E., Stein, J. F., & Richardson, A. J. (2007). Membrane fatty acids, reading and spelling in dyslexic and non-dyslexic adults. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 17, 116–121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahlin, K. (2011). Effects of working memory training on reading in children with special needs. Reading and Writing, 24, 479–491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davey Smith, G., Ebrahim, S., Lewis, S., Hansell, A. L., Palmer, L. J., & Burton, P. R. (2005). Genetic epidemiology and public health: hope, hype, and future prospects. Lancet, 366, 1484–1498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, R. D. (1997). The gift of dyslexia. London: Souvenir Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
de Bree, E., Snowling, M. J., Gerrits, E., van Alphen, P., van der Leij, A., & Wijnen, F. (2012). Phonology and literacy. In Benasich, A. A. & Fitch, R. H. (Eds.), Developmental dyslexia: Early precursors, neurobehavioral markers, and biological substrates (pp. 133–150). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
De Clercq-Quaegebeur, M., Casalis, S., Lemaitre, M., Bourgois, B., Getto, M., & Vallée, L. (2010). Neuropsychological profile on the WISC-IV of French children with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 563–574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Graaff, S. E. H., Bosman, A. M. T., Hasselman, F., & Verhoeven, L. (2009). Benefits of systematic phonics instruction. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 318–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Kovel, C. G. F., Hol, F. A., Heister, J., Willemen, J., Sandkuijl, L. A., Franke, B., et al. (2004). Genomewide scan identifies susceptibility locus for dyslexia on Xq27 in an extended Dutch family. Journal of Medical Genetics, 41, 652–657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Weerdt, F., Desoete, A., & Roeyers, H. (2013). Working memory in children with reading disabilities and/or mathematical disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(5), 461–472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deffenbacher, K. E., Kenyon, J. B., Hoover, D. M., Olson, R. K., Pennington, B. F., DeFries, J. C., et al. (2004). Refinement of the 6p21.3 quantitative trait locus influencing dyslexia: linkage and association analyses. Human Genetics, 115, 128–138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Delacato, C. H. (1959). The treatment and prevention of reading problems. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
Demb, J. B., Boynton, G. M., Best, M., & Heeger, D. J. (1998). Psychophysical evidence for a magnocellular pathway deficit in dyslexia. Vision Research, 38, 1555–1559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demb, J. B., Boynton, G. M., & Heeger, D. J. (1998). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of early visual pathways in dyslexia. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 6939–6951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Démonet, J.-F., Taylor, M. J., & Chaix, Y. (2004). Developmental dyslexia. Lancet, 363, 1451–1460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denckla, M., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186–202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denckla, M., & Rudel, R. (1976a). Naming of object-drawings by dyslexic and other learning disabled children. Brain and Language, 3, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denckla, M., & Rudel, R. (1976b). Rapid “automatized’ naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471–479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denckla, M. B. (1972). Color-naming deficits in dyslexic boys. Cortex, 8(2), 164–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denton, C. A. (2012). Response to intervention for reading difficulties in the primary grades: Some answers and lingering questions. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(3), 232–243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denton, C. A., Cirino, P. T., Barth, A. E., Romain, M., Vaughn, S., Wexler, J., et al. (2011). An experimental study of scheduling and duration of “Tier 2” first-grade reading intervention. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 4, 208–230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denton, C. A., & Hocker, J. L. (2006). Responsive reading instruction: Flexible intervention for struggling readers in the early grades. Longmont, CO: Sopris West.Google Scholar
Denton, C. A., Nimon, K., Mathes, P. G., Swanson, E. A., Kethley, C., Kurz, T. B., & Shih, M. (2010). Effectiveness of a supplemental early reading intervention scaled up in multiple schools. Exceptional Children, 76, 394–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denton, C. A., Tolar, T. D., Fletcher, J. M., Barth, A. E., Vaughn, S., & Francis, D. J. (2013). Effects of Tier 3 Intervention for Students with Persistent Reading Difficulties and Characteristics of Inadequate Responders. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(3), 633–648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denton, C. A., & Vaughn, S. (2008). Reading and writing intervention for older students with disabilities: Possibilities and challenges. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 23, 61–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denton, C. A., Wexler, J., Vaughn, S., & Bryan, D. (2008). Intervention provided to linguistically diverse middle school students with severe reading difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 23, 79–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Department of Education and Science. (1975). A language for life (The Bullock Report). London: Her Majesty's Stationery Office.Google Scholar
Deutsch, G. K., Dougherty, R. F., Bammer, R., Siok, W. T., Gabrieli, J. D., & Wandell, B. (2005). Children's reading performance is correlated with white matter structure measured by diffusion tensor imaging. Cortex, 41, 354–363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diehl, J. J., Frost, S. J., Mencl, W. E., & Pugh, K. R. (2011). Neuroimaging and the phonlogical deficit hypothesis. In Brady, S. A., Braze, D., & Fowler, C. A. (Eds.), Explaining individual differences in reading: Theory and evidence (pp. 217–237). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dirks, E., Spyer, G., van Lieshout, E. C. D. M., & de Sonneville, L. (2008). Prevalence of combined reading and arithmetic disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(5), 460–473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doman, G., & Delacato, C. H. (1968). Doman-Delacato philosophy. Human Potential, 1, 113–116.Google Scholar
Dore, W., & Brookes, D. (2006). Dyslexia: The miracle cure. London: Blake.Google Scholar
Dorsaint-Pierre, R., Penhune, V. B., Watkins, K. E., Neelin, P., Lerch, J. P., Bouffard, M., et al. (2006). Asymmetries of the planum temporale and Heschl's gyrus: relationship to language lateralization. Brain, 129, 1164–1176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dougherty, R. F., Ben-Shachar, M., Deutsch, G. K., Hernandez, A., Fox, G. R., & Wandell, B. A. (2007). Temporal-callosal pathway diffusivity predicts phonological skills in children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 8556–8561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doyon, J., Song, A. W., Karni, A., Lalonde, F., Adams, M. M., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Experience-dependent changes in cerebellar contributions to motor sequence learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 1017–1022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drake, W. E. (1968). Clinical and pathological finding in a child with a developmental learning disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1, 486–502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dresher, B. E. (2011). The phoneme. In Oostendorp, M. v., Ewen, C. J., Hume, E., & Rice, K. (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to phonology (Vol. 1, pp. 241–266). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Duara, R., Kushch, A., Gross-Glenn, K., et al. (1991). Neuroanatomic differences between dyslexic and normal readers on magnetic resonance imaging scans. Archives of Neurology, 48, 410–416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubois, M., Kyllingsboek, S., Prado, C., Musca, S. C., Peiffer, E., Lassus-Sangosse, D., et al. (2010). Fractionating the multi-character processing deficit in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from two case studies. Cortex, 46, 717–738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duff, F. J., & Clarke, P. J. (2011). Practitioner review: Reading disorders – What are effective interventions and how should they be implemented and evaluated? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 52, 3–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duff, F. J., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., & Hulme, C. (2012). Evaluating the effectiveness of a phonologically based reading intervention for struggling readers with varying language profiles. Reading and Writing, 25, 621–640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dufor, O., Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Démonet, J. F. (2007). Top-down processes during auditory phoneme categorization in dyslexia: A PET study. Neuroimage, 34, 1692–1707.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, C. C., Rumsey, J. M., Wilkniss, S. M., Denckla, M. B., Hamburger, S. D., & Odou-Potkin, M. (1994). Developmental dyslexia and attention dysfunction in adults: brain potential indices of information processing. Psychophysiology, 31, 386–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunning, D. L., Holmes, J., & Gathercole, S. E. (2013). Does working memory training lead to generalized improvements in children with low working memory? A randomized controlled trial. Developmental Science, 16(6), 915–925.Google ScholarPubMed
Dynarski, M., Agodini, R., Heaviside, S., Novak, T., Carey, N., Campuzano, L., et al. (2007). Effectiveness of reading and mathematics software products: Findings from the first student cohort. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.Google Scholar
Dyslexia Action. (2005). The incidence of hidden disabilities in the prison population. Egham, UK: Dyslexia Action.Google Scholar
Dyslexia Foundation of New Zealand. (2008). Dealing with dyslexia: The way forward for New Zealand educators. Christchurch, New Zealand: Author.Google Scholar
Eckert, M. A. (2004). Neuroanatomical markers for dyslexia: A review of dyslexia structural imaging studies. Neuroscientist, 10, 362–371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eckert, M. A., Leonard, C. M., Richards, T. L., Aylward, E. H., Thomson, J., & Berninger, V. W. (2003). Anatomical correlates of dyslexia: Frontal and cerebellar findings. Brain, 126, 482–494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eckert, M. A., Leonard, C. M., Wilke, M., Eckert, M., Richards, T., Richards, A., et al. (2005). Anatomical signatures of dyslexia in children: Unique information from manual and voxel based morphometry brain measures. Cortex, 41, 304–315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eden, G. F., VanMeter, J. W., Rumsey, J. M., Maisog, J. M., Woods, R. P., & Zeffiro, T. A. (1996). Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging. Nature, 382, 66–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egeland, J., Aarlien, A. K., & Saunes, B. K. (2013). Few effects of far transfer of working memory training in ADHD: A randomized controlled trial. PLOS ONE, 8(10), e75660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehri, L. C. (1999). Phases of development in learning to read words. In Oakhill, J. & Beard, R. (Eds.), Reading development and the teaching of reading: A psychological perspective (pp. 79–108). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (2002). Phases of acquisition in learning to read words and implications for teaching. British Journal of Educational Psychology: Monograph Series, 1, 7–28.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Stahl, S. A., & Willows, D. M. (2001). Systematic phonics instruction helps students learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel's meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 71, 393–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel's meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eichler, E. E., Flint, J., Gibson, G., Kong, A., Leal, S. M., Moore, J. H., et al. (2010). Missing heritability and strategies for finding the underlying causes of complex disease. Nature Reviews Genetics, 11, 446–450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eicher, J. D., Powers, N. R., Miller, L. L., Akshoomoff, N., Amaral, D. G., et al. (2013). Genome‐wide association study of shared components of reading disability and language impairment. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 12(8), 792–801.CrossRef
Eide, B. L., & Eide, F. F. (2011). The dyslexic advantage: Unlocking the hidden potential of the dyslexic brain. New York: Hudson Street Press.Google Scholar
Eissa, M. (2010). Behavioral and emotional problems associated with dyslexia in adolescence. Current Psychiatry, 17(1), 17–25.Google Scholar
Eklund, K. M., Torppa, M., & Lyytinen, H. (2013). Predicting reading disability: Early cognitive risk and protective factors. Dyslexia, 19(1), 1–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elbeheri, G., & Everatt, J. (2009). Dyslexia and IQ: From research to practice. In Reid, G. (Ed.), The Routledge companion to dyslexia (pp. 22–32). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Elliott, J. G. (2003). Dynamic Assessment in educational settings: Realising potential, Educational Review, 55, 15–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, J. G. (2005). The dyslexia debate continues. The Psychologist, 18, 728–729.Google Scholar
Elliott, J. G. (2008). The dyslexia myth. Learning Difficulties Australia Bulletin, 40, 10–14.Google Scholar
Elliott, J. G., Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Kirkwood, H., & Holmes, J. (2010). An evaluation of a classroom-based intervention to help overcome working memory difficulties. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 9, 227–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, J. G., & Gibbs, S. (2008). Does dyslexia exist? Journal of Philosophy of Education, 42, 475–491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, J. G., & Place, M. (2012). Children in difficulty: A guide to understanding and helping (3rd ed.). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ellis, A. W., Flude, B. M., & Young, A. W. (1987). “Neglect dyslexia” and the early visual processing of letters in words and nonwords. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4, 439–463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elston, R. C., & Johnson, W. D. (2008). Basic biostatistics for geneticists and epidemiologists. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ercan-Sencicek, A. G., Davis Wright, N. R., Sanders, S. S., Oakman, N., Valdes, L., Bakkaloglu, B., et al. (2012). A balanced t(10;15) translocation in a male patient with developmental language disorder. European Journal of Medical Genetics, 55, 128–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Essex, M. J., Boyce, W. T., Hertzman, C., Lam, L. L., Armstrong, J. M., Neumann, S. M. A., et al. (2013). Epigenetic vestiges of early developmental adversity: Childhood stress exposure and DNA methylation in adolescence. Child Development, 84(1), 58–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Everatt, J., & Reid, G. (2009). Dyslexia: An overview of recent research. In Reid, G. (Ed.), The Routledge companion to dyslexia (pp. 3–21). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Fabbro, F., Pesenti, S., Facoetti, A., Bonanomi, M., Libera, L., & Lorusso, M. L. (2001). Callosal transfer in different subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 37, 65–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Facoetti, A., Lorusso, M. L., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., & Molteni, M. (2005). Visual and auditory attentional capture are both sluggish in children with developmental dyslexia. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 65, 61–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Facoetti, A., Lorusso, M. L., Paganoni, P., Umiltà, C., & Mascetti, G. C. (2003). The role of visuospatial attention in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from a rehabilitation study. Cognitive Brain Research, 15, 154–164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Ruffino, M., Lorusso, M. L., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., et al. (2009). Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1011–1025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagerheim, T., Raeymaekers, P., Tonnessen, F. E., Pedersen, M., Tranebjaerg, L., & Lubs, H. A. (1999). A new gene (DYX3) for dyslexia is located on chromosome 2. Journal of Medical Genetics, 35, 664–669.Google Scholar
Fälth, L.Gustafson, S., Tjus, T., Heimann, M., & Svensson, I. (2013). Computer-assisted interventions targeting reading skills of children with reading disabilities – A longitudinal study. Dyslexia, 19, 37–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farmer, M. E., & Klein, R. M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: A Review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 460–493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Favell, J. E. (2005). Sifting sound practice from snake oil. In Jacobson, J. W., Foxx, R. M., & Mulick, J. A. (Eds.), Controversial therapies for developmental disabilities: Fad, fashion and science in professional practice (pp. 19–30). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fawcett, A. J., Nicolson, R., & Maclagan, F. (2001). Cerebellar tests differentiate between groups of poor readers with and without IQ discrepancy. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34(2), 119–135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fawcett, A. J., & Nicolson, R. I. (1999). Performance of dyslexic children on cerebellar and cognitive tests. Journal of Motor Behavior, 31, 68–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fawcett, A. J., Nicolson, R. I., & Lee, R. (2004). Ready to learn. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Fawcett, A. J., & Reid, G. (2009). Dyslexia and alternative interventions for dyslexia: A critical commentary. In Reid, G. (Ed.), The Routledge companion to dyslexia (pp. 157–174). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Feldman, H. M., Yeatman, J. D., Lee, E. S., Barde, L. H., & Gaman-Bean, S. (2010). Diffusion tensor imaging: A review for pediatric researchers and clinicians. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 31, 346–356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, G. M., & Keller, H. (1921). The effect of kinaesthetic factors in the development of word recognition in the case of non-readers. The Journal of Educational Research, 4, 355–377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrer, E., Shaywitz, B. A., Holahan, J. M., Marchione, K., & Shaywitz, S. E. (2010). Uncoupling of reading and IQ over time: Empirical evidence for a definition of dyslexia. Psychological Science, 21, 93–101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, L. L., Shumansky, K., Ryan, J., Truong, D., Swiergala, E. & Kaplan, B. J. (2013). Dense-map genome scan for dyslexia supports loci at 4q13, 16p12, 17q22, suggests novel locus at 7q36. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 12(6), 56–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fields, R. D. (2008). White matter matters. Scientific American, 298, 42–49.Google ScholarPubMed
Fiez, J. A., Tranel, D., Seager-Frerichs, D., & Damasio, H. (2006). Specific reading and phonological processing deficits are associated with damage to the left frontal operculum. Cortex, 42(4), 624–643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finch, A. J., Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2002). Evidence for a neuroanatomical difference within the olivo-cerebellar pathway of adults with dyslexia. Cortex, 38, 529–539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finnucci, J. M., & Childs, B. (1981). Are there really more dyslexic boys than girls? In Ansora, A., Geschwind, N., Galaburda, A., Albert, M., & Gartrell, N. (Eds.), Sex differences in dyslexia (pp. 1–9). Towson, MD: Orton Dyslexia Society.Google Scholar
Fisher, S. E., Francks, C., Marlow, A. J., MacPhie, I. L., Newbury, D. F., Cardon, L. R., et al. (2002). Independent genome-wide scans identify a chromosome 18 quantitative-trait locus influencing dyslexia. Nature Genetics, 30, 86–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flannery, K. A., Liederman, J., Daly, L., & Schultz, J. (2000). Male prevalence for reading disability is found in a large sample of black and white children free from ascertainment bias. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 433–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flesch, R. (1955). Why Johnny can't read and what you can do about it. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Dyslexia: The evolution of a scientific concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 501–508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, J. M., Foorman, B. R., Boudousquie, A., Barnes, M. A., Schatschneider, C., & Francis, D. J. (2002). Assessment of reading and learning disabilities, a research-based intervention-oriented approach. Journal of School Psychology, 40, 27–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Barnes, M. A. (2007). Learning disabilities. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Fletcher, J. M., Morris, R.D., & Lyon, G. R. (2003). Classification and definition of learning disabilities: An integrative perspective. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 30–56). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Shankweiler, D. P., Katz, L., Liberman, I. Y., Stuebing, K. K., et al. (1994). Cognitive profiles of reading disability: Comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 6–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, J. M., Stuebing, K. K., Barth, A. E., Denton, C. A., Cirino, P. T., Francis, D. J., et al. (2011). Cognitive correlates of inadequate response to reading intervention. School Psychology Review, 40, 3–22.Google ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, J. M., Stuebing, K. K., Morris, R.D., & Lyon, G. R. (2013). Classification and definition of learning disabilities: A hybrid model. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities, second edition (pp. 33–50). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fletcher, J. M., & Vaughn, S. (2009). Response to intervention: Preventing and remediating academic difficulties. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 30–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flowers, L., Meyer, M., Lovato, J., Wood, F., & Felton, R. (2001). Does third grade discrepancy status predict the course of reading development?Annals of Dyslexia, 51, 49–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, J. M., & Rahbar, M. H. (1994). Prevalence of reading failure in boys compared with girls. Psychology in the Schools, 31, 66–70.3.0.CO;2-J>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, L. J., Zheng, X., & Swanson, H. L. (2012). Instructing struggling older readers: A selective meta-analysis of intervention research. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 27(1), 21–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foorman, B. R. (2003). Preventing and remediating reading difficulties: Bringing science to scale. Baltimore, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Foorman, B. R., Anthony, J., Seals, L., & Mouzaki, A. (2002). Language development and emergent literacy in preschool. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 9, 172–183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M., & Francis, D. J. (2004). Texas Primary Reading Inventory. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., Chen, D., Mouzaki, A., et al. (1998). Technical report: Texas Primary Reading Inventory (1998 Edition). Houston, TX: Center for Academic and Reading Skills, University of Houston.Google Scholar
Foorman, B. R., Petscher, Y., Lefsky, E. B., & Toste, J. R. (2010). Reading First in Florida: Five years of improvement. Journal of Literacy Research, 42, 71–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foorman, B. R., York, M., Santi, K. L., & Francis, D. (2008). Contextual effects on predicting risk for reading difficulties in first and second grade. Reading and Writing, 21, 371–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foster, L. M., Hynd, G. W., Morgan, A. E., & Hugdahl, K. (2002). Planum temporale asymmetry and ear advantage in dichotic listening in developmental dyslexia and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 22–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Pedrolli, K., & Facoetti, A. (2012). A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Current Biology, 22, 814–819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Viola, S., Molteni, M., & Facoetti, A. (2013). Action video games make dyslexic children read better. Current Biology, 23, 462–466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., Stuebing, K. K., Shaywitz, B. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (1996). Developmental lag versus deficit models of reading disability: A longitudinal individual growth curves analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 3–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Francks, C., Paracchini, S., Smith, S. D., Richardson, A. J., Scerri, T. S., Cardon, L. R., et al. (2004). A 77-kilobase region on chromosome 6p22.2 is associated with dyslexia in families from the United Kingdom and from the United States. American Journal of Human Genetics, 75, 1046–1058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frederickson, N. (1999). The ACID test – or is it?Educational Psychology in Practice, 15, 2–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frederickson, N., & Frith, U. (1998). Identifying dyslexia in bilingual children: a phonological approach with inner London Sylheti speakers. Dyslexia, 4, 119–131.3.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, N., Biran, M., & Gvion, A. (2012). Patterns of visual dyslexia. Journal of Neuropsychology, 6, 1–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedmann, N., & Gvion, A. (2001). Letter position dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 673–696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedmann, N., Kerbel, N., & Shvimer, L. (2010). Developmental attentional dyslexia. Cortex, 46, 1216–1237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedmann, N., & Rahamim, E. (2007). Developmental letter position dyslexia. Journal of Neuropsychology, 1, 201–236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedmann, N., & Rahamim, E. (in press). What can reduce letter migrations in letter position dyslexia?Journal of Research in Reading.
Friend, A., DeFries, J. C., & Olson, R. K. (2008). Parental education moderates genetic influences on reading disability. Psychological Science, 19, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frijjters, J. C., Lovett, M. W., Steinbach, K. A., Wolf, M., Sevcik, R. A., & Morris, R. (2011). Neurocognitive predictors of reading outcomes for children with reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44, 150–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friso-van den Bos, I., van der Ven, S. H. G., Kroesbergen, E. H., & van Luit, J. E. H. (2013). Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 10, 29–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U. (1997). Brain, mind and behaviour in dyslexia. In Hulme, C. & Snowling, M. J. (Eds.), Dyslexia: Biology, cognition, and intervention (pp. 1–19). London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Frye, R. E., Liederman, J., Malmberg, B., McLean, J., Strickland, D., & Beauchamp, M. S. (2010). Surface area accounts for the relation of gray matter volume to reading-related skills and history of dyslexia. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 2625–2635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, L. S., Bryant, J., & Davis, G. N. (2008). Making “secondary intervention” work in a three-tier responsiveness-to-intervention model: findings from the first-grade longitudinal reading study of the National Research Center on Learning Disabilities. Reading and Writing, 21, 413–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, L. S., Bryant, J., Hamlett, C. L., & Lambert, W. (2012). First-grade cognitive abilities as long-term predictors of reading comprehension and disability status. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45, 217–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, D., & Deshler, D. K. (2007). What we need to know about responsiveness to intervention (and shouldn't be afraid to ask). Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 20, 129–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., & Compton, D. L. (2012). Smart RTI: A next generation approach to multilevel prevention. Exceptional Children, 78(3), 263–279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., Mathes, P. G., & Simmons, D. C. (1997). Peer-assisted learning strategies: Making classrooms more responsive to diversity. American Educational Research Journal, 34, 174–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, D., McMaster, K. L., Fuchs, L.S., & Al Otaiba, S. (2013). Data-based individualization as a means of providing intensive instruction to students with serious learning disorders. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 526–544). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (1998). Treatment validity: A unifying concept for reconceptualizing the identification of learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 13, 204–219.Google Scholar
Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (2009). Creating opportunities for intensive intervention for students with learning disabilities. Teaching Exceptional Children, 42, 60–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Compton, D. L. (2010). Commentary: Rethinking response to intervention at middle and high school. School Psychology Review, 39, 22–28.Google Scholar
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Compton, D. L. (2013). Intervention effects for students with comorbid forms of learning disability: Understanding the needs of nonresponders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(6), 534–548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, L. S., & Vaughn, S. (2012). Responsiveness-to-intervention: A decade later. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(3), 195–203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2010). Predicting reading and spelling difficulties in transparent and opaque orthographies: A comparison between Scandinavian and US/Australian children. Dyslexia, 16, 119–142.Google ScholarPubMed
Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2011). Phonological awareness and raid automatized naming predicting early development in reading and spelling: Results from a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 85–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2009). Dyslexia: A new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science, 325, 280–283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Norton, E. S. (2012). Reading abilities: Importance of visual-spatial attention. Cortex, 22, R298–R299.Google ScholarPubMed
Galaburda, A. M. (1993). Neuroanatomic basis of developmental dyslexia. Neurology Clinics, 11, 161–173.Google ScholarPubMed
Galaburda, A. M., & Kemper, T. L. (1979). Cytoarchitectonic abnormalities in developmental dyslexia: a case study. Annals of Neurology, 6, 94–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galaburda, A. M., LoTurco, J. J., Ramus, F., Fitch, R. H., & Rosen, G. D. (2006). From genes to behavior in developmental dyslexia. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 1213–1217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galaburda, A. M., Menard, M. T., & Rosen, G. D. (1994). Evidence for aberrant auditory anatomy in developmental dyslexia. PNAS, 91, 8010–8013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galaburda, A. M., Schrott, L. M., & Sherman, G. F. (1996). Animal models of developmental dyslexia. Baltimore, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Galaburda, A. M., Sherman, G. F., Rosen, G. D., Aboitiz, F., & Gerschwin, N. (1985). Developmental dyslexia: four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. Annals of Neurology, 18, 222–233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gamse, B. C., Tepper-Jacob, R., Horst, M., Boulay, B., & Unlu, F. (2008). Reading First impact study: Final Report. Washington, DC: Institute for Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.Google Scholar
Garner, M. (2004, September 10). Universities exaggerate dyslexia epidemic for own gain, expert claims, Times Higher Education. Retrieved October 6, 2013 from .
Gathercole, S. E., & Alloway, T. P. (2008). Working memory and learning: A practical guide. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2006). Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 265–281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Knight, C., & Stegmann, Z. (2004). Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments and 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gayán, J., & Olson, R. K. (2001). Genetic and environmental influences on orthographic and phonological skills in children with reading disabilities. Developmental Neurology, 20, 483–507.Google ScholarPubMed
Gayán, J., & Olson, R. K. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in printed word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 97–123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelzheiser, L. M., Scanlon, D., Vellutino, F., Halgren-Flynn, L., & Schatschneider, C. (2011). Effects of the interactive strategies approach-extended: A responsive and comprehensive intervention for struggling readers. Elementary School Journal, 112, 280–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgiewa, P., Rzanny, R., Gaser, C., Gerhard, U. J., Vieweg, U., Freesmeyer, D., et al. (2002). Phonological processing in dyslexic children: A study combining functional imaging and event related potentials. Neuroscience Letters, 318, 5–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgiewa, P., Rzanny, R., Hopf, J. M., Knab, R., Glauche, V., Kaiser, W. A., et al. (1999). fMRI during word processing in dyslexic and normal reading children. Neuroreport, 10, 3459–3465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgiou, G. K., Papadopoulos, T. C., Zarouna, E., & Parrila, R. (2012). Are auditory and visual processing deficits related to developmental dyslexia?Dyslexia, 18(2), 110–129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgiou, G. K., & Parrila, R., (2013). Rapid automatized naming and reading: A review. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 169–185). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., Cui, Y., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2013). Why is rapid automatized naming related to reading?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 218–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. R. (2009). RAN components and reading development from grade 3 to grade 5: What underlies their relationship? Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 508–534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgiou, G. K., Protopapas, A., Papadopoulos, T. C., Skaloumbakas, C., & Parrila, R. (2010). Auditory temporal processing and dyslexia in an orthographically consistent language. Cortex, 46, 1330–1344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gersten, R., Compton, D., Connor, C. M., Dimino, J., Santoro, L., Linan-Thompson, S., et al. (2008). Assisting students struggling with reading: Response to intervention and multi-tier intervention in the primary grades. A practice guide (NCEE 2009–4045). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.Google Scholar
Geschwind, N. (1982). Why Orton was right. Annals of Dyslexia, 32, 13–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, N., & Levitsky, W. (1968). Human brain: Left-right asymmetries in temporal speech region. Science, 161, 186–187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Getchell, N., Pabreja, P., Neeld, K., & Carrio, V. (2007). Comparing children with and without dyslexia on the movement assessment battery for children and the test of gross motor development. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 105, 207–214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giedd, J. N., & Rapoport, J. L. (2010). Structural MRI of pediatric brain development: What have we learned and where are we going?Neuron, 67, 728–734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, J. K., Compton, D. L., & Kearns, D. M. (2011). Word and person effects on decoding accuracy: A new look at an old question. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(2), 489–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilger, J. W., Pennington, B. F., & DeFries, J. C. (1992). A twin study of the etiology of comorbidity: Attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder and dyslexia. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 343–348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillam, R. B., Loeb, D. F., Hoffman, L. M., Bohman, T., Champlin, C. A., Thibodeau, L., et al. (2008). The efficacy of Fast ForWord language intervention in school-age children with language impairment: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 97–119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillingham, A., & Stillman, B.W. (1997). The Gillingham manual: Remedial training for children with specific disability in reading, spelling, and penmanship (8th ed.). Cambridge, MA: Educators Publishing Service.Google Scholar
Girirajan, S., Brkanac, Z., Coe, B. P., Baker, C., Vives, L., Vu, T. H., et al. (2011). Relative burden of large CNVs on a range of neurodevelopmental phenotypes. PLOS Genetics, 7, e1002334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goddard, R. (1991). Why LINC matters. English in Education, 25, 32–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goddard Blythe, S. (2005). Releasing educational potential through movement: A summary of individual studies carried out using the INPP test battery and developmental exercise programme for use in schools with children with special needs. Child Care in Practice, 11, 415–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goe, L. (2007). The link between teacher quality and student outcomes: A research synthesis. Retrieved February 16, 2012 from .
Goldston, D. B., Walsh, A., Mayfield-Arnold, E., et al. (2007). Reading problems, psychiatric disorders and functional impairment from mid- to late adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(1), 25–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzaga-Jauregui, C., Lupski, J. R., & Gibbs, R. A. (2012). Human genome sequencing in health and disease. Annual Review of Medicine, 63, 35–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gooch, D., Snowling, M., & Hulme, C. (2011). Time perception, phonological skills and executive function in children with dyslexia and/or ADHD symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 195–203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Good, R. H., & Kaminski, R. A. (2003). Dynamic indicators of basic early literacy skills. Longmont, CO: Sopris West Educational Services.Google Scholar
Good, R. H., Kaminski, R. A., Shinn, M., Bratten, J., Shinn, M., Laimon, L., et al. (2004). Technical adequacy and decision making utility of DIBELS (Technical Report No. 7). Eugene: University of Oregon Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, I., Libenson, A., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2010) Sensitivity to linguistic stress, phonological awareness and early reading ability in preschoolers. Journal of Research in Reading, 33(2), 113–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, K. S. (1965). A linguistic study of cues and miscues in reading. Elementary English, 42, 639–643.Google Scholar
Goodman, K. S. (1967). Reading: A psycholinguistic guessing game. Journal of the Reading Specialist, 6, 126–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, K. S. (1969). Analysis or oral reading miscues: Applied psycholinguistics. Reading Research Quarterly, 5, 9–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, K. S. (1970). Reading: A psycholinguistic guessing game. In Singer, H. & Ruddell, R. B. (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (pp. 259–272). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
Goodman, K. S. (1986). What's whole in whole language?Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Goodman, K. S. (1992). Why whole language is today's agenda in education. Language Arts, 69, 354–363.Google Scholar
Gormley, K. A., & McDermott, P. (2011). Traditions of diagnosis: Learning from the past, moving past traditions. In Allington, R. & McGill-Franzen, A. (Eds.), Handbook of research on reading disabilities (pp. 162–172). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Goswami, U. (2002). Phonology, reading development and dyslexia: A cross-linguistic perspective. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U. (2012). Neuroscience and education: Can we go from basic research to translation? A possible framework from dyslexia research. Educational Neuroscience, B.J.E.P. Monograph Series, 11, 129–142.Google Scholar
Goswami, U., Gerson, D., & Astruc, L. (2010). Amplitude envelope perception, phonology and prosodic sensitivity in children with developmental dyslexia. Reading & Writing, 23, 995–1019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U., Huss, M., Mead, N., Fosker, T., & Verney, J. P. (2013). Perception of patterns of musical beat distribution in phonological developmental dyslexia: Significant longitudinal relations with word reading and reading comprehension. Cortex, 49(5), 1363–1376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goswami, U., Mead, N., Fosker, T., Huss, M., Barnes, L., & Leong, V. (2013). Impaired perception of syllable stress in children with dyslexia: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language 69, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U., & Szucs, D. (2011). Educational neuroscience, developmental mechanisms: Towards a conceptual framework. Neuroimage, 57, 651–658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S., et al. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(16), 10911–10916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goswami, U., Wang, H. L., Cruz, A., Fosker, T., Mead, N., & Huss, M. (2011). Language-universal sensory deficits in developmental dyslexia: English, Spanish, and Chinese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(2), 325–337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gough, P. B. (1983). Context, form and interaction. In Rayner, K. (Ed.), Eye movements in reading: Perceptual and language processes (pp. 203–211). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, P. E. (2012). Evolution of pediatric neuroimaging and application of cutting-edge techniques. In Benasich, A. A. & Fitch, R. H. (Eds.), Developmental dyslexia: Early precursors, neurobehavioral markers, and biological substrates (pp. 227–240). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Gray, S. A., Chaban, P., Martinussen, R., Goldberg, R., Gotlieb, H., Kronitz, R., Hockenberry, M. and Tannock, R. (2012). Effects of a computerized working memory training program on working memory, attention, and academics in adolescents with severe LD and comorbid ADHD: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 1277–1284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenspan, S. (2005). Credulity and gullibility among service providers: An attempt to understand why snake oil sells. In Jacobson, J. W., Foxx, R. M., & Mulick, J. A. (Eds.), Controversial therapies for developmental disabilities: Fad, fashion and science in professional practice (pp. 129–138). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gresham, F. M. (2009). Using response to intervention for identification of specific learning disabilities. In Akin-Little, A., Little, S. G., Bray, M. A., & Kehl, T. J. (Eds.), Behavioral interventions in schools: Evidence-based positive strategies (pp. 205–220). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gresham, F. M., & Vellutino, F.R. (2010). What is the role of intelligence in the identification of specific learning disabilities? Issues and clarifications. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 25(4), 194–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, Y. M., Hill, N. I., Bailey, P. J., & Snowling, M. J. (2003). Auditory temporal order discrimination and backward recognition masking in adults with dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1352–1366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigorenko, E. L. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: An update on genes, brains, and environments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 91–125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigorenko, E. L. (2004). Genetic bases of developmental dyslexia: A capsule review of heritability estimates. Enfance, 3, 273–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L. (2005). A conservative meta-analysis of linkage and linkage-association studies of developmental dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 285–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L. (2006). Learning disabilities in juvenile offenders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 15, 353–371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigorenko, E. L. (2007). Triangulating developmental dyslexia: Behavior, brain, and genes. In Coch, D., Dawson, G., & Fischer, K. (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain (pp. 117–144). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L. (2009). At the height of fashion: what genetics can teach us about neurodevelopmental disabilities. Current Opinion in Neurology, 22, 126–130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigorenko, E. L. (2011). Language-based learning disabilities. In Seel, N. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the sciences of learning. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L. (2012). Commentary: Translating quantitative genetics into molecular genetics: decoupling reading disorder and ADHD – reflections on Greven et al. and Rosenberg et al. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(3), 252–253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigorenko, E. L., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003). Hyperlexia: Disability or superability?Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 1079–1091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigorenko, E. L., & Naples, A. (Eds.). (2008). Single-word reading: Biological and behavioral perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Grigorenko, E. L., & Naples, A. J. (2009). The devil is in the details: Decoding the genetics of reading. In McCardle, P. & Pugh, K. (Eds.), Helping children learn to read: Current issues and new directions in the integration of cognition, neurobiology and genetics of reading and dyslexia (pp. 133–148). New York: Psychological Press.Google Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Dynamic testing. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 111–132.Google Scholar
Grills-Taquechel, A. E., Fletcher, J. M., Vaughn, S. R., & Stuebing, K. K. (2012). Anxiety and reading difficulties in early elementary school: Evidence for unidirectional- or bi-directional relations? Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 43, 35–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross-Glenn, K., Skottun, B. C., Glenn, W., Kushch, A., Lingua, R., Dunbar, M., et al. (1995). Contrast sensitivity in dyslexia. Visual Neuroscience, 12, 153–163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grünling, C., Ligges, M., Huonker, R., Klingert, M., Mentzel, H. J., Rzanny, R., et al. (2004). Dyslexia: The possible benefit of multimodal integration of fMRI- and EEG-data. Journal of Neural Transmission, 111, 951–969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunn, B., Smolkowski, K., Biglan, A., Black, C., & Blair, J. (2005). Fostering the development of reading skill through supplemental instruction: Results for Hispanic and non-Hispanic students. The Journal of Special Education, 39, 66–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guthrie, J. T., & Davis, M. H. (2003). Motivating struggling readers in middle school through an engagement model of classroom practice. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 19, 59–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gwernan-Jones, R., & Burden, R. L. (2010). Are they just lazy? Student teachers’ attitudes about dyslexia. Dyslexia, 16, 66–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Habib, M. (2000). The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia. Brain, 123, 2373–2399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Habib, M., Robichon, F., Levrier, O., Khalil, R., & Salamon, G. (1995). Diverging asymmetries of temporo-parietal cortical areas: a reappraisal of Geschwind/Galaburda theory. Brain and Language, 48, 238–258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 65–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., & Meaney, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 651–659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haddow, J. E., & Palomaki, G. E. (2003). ACCE: A model process for evaluating data on emerging genetic tests. In Khoury, M., Little, J., & Burke, W. (Eds.), Human genome epidemiology: A scientific foundation for using genetic information to improve health and prevent disease (pp. 217–233). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hadzibeganovic, T., van den Noort, M., Bosch, P., Perc, M., van Kralingen, R., Mondt, K., et al. (2011). Cross-linguistic neuroimaging and dyslexia: A critical view. Cortex, 46, 1312–1316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagan-Burke, S., Coyne, M. D., Kwok, O., Simmons, D., Kim, M., Simmons, L., et al. (2013). The effects and interactions of student, teacher, and setting variables on reading outcomes for kindergarteners receiving supplemental reading intervention. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(3), 260–277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hale, J. B., Alfonso, V., Berninger, V., Bracken, B., Christo, C., Clark, E., et al. (2010). Critical issues in response to intervention, comprehensive evaluation, and specific learning disabilities evaluation and intervention: An expert white paper consensus. Learning Disability Quarterly, 33, 223–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hale, J. B., Fiorello, C. A., Miller, J. A., Wenrich, K., Teodori, A. M., & Henzel, J. (2008). WISC-IV assessment and intervention strategies for children with specific learning difficulties. In Prifitera, A., Saklofske, D. H., & Weiss, L. G. (Eds.), WISC-IV clinical assessment and intervention (pp. 109–171). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Hale, J. B., Kaufman, A., Naglieri, J., & Kavale, K. (2006). Implementation of IDEA: Integrating response to intervention and cognitive assessment methods. Psychology in the Schools, 43, 753–770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hallahan, D. P., & Mercer, C. D. (2001). Learning disabilities: Historical perspectives. Washington, DC: Department of Education, Office of Special Education Programs.Google Scholar
Hallahan, D. P., & Mock, D. R. (2003). A brief history of the field of learning disabilities. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 16–29). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, L. F., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2006). Is poor frequency modulation detection linked to literacy problems? A comparison of specific reading disability and mild to moderate sensorineural hearing loss. Brain and Language, 97, 200–213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hämäläinen, J. A., Salminen, H. K., & Leppänen, P. H. T. (2013). Basic auditory processing deficits in dyslexia: review of the behavioural, event-related potential and magnetoencephalographic evidence. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(5), 413–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Handler, S. M., Fierson, W. M., the Section of Ophthalmology and Council on Children with Disabilities, American Academy of Ophthalmology, American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus, & and American Association of Certified Orthoptists. (2011). Joint technical report – Learning disabilities, dyslexia, and vision. Pediatrics, 127, e818–e856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hannula-Jouppi, K., Kaminen-Ahola, N., Taipale, M., Eklund, R., Nopola-Hemmi, J., Kääriäinen, H., et al. (2005). The axon guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate dene for developmental dyslexia. PLoS, 1, e50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hari, R., & Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 525–532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlaar, N., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2007). From learning to read to reading to learn: Substantial and stable genetic influence. Child Development, 78, 116–131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harn, B., Parisi, D., & Stoolmiller, M. (2013). Balancing fidelity with flexibility and fit: What do we really know about fidelity of implementation in schools?Exceptional Children, 79(2), 181–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harn, B. A., Kame'enui, E., & Simmons, D. C. (2007). The nature and role of the third tier in a prevention model for kindergarten students. In Haager, D., Klingner, J., & Vaughn, S. (Eds.), Evidence-based reading practices for responses to intervention (pp. 161–184). Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.Google Scholar
Harn, B. A., Linan-Thompson, S., & Roberts, G. (2008). Intensifying instruction does additional instructional time make a difference for the most at-risk first graders? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(2), 115–125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (2003). The early catastrophe. American Educator, 27, 6–9.Google Scholar
Hart, S. A., Petrill, S. A., DeThorne, L. S., Deater-Deckard, K., Thompson, L. A., Schatschneider, C., et al. (2009). Environmental influences on the longitudinal covariance of expressive vocabulary: measuring the home literacy environment in a genetically sensitive design. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 911–919.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartas, D. (2011). Families’ social backgrounds matter: Socioeconomic factors, home learning and young children's language, literacy and social outcomes. British Educational Research Journal, 37, 893–914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasan, K. M., Molfese, D. L., Walimuni, I. S., Stuebing, K. K., Papanicolaou, A. C., Narayana, P. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (2012). Diffusion tensor quantification and cognitive correlates of the macrostructure and microstructure of the corpus callosum in typically developing and dyslexic children. NMR in Biomedicine, 25(11), 1263–1270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasbrouck, J. E., Ihnot, C., & Rogers, G. (1999). “Read naturally”: A strategy to increase oral reading fluency. Reading Research and Instruction, 39, 27–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., Miles, J. N., Carroll, J. M., Hatcher, J., Gibbs, S., et al. (2006). Efficacy of small group reading intervention for beginning readers with reading delay: A randomised controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 47, 820–827.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Harlaar, N., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2010). Preschool speech, language skills, and reading at 7, 9, and 10 Years: Etiology of the relationshipJournal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 311–332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heath, S. M., Bishop, D. V. M., Hogben, J., & Roach, N. (2006). Psychophysical indices of perceptual functioning in dyslexia: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(6), 905–929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heath, S. M., & Hogben, J. H. (2004). Cost-effective prediction of reading difficulties. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 751–765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, P., & Winterton, P. (1996). Dealing with dyslexia (2nd ed.). London: Whurr Publishers.Google Scholar
Heiervang, E., Hugdahl, K., Steinmetz, H., Inge Smievoll, A., Stevenson, J., Lund, A., et al. (2000). Planum temporale, planum parietale and dichotic listening in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1704–1713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heim, S., & Grande, M. (2012). Fingerprints of developmental dyslexia. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 1(1), 10–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heim, S., & Keil, A. (2004). Large-scale neural correlates of developmental dyslexia. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 125–140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helenius, P., Tarkiainen, A., Cornelissen, P., Hansen, P., & Salmelin, R. (1999). Dissociation of normal feature analysis and deficient processing of letter-strings in dyslexic adults. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 476–483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemminki, K., Forsti, A., Houlston, R., & Bermejo, J. L. (2011). Searching for the missing heritability of complex diseases. Human Mutation, 32, 259–262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, L. M., Tsogda, N., & Snowling, M. J. (2013). Questioning the benefits that coloured overlays can have for reading in students with and without dyslexia. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 13(1), 57–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herbers, J. E., Cutuli, J. J., Supkoff, L. M., Heistad, D., Chan, C., Hinz, E., & Masten, A. S. (2012). Early reading skills and academic achievement trajectories of students facing poverty, homelessness, and high residential mobility. Educational Researcher, 41(9), 366–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrington, M., & Hunter-Carsch, M. (2001). A social interactive model of specific learning difficulties. In Hunter-Carsch, M. (Ed.), Dyslexia: A psycho-social perspective (pp. 107–133). London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Hinshelwood, J. (1895). Word-blindness and visual memory. Lancet, 146, 1564–1570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinshelwood, J. (1902). Congenital word-blindness, with reports of two cases. Ophthalmology Review, 21, 91–99.Google Scholar
Hinshelwood, J. (1907). Four cases of congenital word-blindness occurring in the same family. British Medical Journal, 1, 608–609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinshelwood, J. (1917). Congenital word blindness. London: H. K. Lewis & Co.Google Scholar
Ho, A. (2004). To be labelled, or not to be labelled: That is the question. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 86–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoeft, F., Hernandez, A., McMillon, G., Taylor-Hill, H., Martindale, J. L., Meyler, A., et al. (2006). Neural basis of dyslexia: A comparison between dyslexic and nondyslexic children equated for reading ability. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 10700–10708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoeft, F., McCandliss, B. D., Black, J. M., Gantman, A., Zakerani, N., Hulme, C., et al. (2011). Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 361–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoeft, F., Meyler, A., Hernandez, A., Juel, C., Taylor-Hill, H., Martindale, J. L., et al. (2007). Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 4234–4239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogan, T. P., & Thomson, J. M. (2010). Epilogue to Journal of Learning Disabilities special edition “Advances in the early detection of reading risk”: Future advances in the early detection of reading risk: Subgroups, dynamic relations, and advanced methods. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 383–386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holliman, A. J., Wood, C., & Sheehy, K. (2010). Does speech rhythm sensitivity predict children's reading ability one year later? Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 356–366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holm, V. A. (1983). A western version of the Doman-Delacato eatment of patterning for developmental disabilities. The Western Journal of Medicine, 139, 553–556.Google Scholar
Holmes, J., & Gathercole, S. E. (in press). Taking working memory training from the laboratory into schools, Educational Psychology.
Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Dunning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science, 12, 9–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoogeven, J., Smeets, P., & Lancioni, G. (1989). Teaching moderately mentally retarded children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 10, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornickel, J., & Kraus, N. (2013). Unstable representation of sound: A biological marker of dyslexia. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(8), 3500–3504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hornickel, J., Zecker, S. G., Bradlow, A.R., & Kraus, N. (2012). Assistive listening devices drive neuroplasticity in children with dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 16731–16736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horwitz, B., Rumsey, J. M., & Donohue, B. C. (1998). Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 8939–8944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoskyn, M., & Swanson, H. L. (2000). Cognitive processing of low achievers and children with reading disabilities: A selective meta-analytic review of the published literature. School Psychology Review 29, 102–119.Google Scholar
House of Commons. Science and Technology Committee. (2009). Evidence check 1: Early literacy interventions. London: The Stationery Office.Google Scholar
Howard, D., & Best, W. (1996). Developmental phonological dyslexia: Word reading can be completely normal. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 887–934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howard-Jones, P. A. (2007). Neuroscience and education: Issues and opportunities. Commentary by the Teaching and Learning Research Programme. London: TLRP.Google Scholar
Hu, W., Lee, H. L., Zhang, Q., Liu, T., Geng, L. B., Seghier, M. L., et al. (2010). Developmental dyslexia in Chinese and English populations: Dissociating the effect of dyslexia from language differences. Brain, 133, 1694–1706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huemer, S., Aro, M., Landerl, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2010). Repeated reading of syllables among Finnish-speaking children with poor reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 317–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hugdahl, K., Heiervang, E., Ersland, L., Lundervold, A., Steinmetz, H., & Smievoll, A. I. (2003). Significant relation between MR measures of planum temporale area and dichotic processing of syllables in dyslexic children. Neuropsychologia, 41, 666–675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (1992). Deficits in output phonology: An explanation of reading failure? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 47–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2009). Developmental disorders of language learning and cognition. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hulslander, J., Olson, R. K., Willcutt, E. G., & Wadsworth, S. J. (2010). Longitudinal stability of reading-related skills and their prediction of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 111–136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, P., Kaufmann, W. E., & Galaburda, A. M. (1990). Developmental dyslexia in women: Neuropathological findings in three patients. Annals of Neurology, 28, 727–738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurford, D. P., Potter, T. S., & Hart, G. S. (2002). Examination of three techniques for identifying first-grade children at risk for difficulty in word identification with an emphasis on reducing the false negative error rate. Reading Psychology, 23, 159–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutton, U. M. Z., & Towse, J. N. (2001). Short-term memory and working memory as indices of children's cognitive skills. Memory, 9, 383–394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutzler, F., Kronbichler, M., Jacobs, A. M., & Wimmer, H. (2006). Perhaps correlational but not causal: No effect of dyslexic readers’ magnocellular system on their eye movements during reading. Neuropsychologia, 44, 637–648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyatt, K. J. (2007). Brain Gym®: Building stronger brains or wishful thinking? Remedial and Special Education, 28, 117–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyatt, K. J., Stephenson, J., & Carter, M. (2009). A review of three controversial educational practices: Perceptual motor programs, sensory integration, and tinted lenses. Education and Treatment of Children, 32, 313–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hynd, G. W., Hall, J., Novey, E. S., Eliopulos, D., Black, K., Gonzalez, J. J., et al. (1995). Dyslexia and corpus callosum morphology. Archives of Neurology, 52, 32–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hynd, G. W., Semrud-Clikeman, M., Lorys, A. R., Novey, E. S., & Eliopulos, D. (1990). Brain morphology in developmental dyslexia and attention deficit disorder/hyperactivity. Archives of Neurology, 47, 919–926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Igo, R. P. J., Chapman, N. H., Berninger, V. W., Matsushita, M., Brkanac, Z.,Rothstein, J. H., et al. (2006). Genomewide scan for real-word reading subphenotypes of dyslexia: novel chromosome 13 locus and genetic complexity. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 141, 15–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ihnot, C., Mastoff, J., Gavin, J., & Hendrickson, L. (2001). Read naturally [Curriculum program]. St. Paul, MN: Read Naturally.Google Scholar
Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act, H.R. 1350, 108th Congress (2004).
Ingvar, M., af Trampe, P., Greitz, T., Eriksson, L., Stone-Elander, S., & von Euler, C. (2002). Residual differences in language processing in compensated dyslexics revealed in simple word reading tasks. Brain & Language, 83, 249–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
International Dyslexia Association Scientific Advisory Board (2012). Diagnosis and treatment of dyslexia: Comments on proposed DSM-5 criteria for learning disabilities (disorders) (May 29). Retrieved March 30, 2013 from
Irlen, H. (1991). Reading by the colors: Overcoming dyslexia and other reading disabilities through the Irlen method. New York: Avery.Google Scholar
Jacobson, J. W., Foxx, R. M., & Mulick, J. A. (2005). Controversial therapies for developmental disabilities: Fad, fashion and science in professional practice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jainta, S., & Kapoula, Z. (2011). Dyslexic children are confronted with unstable binocular fixation while reading. PlosOne, 6, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jalal, S. M., Harwood, A. R., Sekhon, G. S., Lorentz, C. P., Ketterling, R. P., Babovic-Vuksanovic, D., et al. (2003). Utility of subtelomeric fluorescent DNA probes for detection of chromosome anomalies in 425 patients. Genetics in Medicine, 5, 28–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeanes, R., Busby, A., Martin, J., Lewis, E., Stevenson, N., Pointon, D., et al. (1997). Prolonged use of coloured overlays for classroom reading. British Journal of Psychology, 88, 531–548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jednoróg, K., Altarelli, I., Monzalvo, K., Fluss, J., Dubois, J., Billard, C., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Ramus, F. (2012). The influence of socioeconomic status on children's brain structure. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e42486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeffries, S., & Everatt, J. (2004). Working memory: Its role in dyslexia and other specific learning difficulties. Dyslexia, 10, 196–214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkins, J. R., Hudson, R. F., & Johnson, E. S. (2007). Screening for at-risk readers in a response-to-intervention (RTI) framework. School Psychology Review, 36, 582–600.Google Scholar
Jenkins, J. R., & O’Connor, R. E. (2002). Early identification and intervention for young children with reading/learning disabilities. In Bradley, R., Danielson, L., & Hallahan, D. P. (Eds.), Identification of learning disabilities: Research to practice (pp. 99–149). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jenner, A. R., Rosen, G. D., & Galaburda, A. M. (1999). Neuronal asymmetries in primary visual cortex of dyslexic and nondyslexic brains. Annals of Neurology, 46, 189–196.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jiménez, J. E., de la Cadena, C., Siegel, L., O’Shanahan, I., García, E., & Rodríguez, C. (2011). Gender ratio and cognitive profiles in dyslexia: A cross-national study. Reading and Writing, 24, 729–747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiménez, J. E., & Garcia de la Cadena, C. (2007). Learning disabilities in Guatemala and Spain: A cross-national study of the prevalence and cognitive processes associated with reading and spelling disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 22, 161–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jobard, G., Crivello, F., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2003). Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading: A metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage, 20, 693–712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johannes, S., Kussmaul, C. L., Munte, T. F., & Mangun, G. R. (1996). Developmental dyslexia: Passive visual stimulation provides no evidence for a magnocellular processing deficit. Neuropsychologia, 34, 1123–1127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, A., Bruno, A., Watanabe, J., Quansah, B., Patel, N., Dakin, S., et al. (2008). Visually-based temporal distortion in dyslexia. Vision Research, 48, 1852–1858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E. P., Pennington, B. F., Lee, N. R., & Boada, R. (2009). Directional effects between rapid auditory processing and phonological awareness in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 902–910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, E. P., Pennington, B. F., Lowenstein, J. H., & Nittrouer, S. (2011). Sensitivity to structure in the speech signal by children with speech sound disorder and reading disability. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44, 294–314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, E. S., Humphrey, M., Mellard, D. F., Woods, K., & Swanson, H. L. (2010). Cognitive processing deficits and students with specific learning disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Learning Disability Quarterly, 33, 3–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E. S., Jenkins, J. R., Petscher, Y., & Catts, H. W. (2009). How can we improve the accuracy of screening instruments? Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 24, 174–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, M. W., Branigan, H. P., Hatzidaki, A., & Obregón, A. (2010). Is the “naming” deficit in dyslexia a misnomer? Cognition, 116, 56–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juel, C., & Minden-Cupp, C. (2000). Learning to read words: Linguistic units and instructional strategies. Reading Research Quarterly, 35, 458–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R., & Hall, L. K. (1994). Processing speed, naming speed, and reading. Developmental Psychology, 30, 949–954.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kairaluoma, L., Nӓrhi, V., Ahonen, T., Westerholm, J., & Aro, M. (2008). Do fatty acids help in overcoming reading difficulties? A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of the effects of eicosapentaenoic acid and carnosine supplementation on children with dyslexia. Child: Care, Health and Development, 35, 112–119.Google ScholarPubMed
Kamerow, D. (2008). Waiting for the genetic revolution. British Medical Journal, 336, 22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamhi, A. G. (2004). A meme's eye view of speech-language pathology. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 105–111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamhi, A. G. (2009). Solving the reading crisis – Take 2: The case for differentiated assessment. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 40, 212–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaminen, N., Hannula-Jouppi, K., Kestila, M., Lahermo, P., Muller, K., Kaaranen, M., et al. (2003). A genome scan for developmental dyslexia confirms linkage to chromosome 2p11 and suggests a new locus on 7q32. Journal of Medical Genetics, 40, 340–345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamps, D., Abbott, M., Greenwood, C., Wills, H., Veerkamp, M., & Kaufman, J. (2008). Effects of small group reading instruction and curriculum differences for students most at risk in kindergarten: Two-year results for secondary- and tertiary-level interventions. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 101–114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, B. J., Wilson, N. B., Dewey, D., & Crawford, S. G. (1998). DCD may not be a discrete disorder. Human Movement Science, 17, 471–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapoula, Z., Ganem, R., Poncet, S., Gintautas, D., Eggert, T., Brémond-Gignac, D., & Bucci, M.P. (2008). Free exploration of painting uncovers particularly loose yoking of saccades in dyslexics. Dyslexia, 15(3), 243–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katusic, S. K., Colligan, R. C., Barbaresi, W. J., Schaid, D. J., & Jacobsen, S. J. (2001). Incidence of reading disability in a population-based birth cohort, 1976–1982, Rochester, Minnesota. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 76, 1081–1092.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katzir, T., Misra, M., & Poldrack, R. A. (2005). Imaging phonology without print: assessing the neural correlates of phonemic awareness using fMRI. Neuroimage, 27, 106–115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kauffman, J. M., Hallahan, D. P., & Lloyd, J. W. (1998). Politics, science, and the future of learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 21, 276–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, A. S. (1994). Intelligent Testing with the WISC-III. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (1984). A meta-analysis of the validity of Wechsler scale profiles and recategorisations: Patterns or parodies? Learning Disability Quarterly, 7, 136–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (1995). The nature of learning disabilities. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (2003). Learning disability as a disciple. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 76–93). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kavale, K. A., & Mattson, P. D. (1983). “One jumped off the balance beam”: Meta-analysis of perceptual-motor training. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 16, 165–173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavale, K. A., & Mostert, M. P. (2004). The positive side of special education: Minimizing its fads, fancies, and follies. Lanham, MD: Scarecrow Education.Google Scholar
Kavale, K. A., & Spaulding, L. S. (2008). Is response to intervention good policy for specific learning disability?Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 23, 169–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kavale, K. A., Spaulding, L. S., & Beam, A. P. (2009). A time to define: Making the specific learning disability definition prescribe specific learning disability. Learning Disability Quarterly, 32, 39–48.Google Scholar
Kearns, D., & Fuchs, D. (2013). Does cognitively focused instruction improve the academic performance of low-achieving students? Exceptional Children, 79(3), 263–290.Google Scholar
Keenan, J. M., Betjemann, R., Wadsworth, S., DeFries, J., & Olson, R. (2006). Genetic and environmental influences on reading and listening comprehension. Journal of Research In Reading, 29, 75–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, T. A., & Just, M. A. (2009). Altering cortical connectivity: Remediation-induced changes in the white matter of poor readers. Neuron, 64, 624–631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, K., & Phillips, S. (2011). Teaching literacy to learners with dyslexia: A multisensory approach. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Kendell, R. E. (1975). The role of diagnosis in psychiatry. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kersting, K. (2004, October). Debating learning-disability identification. APA Monitor, 54–55.Google Scholar
Khoury, M. J., Bowen, S., Bradley, L. A., Coates, R., Dowling, N. F., Gwinn, M., et al. (2008). A decade of public health genomics in the United States: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 1997–2007. Public Health Genomics, 12, 20–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khoury, M. J., Gwinn, M., Yoon, P. W., Dowling, N., Moore, C. A., & Bradley, L. (2007). The continuum of translation research in genomic medicine: How can we accelerate the appropriate integration of human genome discoveries into health care and disease prevention?Genetics in Medicine, 9, 665–674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khoury, M. J., Little, J., Gwinn, M., & Ioannidis, J. P. (2007). On the synthesis and interpretation of consistent but weak gene-disease associations in the era of genome-wide association studies. International Journal of Epidemiology, 36, 439–445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khoury, M. J., Valdez, R., & Albright, A. (2008). Public health genomics approach to type 2 diabetes. Diabetes, 57, 2911–2914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kibby, M. Y., Fancher, J. B., Markanen, R., & Hynd, G. W. (2008). A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging analysis of the cerebellar deficit hypothesis of dyslexia. Journal of Child Neurology, 23, 368–380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kibby, M. Y., Marks, W., Morgan, S., & Long, C. J. (2004). Specific impairment in developmental reading disabilities: A working memory approach. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 349–363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kieffer, M. J. (2012). Before and after third grade: Longitudinal evidence for the shifting role of socioeconomic status in reading growth. Reading and Writing, 25(7), 1725–1746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kipp, K. H., & Mohr, G. (2008). Remediation of developmental dyslexia: Tackling a basic memory deficit. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 38–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirby, A., Woodward, A., Jackson, S., Wang, Y., & Crawford, M. A. (2010). A double-blind, placebo-controlled study investigating the effects of omega-3 supplementation in children aged 8–10 years from a mainstream school population. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 718–730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirby, J. R., Georgiou, G. K., Martinussen, R., Parrila, R., Bowers, P., & Landerl, K. (2010). Naming speed and reading: From prediction to instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 341–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, J., & Reid, G. (2001). An examination of the relationship between dyslexia and offending in young people and the implications for the training system. Dyslexia, 7, 77–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirk, S. A. (1962). Educating exceptional children. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Kiuru, N., Haverinen, K., Salmela-Aro, K., Nurmi, J. E., Savolainen, H., & Holopainen, L. (2011). Students with reading and spelling disabilities: Peer groups and educational attainment in secondary education. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44, 556–569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, R. M., & Farmer, M. E. (1995). Dyslexia and a temporal processing deficit: A reply to the commentaries. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 515–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klicpera, C., & Schabmann, A. (1993). Do German-speaking children have a chance to overcome reading and spelling difficulties? A longitudinal survey from the second until the eighth grade. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8, 307–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 317–324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klingberg, T., Hedehus, M., Temple, E., Salz, T., Gabrieli, J. D., Moseley, M. E., et al. (2000). Microstructure of temporo-parietal white matter as a basis for reading ability: evidence from diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. Neuron, 25, 493–500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klingberg, T., Vaidya, C. J., Gabrieli, J. D., Moseley, M. E., & Hedehus, M. (1999). Myelination and organization of the frontal white matter in children: a diffusion tensor MRI study. Neuroreport, 10, 2817–2821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochunov, P., Fox, P., Lancaster, J., Tan, L. H., Amunts, K., Zilles, K., et al. (2003). Localized morphological brain differences between English-speaking Caucasians and Chinese-speaking Asians: New evidence of anatomical plasticity. Neuroreport, 14, 961–964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koponen, T., Salmi, P., Eklund, K., & Aro, T. (2013). Counting and RAN: Predictors of arithmetic calculation and reading fluency. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 105(1), 162–175.Google Scholar
Kortteinen, H., Närhi, V., & Ahonen, T. (2009). Does IQ matter in adolescents’ reading disability? Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 257–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, J. J., Henning-Stout, M., Ullman, D. P., & Schellenberg, R. P. (1987). The viability of scatter analysis on the WISC-R and the SBIS: Examining a vestige. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 5, 37–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kriss, I., & Evans, B. J. W. (2005). The relationship between dyslexia and Meares-Irlen syndrome. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 350–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kronbichler, M., Hutzler, F., Staffen, W., Mair, A., Ladurner, G., & Wimmer, H. (2006). Evidence for a dysfunction of left posterior reading areas in German dyslexic readers. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1822–1832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kronbichler, M., Hutzler, F., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Dyslexia: Verbal impairments in the absence of magnocellular impairments. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology, 13, 617–620.Google ScholarPubMed
Kronbichler, M., Wimmer, H., Staffen, W., Hutzler, F., Mair, A., & Ladurner, G. (2008). Developmental dyslexia: Gray matter abnormalities in the occipitotemporal cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 29, 613–625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kujala, T., Karma, K., Ceponiene, R., Belitz, S., Turkkila, P., Tervaniemi, M., et al. (2001). Plastic neural changes and reading improvement caused by audiovisual training in reading-impaired children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 10509–10514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kushch, A., Gross-Glenn, K., Jallad, B., Lubs, H., Rabin, M., Feldman, E., et al. (1993). Temporal lobe surface area measurements on MRI in normal and dyslexic readers. Neuropsychologia, 31, 811–821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kussmaul, L. A. (1877). Disturbances of speech. In von Ziemssen, H. (Ed.), Cyclopedia of the Practice of Medicine, Vol. 14. New York: William Wood and Co.Google Scholar
Laasonen, M., Service, E., & Virsu, V. (2001). Temporal order and processing acuity of visual, auditory, and tactile perception in developmentally dyslexic young adults. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 1, 394–410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laasonen, M., Virsu, V., Oinonen, S., Sandbacka, M., Salakari, A., & Service, E. (2012), Phonological and sensory short-term memory are correlates and both affected in developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 25, 2247–2273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2007). Paradoxical enhancement of letter recognition in developmental dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 31, 61–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lack, D. (2010). Another joint statement regarding learning disabilities, dyslexia, and vision – A rebuttal. Optometry, 81, 533–543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lackaye, T. D., & Margalit, M. (2006). Comparisons of achievement, effort, and self-perceptions among students with learning disabilities and their peers from different achievement groups. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 432–446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lallier, M., Donnadieu, S., Berger, C., & Valdois, S. (2010). A case study of developmental phonological dyslexia: Is the attentional deficit in the perception of rapid stimuli sequences amodal?Cortex, 46, 231–241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lallier, M., Tainturier, M., Dering, B., Donnadieu, S., Valdois, S., & Thierry, G. (2010). Behavioral and ERP evidence for amodal sluggish attentional shifting in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 4125–4135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lallier, M., Thierry, G., Tainturier, M., Donnadieu, S., Peyrin, C., Billard, C., et al. (2009). Auditory and visual stream segregation in children and adults: An assessment of the amodality assumption of the sluggish attentional shifting theory. Brain Research, 1302, 132–147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lander, E. S. (2011). Initial impact of the sequencing of the human genome. Nature, 470, 187–197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landerl, K., & Moll, K. (2010). Comorbidity of learning disorders: Prevalence and familial transmission. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 287–294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landerl, K., Ramus, F., Moll, K., Lyytinen, H., Leppänen, P., et al. (2013). Predictors of developmental dyslexia in European orthographies with varying complexity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(6), 686–694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landerl, K., & Willburger, E. (2010). Temporal processing, attention, and learning disorders. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 393–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2000). Deficits in phoneme segmentation are not the core problem in dyslexia: Evidence from German and English children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 243–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsen, J. P., Hoien, T., Lundberg, I., & Odegaard, H. (1990). MRI evaluation of the size and symmetry of the planum temporale in adolescents with developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 39, 289–301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsen, J. P., Hoien, T., & Odegaard, H. (1992). Magnetic resonance imaging of the corpus callosum in developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 123–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laycock, R., Crewther, D. P., & Crewther, S. G. (2012). Abrupt and ramped flicker-defined form shows evidence for a large magnocellular impairment in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 50, 2107–2113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laycock, S. K., Wilkinson, I. D., Wallis, L. I., Darwent, G., Wonders, S. H., Fawcett, A. J., et al. (2008). Cerebellar volume and cerebellar metabolic characteristics in adults with dyslexia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145, 222–236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lebel, C., Shaywitz, B., Holahan, J., Shaywitz, S. E., & Machione, K. (2013). Diffusion tensor imaging correlates of reading ability in dysfluent and non-impaired readers. Brain & Language, 125(2), 215–222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Jan, G., R., L. B.-J., Costet, N., Trolès, N., Scalart, P., Pichancourt, D., et al. (2011). Multivariate predictive model for dyslexia diagnosis. Annals of Dyslexia, 61, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leach, J. M., Scarborough, H. S., & Rescorla, L. (2003). Late-emerging reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 211–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, R., Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2001). Pre-School Screening Test (PREST). Oxford: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Leonard, C. M., & Eckert, M. A. (2008). Asymmetry and dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(6), 663–681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, C. M., Eckert, M. A., Lombardino, L. J., Oakland, T., Kranzler, J., Mohr, C. M., et al. (2001). Anatomical risk factors for phonological dyslexia. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 148–157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leppanen, P. H., & Lyytinen, H. (1997). Auditory event-related potentials in the study of developmental language-related disorders. Audiology & Neuro Otology, 2, 308–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leppänen, P. H. T., Hämäläinen, J. A., Salminen, H. K., Eklund, K. M., Guttorm, T. K., Lohvansuu, K., et al. (2010). Newborn brain event-related potentials revealing atypical processing of sound frequency and the subsequent association with later literacy skills in children with familial dyslexia. Cortex, 46, 1362–1376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lervåg, A., Bråten, I., & Hulme, C. (2009). The cognitive and linguistic foundations of early reading development: A Norwegian latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 45, 764–781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liao, C. H., Georgiou, G. K., & Parrila, R. (2008). Rapid naming speed and Chinese character recognition. Reading and Writing, 21, 231–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, A. M. (1999). The reading researcher and the reading teacher need the right theory of speech. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 95–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., & Shankweiler, D. P. (1985). Phonology and the problems of learning to read and write. Remedial and Special Education, 6, 8–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liddle, E., Jackson, G., & Jackson, S. (2005). An evaluation of a visual biofeedback intervention in dyslexic adults. Dyslexia, 11, 61–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lidz, C. S., & Elliott, J. G. (Eds.). (2000). Dynamic assessment: Prevailing models and applications. London: Elsevier.
Liederman, J., Kantrowitz, L., & Flannery, K. (2005). Male vulnerability to reading disability is not likely to be a myth: A call for new data. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 109–129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ligges, C., & Blanz, B. (2007). [Survey of fMRI results regarding a phonological deficit in children and adults with dyslexia: Fundamental deficit or indication of compensation?]. Zeitschrift fur Kinder und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 35, 107–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ligges, C., Ungureanu, M., Ligges, M., Blanz, B., & Witte, H. (2010). Understanding the time variant connectivity of the language network in developmental dyslexia: New insights using Granger causality. Journal of Neural Transmission, 117, 529–543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lim, K. O., & Helpern, J. A. (2002). Neuropsychiatric applications of DTI – a review. NMR Biomed, 15, 587–593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindmark, L., & Clough, P. (2007). A 5-month open study with long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in dyslexia. Journal of Medicinal Food, 10, 662–666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lionel, A. C., Crosbie, J., Barbosa, N., Goodale, T., Thiruvahindrapuram, B., Rickaby, J., et al. (2011). Rare copy number variation discovery and cross-disorder comparisons identify risk genes for ADHD. Science Translational Medicine, 3, 95–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipka, O., Lesaux, N. K., & Siegel, L. (2006). Retrospective analyses of the reading development of grade 4 students with reading disabilities: Risk status and profiles over 5 years. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 364–378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, L., Wang, W., You, W., Li, Y., Awati, N., et al. (2012). Similar alterations in brain function for phonological and semantic processing to visual characters in Chinese dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 50, 2224–2232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingstone, M. S., Rosen, G. D., Drislane, F. W., & Galaburda, A. M. (1991). Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 88, 7943–7947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lloyd, J. W., Hallahan, D. P., & Kauffman, J. M. (1980). Learning disabilities: Selected topics. In Mann, L. & Sabatino, D. (Eds.), The fourth review of special education (pp. 35–60). New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Lobier, M., Zoubrinetzky, R., & Valdois, S. (2012). The visual attention span deficit in dyslexia is visual and not verbal. Cortex, 48(6), 768–773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Łockiewicz, M., Bogdanowicz, K.M., & Bogdanowicz, M. (in press). Psychological resources of adults with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities.
Logan, J. (2009). Dyslexic entrepreneurs: The incidence, their coping strategies and their business skills. Dyslexia, 15, 328–346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loosli, S. V., Buschkuehl, M., Perrig, W. J., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2012). Working memory training improves reading processes in typically developing children. Child Neuropsychology, 18(1), 62–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopes, J. (2012). Biologising reading problems: The specific case of dyslexia. Contemporary Social Science, 7(2), 215–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovegrove, W. J., Bowling, A., Badcock, D., & Blackwood, M. (1980). Specific reading disability: differences in contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency. Science, 210, 439–440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovett, M. W., Barron, R. W., & Frijters, J. C. (2013). Word identification difficulties in children and adolescents with reading dusabilities. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K.R., and Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 329–360). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lu, X., Shaw, C. A., Patel, A., Li, J., Cooper, M. L., Wells, W. R., et al. (2007). Clinical implementation of chromosomal microarray analysis: Summary of 2513 postnatal cases. PLoS ONE, 2(3), e327. .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luciano, M., Evans, D. M., Hansell, N. K., Medland, S. E., Montgomery, G. W., et al. (2013). A genome‐wide association study for reading and language abilities in two population cohorts. Genes, Brain and Behavior.CrossRef
Lundberg, I., Larsman, P., & Strid, A. (2012). Development of phonological awareness during the preschool year: The influence of gender and socio-economic status. Reading and Writing, 25, 305–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyon, G. R. (1995). Towards a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 3–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyon, G. R., & Moats, L. C. (1997). Critical conceptual and methodological considerations in reading intervention research. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30, 578–588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyon, G. R., & Weiser, B. (2013). The state of the science in learning disabilities: Research impact on the field from 2001 to 2011. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 118–151). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lyytinen, H., Erskine, J., Tolvanen, A., Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A., & Lyytinen, P. (2006). Trajectories of reading development: A follow-up from birth to school age of children with and without risk for dyslexia. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 52, 514–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacArthur, C. A. (2013). Technology applications for improving literacy. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 565–590). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Macaruso, P., Locke, J., Smith, S. T., & Powers, S. (1995). Short-term memory and phonological coding in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 9, 135–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macdonald, S. (2009). Towards a social reality of dyslexia. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 271–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machek, G. R., & Nelson, J. M. (2007). How should reading disabilities be operationalized? A survey of practicing school psychologists. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 22, 147–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machin, S., & Pekkarinen, T. (2008). Global sex differences in test score variability. Science, 322, 1331–1332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macmillan, D. L., & Siperstein, G. N. (2002). Learning disabilities as operationally defined by schools. In Bradley, R., Danielson, L., & Hallahan, D. P. (Eds.), Identification of learning disabilities: Research to policy (pp. 287–333). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Maehler, C., & Schuchardt, K. (2009). Working memory in children with learning disabilities: Does intelligence make a difference? Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 53, 3–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maehler, C., & Schuchardt, K. (2011). Working memory in children with learning disabilities: Rethinking the criterion of disability. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 58(1), 5–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maisog, J. M., Einbinder, E. R., Flowers, D. L., Turkeltaub, P. E., & Eden, G. F. (2008). A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of dyslexia. In Eden, G. F. & Flowers, D. L. (Eds.), Learning, skill acquisition, reading, and dyslexia (pp. 237–259). New York: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M., & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and orthographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 325–333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, L. (1979). On the trail of process. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Marazzi, C. (2011). Dyslexia and the economy. Angelaki: Journal of the Theoretical Humanities, 16(3), 19–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markham, R. (2002). Developmental dyslexia. Focus: Occasional Update from the Royal College of Opthalmologists, No 23.
Marshall, C. M., Snowling, M. J., & Bailey, P. J. (2001). Rapid auditory processing and phonological ability in normal readers and readers with dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 925–940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martelli, M., Di Filippo, G., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2009). Crowding, reading, and developmental dyslexia. Journal of Vision, 9, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, N., & Wendling, B. J. (2012). Essentials of dyslexia assessment and intervention. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Mathes, P. G., & Denton, C. A. (2002). The prevention and identification of reading disability. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 9, 185–191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathes, P. G., Denton, C. A., Fletcher, J. M., Anthony, J. L., Francis, D. J., & Schatschneider, C. (2005). The effects of theoretically different instruction and student characteristics on the skills of struggling readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 40, 148–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattson, M. P. (2002). Neurogenetics: White matter matters. Trends in Neurosciences, 25, 135–136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maurer, U., Brem, S., Bucher, K., Kranz, F., Benz, R., Steinhausen, H. C., et al. (2007). Impaired tuning of a fast occipito-temporal response for print in dyslexic children learning to read. Brain, 130(12), 3200–3210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2006). Frequency of reading, math, and writing disabilities in children with clinical disorders. Learning and Individual Differences, 16, 145–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazzocco, M. M., & Grimm, J. J. (2013). Growth in rapid automatized naming from Grades K to 8 in children with math or reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(6), 517–533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArthur, G. (2007). Test-retest effects in treatment studies of reading disability: The devil is in the detail. Dyslexia, 13, 240–252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArthur, G. M. (2009). Auditory processing disorders: Can they be treated? Current Opinion in Neurology, 22, 137–143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArthur, G. M., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2001). Auditory perceptual processing in people with reading and oral language impairments: Current issues and recommendations. Dyslexia, 7, 150–170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArthur, G. M., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004). Frequency discrimination deficits in people with specific language impairment: Reliability, validity, and linguistic correlates. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 527–541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArthur, G. M., & Castles, A. (2013). Phonological processing deficits in specific reading disability and specific language impairment: Same or different?Journal of Research in Reading, 36(3), 280–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McArthur, G. M., Castles, A., Kohnen, S., Larsen, L., Jones, K., et al. (in press). Sight word and phonics training in children with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities.
McArthur, G. M., Ellis, D., Atkinson, C. M., & Coltheart, M. (2008). Auditory processing deficits in children with reading and language impairments: Can they (and should they) be treated? Cognition, 107, 946–977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArthur, G. M., & Hogben, J. H. (2012). Poor auditory task scores in children with specific reading and language difficulties: Some poor scores are more equal than others. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 63–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McArthur, G. M., Hogben, J. H., Edwards, V. T., Heath, S. M., & Mengler, E. D. (2000). On the “specifics” of specific reading disability and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(7), 869–874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCandliss, B. D., & Noble, K. G. (2003). The development of reading impairment: A cognitive neuroscience model. Mental Retardation & Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 9, 196–204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCardle, P., & Chhabra, V. (2004). The voice of evidence in reading research. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
McCardle, P., & Miller, B. (2012). Conclusion/next steps: Critical research directions and priorities. In Benasich, A. A. & Holly Fitch, R. (Eds.), Developmental dyslexia: Early precursors, neurobehavioral markers, and biological substrates (pp. 329–337). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
McCrory, E. J., Mechelli, A., Frith, U., & Price, C. J. (2005). More than words: A common neural basis for reading and naming deficits in developmental dyslexia? Brain, 128, 261–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDermott, R., Goldman, S., & Varenne, H. (2006). The cultural work of learning disabilities. Educational Researcher, 35, 12–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGee, R., Williams, S., Share, D. L., Anderson, J., & Silva, P. A. (1986). The relationship between specific reading retardation, general reading backwardness and behavioral problems in a large sample of Dunedin boys: A longitudinal study from five to eleven years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 597–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrath, L. M., Pennington, B. P., Shanahan, M. A., Santerre-Lemmon, L. E., Barnard, H. D., Willcutt, E. G., et al. (2011). A multiple deficit model of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Searching for shared cognitive deficits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 547–557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, R. D., & Ritchie, S. J. (2012). Rose-tinted? The use of coloured overlays to treat reading difficulties. In Sala, S. D. & Anderson, M. (Eds.), Neuroscience in education: The good, the bad and the ugly (pp. 230–243). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McKenzie, R. G. (2009). Obscuring vital distinctions: The oversimplification of learning disabilities within RTI. Learning Disability Quarterly, 32, 203–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLean, G. M. T., Castles, A., Coltheart, V., & Stuart, G. W. (2010). No evidence for a prolonged attentional blink in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 46, 1317–1329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLean, G. M. T., Stuart, G. W., Coltheart, V., & Castles, A. (2011). Visual temporal processing in dyslexia and the magnocellular deficit theory: The need for speed? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(6), 1957–1975.Google ScholarPubMed
McLeskey, J., & Waldron, N. L. (2002). Inclusion and school change: Teacher perceptions of curricular and instructional adaptations. Teacher Education and Special Education, 25, 41–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLeskey, J., & Waldron, N. L. (2011). Educational programs for elementary students with learning disabilities: Can they be both effective and inclusive? Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 26(1), 48–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLoughlin, D., & Leather, C. (2009). Dyslexia: Meeting the needs of employers and employees in the workplace. In Reid, G. (Ed.), The Routledge companion to dyslexia (pp. 286–294). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
McPhillips, M., Hepper, P. G., & Mulhern, G. (2000). Effects of replicating primary-reflex movements on specific reading difficulties in children: A randomised, double-blind, controlled trial. Lancet, 355, 537–541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McPhillips, M., & Jordan-Black, J. A. (2007). Primary reflex persistence in children with reading difficulties (dyslexia): A cross-sectional study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 748–754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meaburn, E., Harlaar, N., Craig, I., Schalkwyk, L., & Plomin, R. (2008). Quantitative trait locus association scan of early reading disability and ability using pooled DNA and 100K SNP microarrays in a sample of 5760 children. Molecular Psychiatry, 13, 729–740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mechelli, A., Price, C. J., Friston, K. J., et al., (2005). Voxel-based morphometry of the human brain: methods and applications. Current Methods in Imaging, 1, 105–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270–291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322–352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellard, D. F., Deshler, D. D., & Barth, A. (2004). LD identification: It's not simply a matter of building a better mousetrap. Learning Disability Quarterly, 27, 229–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menghini, D., Carlesimo, G. A., Marotta, L., Finzi, A., & Vicari, S. (2010). Developmental dyslexia and explicit long-term memory. Dyslexia, 16, 213–225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menghini, D., Finzi, A., Carlesimo, G. A., & Vicari, S. (2011). Working memory impairment in children with developmental dyslexia: Is it just a phonological deficit?Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 199–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menghini, D., Hagberg, G. E., Petrosini, L., Bozzali, M., Macaluso, E., Caltagirone, C., et al. (2008). Structural correlates of implicit learning deficits in subjects with developmental dyslexia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145, 212–221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merzenich, M. M., Jenkins, W. M., Johnston, P., Schreiner, C., Miller, S. L., & Tallal, P. (1996). Temporal processing deficits of language-learning impaired children ameliorated by training. Science, 271, 77–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, M. S., Wood, F. B., Hart, L. A., & Felton, R. H. (1998). Longitudinal course of rapid naming in disabled and nondisabled readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 91–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyler, A., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Lee, D., Hoeft, F., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., et al. (2007). Brain activation during sentence comprehension among good and poor readers. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2780–2787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Middleton, F. A., & Strick, P. L. (1997). Cerebellar output channels. International Review of Neurobiology, 41, 61–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miles, T. R., Haslum, M. N., & Wheeler, T. J. (1998). Gender ratio in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 27–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, D. T., Adam, M. P., Aradhya, S., Biesecker, L. G., Brothman, A. R., Carter, N. P., et al. (2010). Consensus statement: Chromosomal microarray is a first-tier clinical diagnostic test for individuals with developmental disabilities or congenital anomalies. American Journal of Human Genetics, 86, 749–764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moats, L. C., & Farrell, M. L. (1999). Multi-sensory instruction. In Birsh, J. (Ed.), Multi-sensory teaching of basic language skills (pp. 1–18). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Moats, L. C., & Farrell, M. L. (2005). Multisensory structured language education. In Birsh, J. (Ed.), Multi-sensory teaching of basic language skills (2nd ed., pp. 23–37). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Moats, L. C., & Foorman, B. (1997). Introduction to special issue of SSR: Components of effective reading instruction. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 187–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mody, M., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Brady, S. (1997). Speech perception deficits in poor readers: Auditory processing or phonological coding? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 199–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mol, S. E., & Bus, A. G. (2011). To read or not to read: A meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 267–296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molfese, D. L., Molfese, V. J., Barnes, M. E., Warren, C. G., & Molfese, P. J. (2008). Familial predictors of dyslexia: Evidence from preschool children with and without familial dyslexia risk. In Reid, G., Fawcett, A. J., Manis, F., & Siegel, L. S. (Eds.), The Sage handbook of dyslexia (pp. 99–121). London: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moll, K., Fussenegger, B., Willburger, E., & Landerl, K. (2009). RAN is not a measure of orthographic processing. Evidence from the asymmetric German orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moll, K., Loff, A., & Snowling, M. J. (2013). Cognitive endophenotypes of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(6), 385–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monzalvo, K., Fluss, J., Billard, C., Dehaene, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2012). Cortical networks for vision and language in dyslexic and normal children of variable socio-economic status. Neuroimage, 61, 258–274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moody, S., Vaughn, S., Hughes, M., & Fischer, M. (2000). Reading instruction in the resource room: Set up for failure. Exceptional Children, 53, 391–316.Google Scholar
Moonesinghe, R., Khoury, M. J., Liu, T., & Ioannidis, J. P. (2008). Required sample size and nonreplicability thresholds for heterogeneous genetic associations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 617–622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moores, E., Cassim, R., & Talcott, J. B. (2011). Adults with dyslexia exhibit large effects of crowding, increased dependence on cues, and detrimental effects of distractors in visual search tasks. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3881–3890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Tufis, P. A., & Sperling, R. A. (2008). Are reading and behaviour problems risk factors for each other? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 417–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, P. L., & Fuchs, D. (2007). Is there a bidirectional relationship between children's reading skills and reading motivation?Exceptional Children, 73, 165–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, P. L., Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., Cordray, D. S., & Fuchs, L. S. (2008). Does early reading failure decrease children's reading motivation?Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 387–404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, W. P. (1896). A case of congenital word-blindness (inability to learn to read). British Medical Journal, 2, 1543–1544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, R. D., Lovett, M. W., Wolf, M., Sevcik, R. A., Steinbach, K. A., Frijters, J. C., et al. (2012). Multiple-component remediation for developmental reading disabilities: I.Q., socioeconomic status, and race as factors in remedial outcome. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(2), 99–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, R. D., Steubing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Lyon, G. R., Shankweiler, D. P., et al. (1998). Subtypes of reading disability: Variability around a phonological core. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 347–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moss, M., Fountain, A. R., Boulay, B., Horst, M., Rodger, C., & Brown-Lyons, M. (2008). Reading First implementation evaluation: Final Report. Cambridge, MA: ABT Associates.Google Scholar
Mugnaini, D., Lassi, S., La Malfa, G., & Albertini, G. (2009). Internalizing correlates of dyslexia. World Journal of Pediatrics, 5, 255–264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, I. R., & Carroll, J. M. (2012). Speech prosofy and developmental dyslexia: Reduced phonological awareness in the context of intact phonological representations. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 560–581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nandakumar, K., & Leat, S. J. (2008). Dyslexia: A review of two theories. Clinical and Experimental Optometry, 91, 333–340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naples, A. J., Chang, J. T., Katz, L., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2009). Same or different? Insights into the etiology of phonological awareness and rapid naming. Biological Psychology, 80, 226–239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naples, A. J., Katz, L., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2012). Reading and a diffusion model analysis of reaction time. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37(4), 299–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nash, H. M., Hulme, C., Gooch, D., & Snowling, M. J. (2013). Preschool language profiles of children at family risk of dyslexia: continuities with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(9), 958–968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy.Google Scholar
National Institute of Child Health and Development. (2007). Learning disabilities: What are learning disabilities? Retrieved August 29, 2010 from .
National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.Google Scholar
Naumova, O., Lee, M., Koposov, R., Szyf, M., Dozier, M., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2012). Differential patterns of whole-genome DNA methylation in institutionalized children and children raised by their biological parents. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 143–155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, J. R., Benner, G. J., & Gonzalez, J. (2003). Learner characteristics that influence the treatment of effectiveness of early literacy interventions: A meta-analytic review. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 18, 255–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nevo, E., & Breznitz, Z. (2011). Assessment of working memory components at 6 years of age as predictors of reading achievements a year later. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 73–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newbury, D. F., Paracchini, S., Scerri, T. S., Winchester, L., Addis, L.,Richardson, A. J., Walter, J., et al. (2011). Investigation of dyslexia and SLI risk-variants in reading- and language-impaired subjects. Behavior Genetics, 41, 90–104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolson, R. (2005). Dyslexia: Beyond the myth. The Psychologist, 18, 658–659.Google Scholar
Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1990). Automaticity: A new framework for dyslexia research?Cognition, 35, 159–182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2004). Dyslexia Early Screening Test (DEST). Oxford: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2006). Do cerebellar deficits underlie phonological problems in dyslexia?Developmental Science, 9, 259–262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2007). Procedural learning difficulties: Reuniting the developmental disorders?Trends in Neurosciences, 30, 135–141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2008). Dyslexia, learning, and the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (2001a). Developmental dyslexia: The cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences, 24, 508–511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (2001b). Dyslexia, development and the cerebellum. Trends in Neurosciences, 24, 515–516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolson, R. I., & Reynolds, D. (2007). Sound design and balanced analyses: Response to Rack and colleagues. Dyslexia, 13, 105–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niogi, S. N., & McCandliss, B. D. (2006). Left lateralized white matter microstructure accounts for individual differences in reading ability and disability. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2178–2188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nittrouer, S. (1999). Do temporal processing deficits cause phonological processing problems?Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 925–942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noble, K. G., Farah, M. J., & McCandliss, B. D. (2006). Socioeconomic background modulates cognition-achievement relationships in reading. Cognitive Development, 21, 349–368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nopola-Hemmi, J., Myllyluoma, B., Voutilainen, A., Leinonen, S., Kere, J., & Ahonen, T. (2002). Familial dyslexia: Neurocognitive and genetic correlation in a large Finnish family. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 44, 580–586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norton, E. S., & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 427–452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norwich, B. (2010). Book review of Developmental disorders of language, learning and cognition by Hulme, C. and Snowling, M. J.. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 10, 133–135.CrossRef
O’Connor, R. E., Bocian, K. M., Sanchez, V., & Beach, K. D. (in press). Access to a responsiveness to intervention model: Does beginning intervention in kindergarten matter? Journal of Learning Disabilities.
O’Connor, R. E., Fulmer, D., Harty, K. R., & Bell, K. M. (2005). Layers of reading intervention in kindergarten through third grade: Changes in teaching and student outcomes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 440–455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, R. E., White, A., & Swanson, H. L. (2007). Repeated reading versus continuous reading: Influences on reading fluency and comprehension. Exceptional Children, 74, 31–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Donnell, P. S., & Miller, D. N. (2011). Identifying students with specific learning disabilities: School psychologists’ acceptability of the discrepancy model versus response to intervention. Journal of Disability Policy Studies, 22, 83–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Office of Inspector General of the U.S. Department of Education. (2007). The department's administration of selected aspects of the Reading First program: Final audit report (ED-OIG/A03G0006). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.Google Scholar
Oganian, Y., & Ahissar, M. (2012). Poor anchoring limits dyslexics’ perceptual, memory, and reading skills. Neuropsychologia, 50, 1895–1905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, R. K. (1985). Disabled reading processes and cognitive profiles. In Gray, D. B. & Kavanagh, J. F. (Eds.), Biobehavioral measures of dyslexia (pp. 215–244). Parkton, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Olson, R. K. (2011). Genetic and environmental influences on phonological abilities and reading achievement. In Brady, S. A., Braze, D., & Fowler, C. A. (Eds.), Explaining individual differences in reading: Theory and evidence (pp. 197–216). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Olson, R. K., & Datta, H. (2002). Visual-temporal processing in reading-disabled and normal twins. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15(15), 127–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olulade, O. A., Napoliello, E. M., & Eden, G. F. (2013). Abnormal visual motion processing is not a cause of dyslexia. Neuron, 79, 180–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Roak, B. J., Deriziotis, P., Lee, C., Vives, L., Schwartz, J. J., Girirajan, S., et al. (2011). Exome sequencing in sporadic autism spectrum disorders identifies severe de novo mutations. Nature Genetics, 43, 585–589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orton, S. T. (1937). Reading, writing, and speech problems in children. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Orton, S. T. (1939). A neurological explanation of the reading disability. Education Record, 12, 58–68.Google Scholar
Pammer, K. (2012). The role of the dorsal pathway in word recognition. In Stein, J. & Kapoula, Z. (Eds.), Visual aspects of dyslexia (pp. 137–149). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paracchini, S., Steer, C. D., Buckingham, L. L., Morris, A. P., Ring, S., Scerri, T., et al. (2008). Association of the KIAA0319 dyslexia susceptibility gene with reading skills in the general population. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1576–1584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, R. L. (2003). The seven warning signs of bogus science. Chronicle of Higher Education, 49, 20–21.Google Scholar
Parker, R. M. (1990). Power, control, and validity in research. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 613–620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pashler, H., McDaniel, M., Rohrer, D., & Bjork, R. (2008). Learning styles: Concepts and evidence. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 9, 106–119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, I., Bott, C., Heim, S., Eulitz, C., & Elbert, T. (2006). Reduced hemispheric asymmetry of the auditory N260m in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 44, 785–794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulesu, E., Demonet, J.-F., Fazio, F., McCrory, E., Chanoine, V., Brunswick, N., et al. (2001). Dyslexia: Cultural diversity and biological unity. Science, 291, 2165–2167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulesu, E., Frith, U., Snowling, M., Gallagher, A., Morton, J., Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (1996). Is developmental dyslexia a disconnection syndrome? Evidence from PET scanning. Brain, 119, 143–157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, P. D. (2004). The reading wars. Education Policy, 18, 216–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellicano, E., & Gibson, L.Y. (2008). Investigating the functional integrity of the dorsal visual pathway in autism and dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 46(10), 2593–2596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennington, B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 385–413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennington, B. F. (2009). Diagnosing learning disorders: A neuropsychological framework (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Pennington, B. F., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2009). Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 283–306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennington, B. F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Green, P. A., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Comparing the phonological and double deficit hypotheses for developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 14, 707–755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennington, B. F., & Olson, R. K. (2005). Genetics of dyslexia. In Snowling, M. & Hulme, C. (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 453–472). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennington, B. F., Santerre-Lemmon, L., Rosenberg, J., MacDonald, B., Boada, R., et al. (2012). Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(1), 212–224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennington, B. F., van Orden, G. C., Smith, S. D., Green, P. A., & Haith, M. M. (1990). Phonological processing skills and deficits in adult dyslexics. Child Development, 61, 1753–1778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penolazzi, B., Spironelli, C., & Angrilli, A. (2008). Delta EEG activity as a marker of dysfunctional linguistic processing in developmental dyslexia. Psychophysiology, 45, 1025–1033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penolazzi, B., Spironelli, C., Vio, C., & Angrilli, A. (2006). Altered hemispheric asymmetry during word processing in dyslexic children: An event-related potential study. Neuroreport, 17, 429–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penolazzi, B., Spironelli, C., Vio, C., & Angrilli, A. (2010). Brain plasticity in developmental dyslexia after phonological treatment: A beta EEG band study. Behavioural Brain Research, 209, 179–182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perea, M., Panadero, V., Moret-Tatay, C., & Gómez, P. (2012). The effects of inter-letter spacing in visual-word recognition: Evidence with young normal readers and developmental dyslexics. Learning and Instruction, 22, 420–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perceptual Development Corporation (1998). Irlen Institute Website: Who we help. Retrieved July 14, 2012 from .
Perfetti, C. A. (1991). The psychology, pedagogy and politics of reading. Psychological Science, 2, 70–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model: Some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 112, 43–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pernet, C. R., Andersson, J., Paulesu, E., & Demonet, J. F. (2009). When all hypotheses are right: A multifocal account of dyslexia. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 2278–2292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perrachione, T. K., Del Tufo, S. N., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2011). Human voice recognition depends on language ability. Science, 333(6042), 595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, R. L., & Pennington, B. F. (2012). Developmental dyslexia. The Lancet, 379, 1997–2007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, R. L., Pennington, B. F., Shriberg, L. D., & Boada, R. (2009). What influences literacy outcome in children with speech sound disorder?Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 1175–1188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrill, S. A., Deater-Deckard, K., Thompson, L. A., Schatschneider, C., Dethorne, L. S., & Vandenbergh, D. J. (2007). Longitudinal genetic analysis of early reading: The Western Reserve Reading Project. Reading and Writing, 20, 127–146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrill, S. A., Hart, S. A., Harlaar, N., Logan, J., Justice, L. M., Schatschneider, C., et al. (2010). Genetic and environmental influences on the growth of early reading skills. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 51(6), 660–667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peyrin, C., Démonet, J. F., N’guyen-Morel, M. A., Le Bas, J. F., & Valdois, S. (2011). Superior parietal lobe dysfunction in a homogeneous group of dyslexic children with a visual attention span disorder. Brain & Language, 118, 128–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peyrin, C., Lallier, M., Démonet, J. F., Pernet, C., Baciu, M., Le Bas, J. F., et al. (2012). Neural dissociation of phonological and visual attention span disorders in developmental dyslexia: FMRI evidence from two case reports. Brain & Language, 120, 381–394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfost, M., Dörfler, T., & Artelt, C. (2012). Reading competence development of poor readers in a German elementary school sample: An empirical examination of the Matthew effect model. Journal of Research in Reading, 35(4), 411–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piasta, S. B., & Wagner, R. K. (2010). Learning letter names and sounds: Effects of instruction, letter type, and phonological processing skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 324–344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pierrehumbert, J. (2003). Phonetic diversity, statistical learning and acquisition of phonology. Language & Speech, 46, 115–154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pine, D. (2011). Commentary: Diagnosis and classification: There must be something left about which to argue – reflections on Rutter (2011). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(6), 663–664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1998). Foreword. In McGuinness, D. (Ed.), Why children can't read: And what can we do about it. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Plakas, A., van Zuijen, T., van Leevwen, T., Thomson, J. M., & van der Leij, A. (2003). Impaired non-speech auditory processing at a pre-reading age is a risk factor for dyslexia but not a predictor: An ERP study. Cortex, 49(4), 1034–1045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plante, E. (2012). Windows into receptive processing. In Benasich, A. A. & Fitch, R. H. (Eds.), Developmental dyslexia: Early precursors, neurobehavioral markers, and biological substrates (pp. 257–274). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., & Kovas, Y. (2005). Generalist genes and learning disabilities. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 592–617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pokorni, J. L., Worthington, C. K., & Jamison, P. J. (2004). Phonological awareness intervention: Comparison of Fast ForWord, Earobics, and LiPS. Journal of Educational Research, 97, 147–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulsen, M., Juul, H., & Elbro, C. (in press). Multiple mediation analysis of the relationship between rapid naming and reading. Journal of Research in Reading.
Powell, D., Stuart, M., Garwood, H., Quinlan, P., & Stainthorp, R. (2007). An experimental comparison between rival theories of Rapid Automatised Naming (RAN) performance and its relationship to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98, 46–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prado, C., Dubois, M., & Valdois, S. (2007). Eye movements in reading aloud and visual search in developmental dyslexia: Impact of the VA span. Vision Research, 47, 2521–2530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pressley, M. (2006). Reading instruction that works: The case for balanced teaching. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Pressley, M., & Wharton-McDonald, R. (1997). Skilled comprehension and its development through instruction. School Psychology Review, 26, 448–466.Google Scholar
Price, C. J., & Mechelli, A. (2005). Reading and reading disturbance. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 231–238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Priebe, S. J., Keenan, J. M., & Miller, A. C. (2012). How prior knowledge affects word identification and comprehension. Reading and Writing, 25, 131–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prifitera, A., & Dersch, J. (1993). Base rates of WISC-III diagnostic subtest patterns among normal, learning disabled and ADHD samples. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, WISC-III Monograph, 43–55.
Pringle-Morgan, W. (1896). A case of congenital word blindness. British Medical Journal, 2, 178.Google Scholar
Pugh, K. R., Landi, N., Preston, J. L., Mencl, W. E., Austin, A. C., Sibley, D., et al. (2013). The relationship between phonological and auditory processing and brain organization in beginning readers. Brain & Language, 125(2), 173–183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pugh, K. R., & McCardle, P. (Eds.). (2009). How children learn to read: Current issues and new directions in the integration of cognition, neurobiology and genetics of reading and dyslexia research and practice. New York: Psychology Press.
Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Jenner, A. R., Katz, L., Frost, S. J., Lee, J. R., et al. (2000). Functional neuroimaging studies of reading and reading disability (developmental dyslexia). Mental Retardation & Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 6, 207–213.3.0.CO;2-P>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puolakanaho, A., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Leppanen, P. H. T., Poikkeus, A.-M., Tolvanen, A., Torppa, M., & Lyytinen, H. (2007). Very early phonological and language skills: Estimating individual risk of reading disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(9), 923–931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quercia, P., Demougeot, L., Dos Santos, M.Bonnetblanc, F. (2011). Integration of proprioceptive signals and attentional capacity during postural control are impaired but subject to improvement in dyslexic children. Experimental Brain Research, 209(4), 599–608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quercia, P., Feiss, L., & Michel, C. (2013). Developmental dyslexia and vision. Clinical Opthalmology, 7, 869–881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quinn, J. M., & Wagner, R. K. (in press). Gender differences in reading impairment and in the identification of impaired readers: results from a large-scale study of at-risk readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities.
Rack, J. P. (2003). The who, what, why and how of intervention programmes: Comments on the DDAT evaluation. Dyslexia, 9, 137–139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., & Gibbs, S. (2007). No evidence that an exercise-based treatment programme (DDAT) has specific benefits for children with reading difficulties. Dyslexia, 13, 97–104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia – a review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 28–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rae, C., Lee, M. A., Dixon, R. M., Blamire, A. M., Thompson, C. H., Styles, P., et al. (1998). Metabolic abnormalities in developmental dyslexia detected by 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Lancet, 351, 1849–1852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raij, T., Uutela, K., & Hari, R. (2000). Audiovisual integration of letters in the human brain. Neuron, 28, 617–625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsden, S., Richardson, F. M., Josse, M. S. C., Ellis, C., Shakeshaft, C., Seghier, M. L., et al. (2011). Verbal and non-verbal intelligence changes in the teenage brain. Nature, 479, 113–116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficits or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurology, 13, 212–218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F. (2004). Neurobiology of dyslexia: A reinterpretation of the data. Trends in Neurosciences, 27, 720–726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F., & Ahissar, M. (2012). Developmental dyslexia: The difficulties of interpreting poor performance, and the importance of normal performance. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29(1–2), 104–122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F., Marshall, C. R., Rosen, S., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2013). Phonological deficits in specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: Towards a multidimensional model. Brain, 136, 630–645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F., Pidgeon, E., & Frith, U. (2003). The relationship between motor control and phonology in dyslexic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44, 712–722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., et al. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841–865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F., & Szenkovits, G. (2008). What phonological deficit?The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 129–141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raschle, N. M., Stering, P. L., Meissner, S. N., & Gaab, N. (in press). Altered neuronal response during rapid auditory processing and its relation to phonological processing in prereading children at familial risk for dyslexia. Cerebral Cortex.
Rashotte, C. A., MacPhee, K., & Torgesen, J. K. (2001). The effectiveness of a group reading instruction program with poor readers in multiple grades. Learning Disability Quarterly, 24, 119–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rasinski, T., Rikli, A., & Johnston, S. (2009). Reading fluency: More than automaticity? More than a concern for the primary grades?Literacy Research and Instruction, 48, 350–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raskind, W. H., Igo, R. P. J., Chapman, N. H., Berninger, V. W., Thomson, J. B., Matsushita, M., et al. (2005). A genome scan in multigenerational families with dyslexia: Identification of a novel locus on chromosome 2q that contributes to phonological decoding efficiency. Molecular Psychiatry, 10, 699–711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reason, R. (2001). Letter to the Editor. Dyslexia, 7, 174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reason, R., & Stothard, J. (2013). Is there a place for dyslexia in educational psychology practice?Debate, 146, 8–13.Google Scholar
Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(2), 359–379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Regan, T., & Woods, K. (2000). Teachers’ understandings of dyslexia: Implications for educational psychology practice. Educational Psychology in Practice, 16, 333–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reid, D. K., & Valle, J. W. (2004). The discursive practice of learning disability: Implications for instruction and parent-school relations. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 466–481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Renvall, A., & Hari, R. (2002). Auditory cortical responses to speech-like stimuli in dyslexic adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 757–768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reschly, D. (2005). Learning disabilities identification: Primary intervention, secondary intervention, and then what?Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 510–515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reschly, D. J., & Tilley, W. D. (1999). Reform trends and system design alternatives. In Reschly, D. J., Tilley, W. D., & Grimes, J. P. (Eds.), Special education in transition: Functional assessment and noncategorical programming (pp. 19–48). Longmont, CO: Sopris West.Google Scholar
Reynolds, C. R., & Shaywitz, S. E. (2009a). Response to intervention: Prevention and remediation, perhaps. Diagnosis, no. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 44–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, C. R., & Shaywitz, S. E. (2009b). Response to intervention: Ready or not? Or, from wait-to-fail to watch-them-fail. School Psychology Quarterly, 24, 130–145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, D., & Nicolson, R. I. (2007). Follow-up of an exercise-based treatment for children with reading difficulties. Dyslexia, 13, 78–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, D., Nicolson, R. I., & Hambly, H. (2003). Evaluation of an exercise-based treatment for children with reading difficulties. Dyslexia, 9, 48–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rezaie, R., Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M., Cirino, P. T., Vaughn, S., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2011). Temporo-parietal brain activity as a longitudinal predictor of response to educational interventions among middle school struggling readers. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 875–885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riccio, C. A., Sullivan, J. R., & Cohen, M. J. (2010). Neuropsychological assessment and intervention for childhood and adolescent disorders. Trenton, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, M. (1999). Literacy and behaviour: The prison reading survey. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rice, M., & Brooks, G. (2004). Developmental dyslexia in adults: A research review. London: National Research and Development Centre for Adult Literacy and Numeracy.Google Scholar
Richards, T. L., & Berninger, V. W. (2008). Abnormal fMRI connectivity in children with dyslexia during a phoneme task: before but not after treatment. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21, 294–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richards, T. L., Corina, D. P., Serafini, S., Steury, K., Echelard, D. R., Dager, S. R., et al. (2000). Effects of a phonologically driven treatment for dyslexia on lactate levels measured by proton MR spectroscopic imaging. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 21, 916–922.Google ScholarPubMed
Richards, T. L., Dager, S. R., Corina, D., Serafini, S., Heide, A. C., Steury, K., et al. (1999). Dyslexic children have abnormal brain lactate response to reading-related language tasks. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 20, 1393–1398.Google ScholarPubMed
Richards, T. L., Stevenson, J., Crouch, J., Johnson, L. C., Maravilla, K., Stock, P., et al. (2008). Tract-based spatial statistics of diffusion tensor imaging in adults with dyslexia. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 29, 1134–1139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, A. J. (2006). Omega-3 fatty acids in ADHD and related neurodevelopmental disorders. International Review of Psychiatry, 18, 155–172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, A. J., Burton, J. R., Sewell, R. P., Spreckelsen, T. F., & Montgomery, P. (2012). Docosahexaenoic acid for reading, cognition and behavior in children aged 7–9 Years: A randomized, controlled trial (The DOLAB Study). PLoS ONE, 7(9), e43909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, A. J., Cox, I. J., Sargentoni, J., & Puri, B. K. (1997). Abnormal cerebral phospholipid metabolism in dyslexia indicated by phosphorus-31 magnetic resonance spectroscopy. NMR in Biomedicine, 10, 309–314.3.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, A. J., & Montgomery, P. (2005). The Oxford-Durham study: A randomized, controlled trial of dietary supplementation with fatty acids in children with developmental coordination disorder. Pediatrics, 115, 1360–1366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, U., Leppanen, P. H. T., Leiwo, M., & Lyytinen, H. (2003). Speech perception of infants with high familial risk for dyslexia differ at the age of 6 months. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 385–397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richlan, F., Kronbichler, M., & Wimmer, H. (2009). Functional abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: A quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 3299–3308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richlan, F., Kronbichler, M., & Wimmer, H. (2011). Meta-analyzing brain dysfunctions in dyslexic children and adults. Neuroimage, 56, 1735–1742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ricketts, J. (2011). Research review: Reading comprehension in developmental disorders of language and communication. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(11), 1111–1123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riddick, B. (2000). An examination of the relationship between labeling and stigmatization with special reference to dyslexia. Disability and Society, 15, 653–657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riddick, B. (2001). Dyslexia and inclusion: Time for a social model of disability?International Studies in Sociology of Education, 11, 223–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riddick, B. (2010). Living with dyslexia: The social and emotional consequences of specific learning difficulties/disabilities. Oxford: Routledge.Google Scholar
Riddick, B., Wolfe, J., & Lumsdon, D. (2002). Dyslexia: A practical guide for teachers and parents. London: David Fulton.Google Scholar
Rimkute, L., Torppa, M., Eklund, K., Nurmi, J. E., & Lyytinen, H. (2013). The impact of adolescents’ dyslexia on parents’ and their own educational expectations. Reading and Writing.
Ritchie, S. J., Della Sala, S., & McIntosh, R. D. (2011). Irlen colored overlays do not alleviate reading difficulties. Pediatrics, 128, e932–e938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ritchie, S. J., Della Sala, S., & McIntosh, R. D. (2012). Irlen colored overlays in the classroom: A one-year follow-up. Mind, Brain, and Education, 6, 74–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roach, N., Edwards, V., & Hogben, J. (2004). The tale is in the tail: An alternative hypothesis for psychophysical performance variability in dyslexia. Perception, 33(7), 817–830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, A. E., Cox, G. F., Kimonis, V., Lamb, A., & Irons, M. (2004). Clinical presentation of 13 patients with subtelomeric rearrangements and a review of the literature. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 128A, 352–363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, G., Torgesen, J. K., Boardman, A., & Scammacca, N. (2008). Evidence-based strategies for reading instruction of older students with learning disabilitiesLearning Disabilities Research and Practice, 23, 63–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robertson, C., & Salter, W. (1997). The Phonological Awareness Test. East Moline, IL: LinguiSystems.Google Scholar
Rochelle, K. S., & Talcott, J. B. (2006). Impaired balance in developmental dyslexia? A meta-analysis of the contending evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 1159–1166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roeske, D., Ludwig, K. U., Neuhoff, N., Becker, J., Bartling, J., Bruder, J., et al. (2011). First genome-wide association scan on neurophysiological endophenotypes points to trans-regulation effects on SLC2A3 in dyslexic children. Molecular Psychiatry, 16, 97–107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romani, C., Tsouknida, E., di Betta, A. M., & Olson, A. (2011). Reduced attentional capacity, but normal processing speed and shifting of attention in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from a serial task. Cortex, 47, 715–733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rooney, K. J. (1995). Dyslexia revisited: History, educational philosophy, and clinical assessment applications. Intervention in School and Clinic, 31, 6–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, J. (2009). Identifying and teaching children and young people with dyslexia and literacy difficulties. (The Rose Report). Nottingham, UK: DCSF Publications.Google Scholar
Rose, L. T., & Rouhani, P. (2012). Influence of verbal working memory depends on vocabulary: oral reading fluency in adolescents with dyslexia. Mind, Brain, and Education, 6, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosen, V., & Engle, R.W. (1997). Forward and backward serial recall. Intelligence, 25, 37–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenberg, J., Pennington, B. F., Willcutt, E. G., & Olson, R. K. (2012). Gene by environment interactions influencing reading disability and the inattentive symptom dimension of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(3), 243–251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubenstein, K., Matsushita, M., Berninger, V. W., Raskind, W. H., & Wijsman, E. M. (2011). Genome scan for spelling deficits: Effects of verbal IQ on models of transmission and trait gene localization. Behavior Genetics, 41, 31–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubie-Davies, C. M., Blatchford, P., Webster, R., Koutsoubou, M., & Bassett, P. (2010). Enhancing learning? A comparison of teacher and teaching assistant interactions with pupils. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 21, 429–449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruffino, M., Trussardi, A. N., Gori, S., Finzi, A., Giovagnoli, S., Menghini, D., et al. (2010). Attentional engagement deficits in dyslexic children. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3793–3801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumsey, J. M., Andreason, P., Zametkin, A. J., Aquino, T., King, A. C., Hamburger, S. D., et al. (1992). Failure to activate the left temporoparietal cortex in dyslexia. An oxygen 15 positron emission tomographic study. Archives of Neurology, 49, 527–534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumsey, J. M., Casanova, M., Mannheim, G. B., Patronas, N., De Vaughn, N., Hamburger, S. D., et al. (1996). Corpus callosum morphology, as measured with MRI, in dyslexic men. Biological Psychiatry, 39, 769–775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumsey, J. M., Donohue, B. C., Brady, D. R., Nace, K., Giedd, J. N., & Andreason, P. (1997). A magnetic resonance imaging study of planum temporale asymmetry in men with developmental dyslexia. Archives of Neurology, 54, 1481–1489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumsey, J. M., Nace, K., Donohue, B., Wise, D., Maisog, J. M., & Andreason, P. (1997). A positron emission tomographic study of impaired word recognition and phonological processing in dyslexic men. Archives of Neurology, 54, 562–573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumsey, J. M., Zametkin, A. J., Andreason, P., Hanahan, A. P., Hamburger, S. D., Aquino, T., et al. (1994). Normal activation of frontotemporal language cortex in dyslexia, as measured with oxygen 15 positron emission tomography. Archives of Neurology, 51, 27–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (1978). Prevalence and types of dyslexia. In Benton, A. & Pearl, D. (Eds.), Dyslexia: An appraisal of current knowledge (pp. 5–28). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (2011). Child psychiatric diagnosis and classification: Concepts, findings, challenges and potential. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(6), 647–660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Caspi, A., Fergusson, D., Horwood, L. J., Goodman, R., Maughan, B., et al. (2004). Sex differences in developmental reading disability: New findings from 4 epidemiological studies. Journal of the American Medical Association, 291, 2007–2012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Kim-Cohen, J., & Maughan, B. (2006). Continuities and discontinuities in psychopathology between childhood and adult life. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 276–295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., & Maughan, B. (2005). Dyslexia: 1965–2005. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 33, 389–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryder, J. F., Tunmer, W. E., & Greaney, K. T. (2008). Explicit instruction in phonemic awareness and phonemically based decoding skills as an intervention strategy for struggling readers in whole language classrooms. Reading and Writing, 21, 299–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saine, N. L., Lerkkanen, M. K., Ahonen, T., Tolvanen, A., & Lyytinen, H. (2011). Computer assisted remedial reading intervention for school beginners at risk for reading disability. Child Development, 82(3), 1013–1028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salmelin, R., Service, E., Kiesilä, P., Uutela, K., & Salonen, O. (1996). Impaired visual word processing in dyslexia revealed with magnetoencephalography. Annals of Neurology, 40, 157–162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samuelsson, S., Byrne, B., Olson, R. K., Hulslander, Wadsworth, S., Corley, R., et al. (2008). Response to early literacy instruction in the United States, Australia, and Scandinavia: A behavioral-genetic analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 289–295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samuelsson, S., Herkner, B., & Lundberg, I. (2003). Reading and writing difficulties among prison inmates: A matter of experiential factors rather than dyslexic problems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 53–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandak, R., Mencl, W. E., Frost, S. J., & Pugh, K. R. (2004). The neurobiological basis of skilled and impaired reading: Recent findings and new directions. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 273–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanders, S. J., Ercan-Sencicek, A. G., Hus, V., Luo, R., Murtha, M. T., Moreno-De-Luca, D., et al. (2011). Multiple recurrent de novo CNVs, including duplications of the 7q11.23 Williams Syndrome region, are strongly associated with autism. Neuron, 70, 863–885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanders, S. J., Murtha, M. T., Gupta, A. R., Murdoch, J. D., Raubeson, M. J., Willsey, A. J., et al. (2012). De novo mutations revealed by whole-exome sequencing are strongly associated with autism. Nature, 485, 237–241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandu, A. L., Specht, K., Beneventi, H., Lundervold, A., & Hugdahl, K. (2008). Sex-differences in grey-white matter structure in normal-reading and dyslexic adolescents. Neuroscience Letters, 438, 80–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santi, K. L., York, M., Foorman, B., & Francis, D. J. (2009). The timing of early reading assessment in kindergarten. Learning Disability Quarterly, 32, 217–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage, R. S. (2004). Motor skills, automaticity and developmental dyslexia: A review of the research literature. Reading and Writing, 17, 301–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage, R. S., & Frederickson, N. (2005). Evidence of a highly specific relationship between rapid automatic naming of digits and text-reading speed. Brain and Language, 93, 152–159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, R. S., Frederickson, N., Goodwin, R., Patni, U., Smith, N., & Tuersley, L. (2005). Relationships among rapid digit naming, phonological processing, motor automaticity, and speech perception in poor, average, and good readers and spellers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 12–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, R. S., Lavers, N., & Pillay, V. (2007). Working memory and reading difficulties: What we know and what we don't know about the relationship. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 185–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saygin, Z. M., Norton, E. S., Osher, D. E., Beach, S. D., Cyr, A. B., et al. (2013). Tracking the roots of reading ability: White matter volume and integrity correlate with phonological awareness in prereading and early-reading kindergarten children. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(33), 13251–13258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scammacca, N., Roberts, G., Vaughn, S., Edmonds, M., Wexler, J., Reutebuch, C. K., et al. (2007). Reading interventions for adolescent struggling readers: A meta-analysis with implications for practice. Portsmouth, NH: RMC Research Corporation Center on Instruction.Google Scholar
Scammacca, N., Roberts, G.Vaughn, S., & Stuebing, K. K. A. (in press). A meta-analysis of interventions for struggling readers in Grades 4–12: 1980–2011. Journal of Learning Disabilities
Scanlon, D. (2013). Specific learning disability and its newest definition: Which is comprehensive and which is insufficient?Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(1), 26–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scanlon, D. M. (2011). Response to intervention as an assessment approach. In McGill-Franzen, A. & Allington, R. L. (Eds.), Handbook of reading disability research (pp. 139–148). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Scanlon, D. M., Gelzheiser, L. M., Vellutino, F. R., Schatschneider, C., & Sweeney, J. M. (2008). Reducing the incidence of early reading difficulties: Professional development for classroom teachers versus direct interventions for children. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 346–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scanlon, D. M., Vellutino, F. R., Small, S. G., Fanuele, D., & Sweeney, J. M. (2005). Severe reading difficulties: Can they be prevented? A comparison of prevention and intervention approaches. Exceptionality, 13, 209–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (1998a). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In Shapiro, B. K., Accardo, P. J., & Capute, A. J. (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75–119). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (1998b). Predicting the future achievement of second graders with reading disabilities: Contributions of phonemic awareness, verbal memory, rapid naming, and IQ. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 115–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarborough, H. S., & Brady, S. A. (2002). Toward a common terminology for talking about speech and reading: A glossary of the “phon” words and some related terms. Journal of Literacy Research, 34, 299–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scerri, T. S., Paracchini, S., Morris, A., MacPhie, I. L., Talcott, J., Stein, J., et al. (2010). Identification of candidate genes for dyslexia susceptibility on chromosome 18. PLoS ONE, 5, e13712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 265–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schatschneider, C., Wagner, R. K., & Crawford, E. (2008). The importance of measuring growth in response to intervention models: Testing a core assumption. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 310–315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlaggar, B. L., & McCandliss, B. D. (2007). Development of neural systems for reading. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 475–503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmahmann, J. D., & Pandya, D. N. (1997). The cerebrocerebellar system. International Review of Neurobiology, 41, 31–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneps, M. H., Thomson, J. M., Sonnert, G., Pomplun, M., Chen, C., & Heffner-Wong, A. (2013). Shorter lines facilitate reading in those who struggle. PLoS One, 8(8), e 71161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuchardt, K., Maehler, C., & Hasselhorn, M. (2008). Working memory deficits in children with specific learning disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 514–523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulte-Körne, G., & Bruder, J. (2010). Clinical neurophysiology of visual and auditory processing in dyslexia: A review. Clinical Neurophysiology, 121, 1794–1809.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W., Bartling, J., & Remschmidt, H. (1998). Auditory processing and dyslexia: Evidence for a specific speech processing deficit. Neuroreport, 9, 337–340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, E., Maurer, U., van der Mark, S., Bucher, K., Brem, S., Martin, E., et al. (2008). Impaired semantic processing during sentence reading in children with dyslexia: Combined fMRI and ERP evidence. Neuroimage, 41, 153–168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schumacher, J., Hoffmann, P., Schmal, C., Schulte-Korne, G., & Nothen, M. M. (2007). Genetics of dyslexia: The evolving landscape. Journal of Medical Genetics, 44, 289–297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sela, I. (2012). The relationships between motor learning, the visual system and dyslexia. Reading, Writing, Mathematics and the Developing Brain: Listening to Many Voices, Literacy Studies, 6, 177–189.Google Scholar
Selemon, L. D., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1999). The reduced neuropil hypothesis: A circuit based model of schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 17–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senco Forum. (2005). Points of view from the SENCo Forum: Is it dyslexia?British Journal of Special Education, 32, 165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sexton, C. C., Gelhorn, H. L., Bell, J. A., & Classi, P. M. (2012). The co-occurrence of reading disorder and ADHD: Epidemiology, treatment, psychosocial impact, and economic burden. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(6), 538–564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T., & Warrington, E. K. (1977). The possible role of selective attention in acquired dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 15, 31–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shalom, D. B., & Poeppel, D. (2008). Functional anatomic models of language: Assembling the pieces. Neuroscientist, 14, 119–127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shanahan, M. A., Pennington, B. F., Yerys, B. E., Scott, A., Boada, R., Willcutt, E. G., et al. (2006). Processing speed deficits in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and reading disability. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 585–602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shankweiler, D., & Crain, S. (1986). Language mechanisms and reading disorders: A modular approach. Cognition, 24, 139–168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shankweiler, D., & Fowler, A. E. (2004). Questions people ask about the role of phonological processes in learning to read. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 483–515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapleske, J., Rossell, S. L., & Woodruff, P. W. (1999). The planum temporale: A systematic, quantitative review of its structural, functional and clinical significance. Brain Research Review, 29, 26–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 95–129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Share, D. L. (2004). Orthographic learning at a glance: On the time course and developmental onset of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 267–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Share, D. L. (2008). On the anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., Maclean, R., & Matthews, R. (2002). Temporal processing and reading disability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 151–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Share, D. L., & Silva, P. A. (2003). Gender bias in IQ-discrepancy and post-discrepancy definitions of reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 4–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Share, D. L., & Stanovich, K. E. (1995). Cognitive processes in early reading development: Accommodating individual differences into a model of acquisition. Issues in Education, 1, 1–57.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, S., Morris, R., & Shaywitz, B. (2008). The education of dyslexic children from childhood to young adulthood. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 451–475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Blachman, B. A., & Pugh, K. R., et al. (2004a). Development of left occipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically-based intervention. Biological Psychiatry, 55(9), 926–933.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Blachman, B. A., Pugh, K. R., Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P., et al. (2004b). Development of left occipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically-based intervention. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 926–933.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P., et al. (2002). Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 101–110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaywitz, B. A., Skudlarski, P., Holahan, J. M., Marchione, K. E., Constable, R. T., Fulbright, R. K., et al. (2007). Age-related changes in reading systems of dyslexic children. Annals of Neurology, 61, 363–370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaywitz, S. E. (1996). Dyslexia. Scientific American, 275(5), 98–104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaywitz, S. E. (2005). Overcoming dyslexia. New York: Alfred Knopf.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, S. E., Morris, R., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2008). The education of dyslexic children from childhood to young adulthood. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 451–475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2005). Dyslexia (specific reading disability). Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1301–1309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2008). Paying attention to reading: The neurobiology of reading and dyslexia. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 1329–1349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2013). Making a hidden disability visible: What has been learned from neurobiological studies of dyslexia. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 643–657). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Pugh, K. R., Fulbright, R. K., Constable, R. T., Mencl, W. E., et al. (1998). Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95, 2636–2641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shen, H., Liu, Y., Liu, P., Recker, R. R., & Deng, H. W. (2005). Nonreplication in genetic studies of complex diseases – lessons learned from studies of osteoporosis and tentative remedies. Journal of Bone & Mineral Research, 20, 365–376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shipstead, Z., Hicks, K. L., & Engle, R. W. (2012). CogMed working memory training: Does the evidence support the claims?Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 85–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138, 628–654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiran, A., & Breznitz, Z. (2011). The effect of cognitive training on recall range and speed of information processing in the working memory of dyslexic and skilled readers. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24, 524–537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shovman, M. M., & Ahissar, M. (2006). Isolating the impact of visual perception on dyslexics’ reading ability. Vision Research, 46, 3514–3525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, L. S., & Lipka, O. (2008). The definition of learning disabilities: Who is the individual with learning disabilities? In Reid, G., Fawcett, A., Manis, F., & Siegel, L. (Eds.), The Sage handbook of dyslexia (pp. 290–307). London: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, L. S., & Mazabel, S. (2013). Basic cognitive processes and reading disabilities. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 186–213). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1989). The development of working memory in normally achieving and subtypes of learning disabled children. Child Development, 60, 973–980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silani, G., Frith, U., Demonet, J. F., Fazio, F., Perani, D., Price, C., et al. (2005). Brain abnormalities underlying altered activation in dyslexia: A voxel based morphometry study. Brain, 128, 2453–2461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silver, L. B. (1987). A review of the current controversial approaches for treating learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20, 498–504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simmers, A. J., Bex, P. J., Smith, F. K. H., & Wilkins, A. J. (2001). Spatiotemporal visual function in tinted lens wearers. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 42, 879–884.Google ScholarPubMed
Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Foorman, B. R., Bergman, E., Fishbeck, K., et al. (2000). Brain activation profiles in dyslexic children during non-word reading: a magnetic source imaging study. Neuroscience Letters, 290, 61–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Wheless, J. W., Maggio, W. W., Fletcher, J. M., Castillo, E. M., et al. (2000). Brain mechanisms for reading: The role of the superior temporal gyrus in word and pseudoword naming. Neuroreport, 11, 2443–2447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M., Bergman, E., Breier, J. I., Foorman, B. R., Castillo, E. M., et al. (2002). Dyslexia-specific brain activation profile becomes normal following successful remedial training. Neurology, 58, 1203–1213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simos, P. G., Rezaie, R., Fletcher, J. M., Juranek, J., Passaro, A. D., Li, Z., et al. (2011). Functional disruption of the brain mechanism for reading: Effects of comorbidity and task difficulty among children with developmental learning problems. Neuropsychology, 25, 520–534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, J., & Everatt, J. (2001). Phonological skills and naming speed as predictors of future literacy deficits. Paper presented at the British Dyslexia Association International Conference, Dyslexia: Dawn of the New Century, University of York. Reading.Google Scholar
Simpson, J., & Everatt, J. (2005). Reception class predictors of literacy skills. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 171–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singleton, C. H. (2009a). Visual stress and dyslexia. In Reid, G. (Ed.), The Routledge companion to dyslexia (pp. 43–57). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Singleton, C. H. (2009b). Intervention for dyslexia: A review of published evidence on the impact of specialist dyslexia teaching. London: The Dyslexia-SpLD Trust.Google Scholar
Singleton, C. H. (2012). Visual stress and its relationship to dyslexia. In Stein, J. & Kapoula, Z. (Eds.), Visual aspects of dyslexia (pp. 91–110). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Jin, Z., & Tan, L. H. (2004). Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture. Nature, 431, 71–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skiba, T., Landi, N., Wagner, R., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2011). In search of the perfect phenotype: An analysis of linkage and association studies of reading and reading-related processes. Behavior Genetics, 41, 6–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skottun, B. C. (2000). The magnocellular deficit theory of dyslexia: The evidence from contrast sensitivity. Vision Research, 40, 111–127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skottun, B. C. (2011). On the use of visual motion perception to assess magnocellular integrity. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, 10(1), 15–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skottun, B. C., & Skoyles, J. (2006b). Is coherent motion an appropriate test for magnocelular sensitivity?Brain and Cognition, 61, 172–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skottun, B. C., & Skoyles, J. (2011). Dyslexia, magnocellular integrity and rapidly presented stimuli. Nature Procedings. Retrieved June 10, 2012 from
Skottun, B. C., & Skoyles, J. R. (2006a). Attention, reading and dyslexia. Clinical & Experimental Optometry, 89, 241–245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skottun, B. C., & Skoyles, J. R. (2008). Dyslexia and rapid visual processing: A commentary. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 666–673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skottun, B. C., & Skoyles, J. R. (2010a). L- and M-cone ratios and magnocellular sensitivity in reading. International Journal of Neuroscience, 120, 241–244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skottun, B. C., & Skoyles, J. R. (2010b). Temporal order judgment in dyslexia – Task difficulty or temporal processing deficiency? Neuropsychologia, 48, 2226–2229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skoyles, J. R., & Skottun, B. C. (2004). On the prevalence of magnocellular deficits in the visual system of non-dyslexic individuals. Brain and Language, 88, 79–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavin, R. E., Lake, C., Chambers, B., Cheung, A., & Davis, S. (2009). Effective reading programs for the elementary grades: A best-evidence synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 79, 1391–1466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slavin, R. E., Lake, C., Davis, S., & Madden, N. A. (2011). Effective programs for struggling readers: A best-evidence synthesis. Educational Research Review, 6, 1–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, F. (1971). Understanding reading: A psycholinguistic analysis of reading and learning to read. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Smith, S. D. (2007). Genes, language development, and language disorders. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 13, 95–105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. D., Gilger, J. W., & Pennington, B. F. (2001). Dyslexia and other specific learning disorders. In Rimoin, D. L., Conner, J. M., & Pyeritz, R. E. (Eds.), Emery and Rimoin's principles and practice of medical genetics (pp. 2827–2865). New York: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
Smith, S. D., Kimberling, W. J., Pennington, B. F., & Lubs, H. A. (1983). Specific reading disability: Identification of an inherited form through linkage analyses. Science, 219, 1345–1347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith-Spark, J. H., & Fisk, J. E. (2007). Working memory functioning in developmental dyslexia. Memory, 15(1), 34–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith-Spark, J. H., Fisk, J. E., Fawcett, A. J., & Nicolson, R. I. (2003). Central executive impairments in adult dyslexics: Evidence from visuospatial working memory performance. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 15, 567–587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., & Juel, C. (2005). Teaching children to read: What do we know about how to do it? In Snowling, M. J. & Hulme, C. (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 501–520). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia. Oxford: Blackwell.Google ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J. (2008). Specific disorders and broader phenotypes: The case of dyslexia. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 142–156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J. (2010). Dyslexia. In Cooper, C. L., Field, J., Goswami, U., Jenkins, R., & Sahakian, B. J. (Eds.), Mental capital and mental wellbeing (pp. 775–783). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J. (2011). Beyond phonological deficits: Sources of individual differences in reading disability. In Brady, S. A., Braze, D., & Fowler, C. A. (Eds.), Explaining individual differences in reading: Theory and evidence (pp. 121–136). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J. (2013). Early identification and interventions for dyslexia: A contemporary view. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 13(1), 7–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J., Duff, F., Petrou, A., Schiffeldrin, J., & Bailey, A. M. (2011). Identification of children at risk of dyslexia: The validity of teacher judgements using ‘Phonic Phases’. Journal of Research in Reading, 34, 157–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (1994). The development of phonological skills in children. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 346, 21–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2003). A critique of claims from Reynolds, Nicolson & Hambly (2003) that DDAT is an effective treatment for reading problems: “Lies, damned lies and (inappropriate) statistics.”Dyslexia, 9, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2011). Evidence-based interventions for reading and language difficulties: Creating a virtuous circle. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2012). Annual research review: The nature and classification of reading disorders – commentary on proposals for DSM-5. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 593–607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soler, J. (2009). The historical construction of dyslexia: Implications for higher education. In Soler, J., Fletcher-Campbell, F., & Reid, G. (Eds.), Understanding difficulties in literacy development: Issues and concepts (pp. 39–50). London: Sage.Google Scholar
Soler, J. (2010). Dyslexia lessons: The politics of dyslexia and reading problems. In Hall, K., Goswami, U., Harrison, C., Ellis, S., & Soler, J. (Eds.), Interdisciplinary perspectives on learning to read (pp. 179–192). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Song, M., & Miskel, C. (2002). Interest groups in national reading policy: Perceived influence and beliefs on teaching reading. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan, Center for the Improvement of Early Reading Achievement.Google Scholar
Song, R., Zhang, J., Wang, B., Zhang, H., & Wu, H. (2013). A near-infrared brain function study of Chinese dyslexic children. Neurocase: The Neural Basis of Cognition, 19(4), 382–389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soroli, E., Szenkovits, G., & Ramus, F. (2010). Exploring dyslexics’ phonological deficit III: Foreign speech perception and production. Dyslexia, 16, 318–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spear-Swerling, L. (2004). A road map for understanding reading disability and other reading problems: Origins, intervention and prevention. In Ruddell, R. & Unrau, N. (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (Vol. 5). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
Spear-Swerling, L. (2011). Patterns of reading disabilities across development. In Allington, R. & McGill-Franzen, A. (Eds.), Handbook of research on reading disabilities (pp. 149–161). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Spear-Swerling, L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1994). The road not taken: An integrative theoretical model of reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 91–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Speece, D. L. (2005). Hitting the moving target known as reading development: Some thoughts on screening children for secondary interventions. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 487–493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Speece, D. L., Ritchey, K. D., Silverman, R., Schatsneider, C., Walker, C. Y., & Andrusik, K. N. (2010). Identifying children in middle childhood who are at risk for reading problems. School Psychology Review, 39, 258–276.Google ScholarPubMed
Speece, D. L., & Shekitka, L. (2002). How should reading disabilities be operationalized? A survey of experts. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 17, 118–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, S., & Manis, F. (2010). The effects of a fluency intervention program on fluency and comprehension outcomes of middle-school students with severe reading deficits. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 25, 76–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spinelli, D., De Luca, M., Judica, A., & Zoccolotti, P. (2002). Crowding effects on word identification in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 38, 179–200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spironelli, C., & Angrilli, A. (2006). Language lateralization in phonological, semantic and orthographic tasks: A slow evoked potential study. Behavioural Brain Research, 175, 296–304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spironelli, C., Angrilli, A., & Pertile, M. (2008). Language plasticity in aphasics after recovery: Evidence from slow evoked potentials. Neuroimage, 40, 912–922.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spironelli, C., Penolazzi, B., & Angrilli, A. (2008). Dysfunctional hemispheric asymmetry of theta and beta EEG activity during linguistic tasks in developmental dyslexia. Biological Psychology, 77, 123–131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spironelli, C., Penolazzi, B., Vio, C., & Angrilli, A. (2010). Cortical reorganization in dyslexic children after phonological training: Evidence from early evoked potentials. Brain, 133, 3385–3395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stackhouse, J., & Wells, B. (1997). Children's speech and literacy difficulties: A psycholinguistic framework. London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Stahl, S. A., & Miller, P. D. (1989). Whole language and language experience approaches for beginning reading: A quantitative research synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 59, 87–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stainthorp, R., Stuart, M., Powell, D., Quinlan, P., & Garwood, H. (2010). Visual processing deficits in children with slow RAN performance. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 266–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stankiewicz, P., & Lupski, J. R. (2010). Structural variation in the human genome and its role in disease. Annual Review of Medicine, 61, 437–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1980). Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 590–604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Response to Christensen. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 279–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1994). Annotation: Does dyslexia exist? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 579–595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E. (1996). Toward a more inclusive definition of dyslexia. Dyslexia, 2, 154–166.3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1998). Twenty-five years of research on the reading process: The grand synthesis and what it means for our field. In Shanahan, T. & Rodriguez-Brown, F. (Eds.), Forty-seventh yearbook of the National Reading Conference (pp. 44–58). Chicago: National Reading Conference.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in understanding reading: Scientific foundations and new frontiers. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2005). The future of a mistake: Will discrepancy measurement continue to make the learning disabilities field a pseudoscience? Learning Disability Quarterly, 28, 103–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & Stanovich, P. J. (1997). Further thoughts on aptitude/achievement discrepancy. Educational Psychology in Practice, 13, 3–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, J. (2008). The neurobiological basis of dyslexia. In Reid, H., Fawcett, A., Manis, F., & Siegel, L. (Eds.), The Sage handbook of dyslexia (pp. 53–76). London: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, J. (2012a). The magnocellular theory of dyslexia. In Benasich, A. A. & Fitch, R. H. (Eds.), Developmental dyslexia: Early precursors, neurobehavioral markers, and biological substrates (pp. 32–45). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Stein, J. (2012b).Visual contributions to reading difficulties: The magnocellular theory. In Stein, J. & Kapoula, Z. (Eds.), Visual aspects of dyslexia (pp. 171–197). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, J., & Kapoula, Z. (Eds.). (2012). Visual aspects of dyslexia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Stein, J., & Talcott, J. B. (1999). Impaired neuronal timing in developmental dyslexia – The magnocellular hypothesis. Dyslexia, 5, 59–77.3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, J., Talcott, J. B., & Walsh, V. (2000). Controversy about the visual magnocellular deficit in developmental dyslexics. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 209–211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, J., & Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read: The magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends in Neuroscience, 20, 147–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, E., & Andrist, C. G. (2012). Dyslexia comes to Congress: A call to action. Retrieved April 17, 2012 from .
Steinbrink, C., Vogt, K., Kastrup, A., Muller, H. P., Juengling, F. D., Kassubek, J., et al. (2008). The contribution of white and gray matter differences to developmental dyslexia: Insights from DTI and VBM at 3.0 T. Neuropsychologia, 46, 3170–3178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stenneken, P., Egetemeir, J., Schulte-Körne, G., Müller, H. J., Schneider, W. X., & Finke, K. (2011). Slow perceptual processing at the core of developmental dyslexia: A parameter-based assessment of visual attention. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3454–3465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephenson, J. (2009). A case study of unfounded concepts underpinning controversial practices: Lost in “Space Dyslexia”. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 56, 37–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephenson, S. (1904). Congenital word blindness. Lancet, 2, 827–828.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephenson, S. (1907). Six cases of congenital word-blindness affecting three generations of one family. Ophthalmoscope, 5, 482–484.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (1999a). Myths in psychology and education regarding the gene-environment debate. Teachers College Record, 100, 536–553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (1999b). Our labeled children. (What every parent and teacher needs to know about learning disabilities). Reading, MA: Perseus Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2002a). Difference scores in the identification of children with learning disabilities: It's time to use a different method. Journal of School Psychology, 40, 65–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2002b). Dynamic testing. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stevens, C., Harn, B., Chard, D., Currin, J., Parisi, D., & Neville, H. (2013). Examining the role of attention and instruction in at-risk kindergarteners: Electrophysiological measures of selective auditory attention before and after an early literacy intervention. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(1), 73–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevenson, J. (2011). Commentary: A contribution to evidence-informed education policy – reflections on Strong, Torgerson, Torgerson, and Hulme. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 236–237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoodley, C. J., Fawcett, A. J., Nicolson, R. I., & Stein, J. F. (2006). Balancing and pointing tasks in dyslexic and control adults. Dyslexia, 12, 276–288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoodley, C. J., & Stein, J. F. (2011). The cerebellum and dyslexia. Cortex, 47, 101–116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoodley, C. J., & Stein, J. F. (2013). Cerebellar function in developmental dyslexia. Cerebellum, 12(2), 267–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, A. A., & Lehtinen, L. E. (1947). Psychopathology and education of the brain-injured child. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Strong, G. K., Torgerson, C. J., Torgerson, D., & Hulme, C. (2011). A systematic meta-analytic review of evidence for the effectiveness of the “Fast ForWord” language intervention program. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(3), 224–235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (2002). Deficits in phoneme awareness do not arise from failures in rapid auditory processing. Reading and Writing, 15, 5–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Mody, M. (1995). Auditory temporal perception deficits in the reading impaired: A critical review of the evidence. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 508–514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stuebing, K. K., Barth, A. E., Molfese, P. J., Weiss, B., & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). IQ is not strongly related to response to reading instruction: A meta-analytic interpretation. Exceptional Children, 76, 31–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., Branum-Martin, L., & Francis, D. J. (2012). Evaluation of the technical adequacy of three methods for identifying specific learning disabilities based on cognitive discrepancies. School Psychology Review, 41, 3–22.Google ScholarPubMed
Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., & Hughes, L. C. (2012). Meta-analysis and inadequate responders to intervention: A response. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(6), 565–569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., LeDoux, J. M., Lyon, R. G., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2002). Validity of IQ-discrepancy classifications of reading disabilities: A meta-analysis. American Educational Research Journal, 39, 469–518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sumner, C. R., Gathercole, S., Greenbaum, M., Rubin, R., Williams, D., Hollandbeck, M., et al. (2009). Atomoxetine for the treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children with ADHD and dyslexia. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 3, 40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sunseth, K., & Bowers, P. G. (2002). Rapid naming and phonemic awareness: Contributions to reading, spelling, and orthographic knowledge. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 401–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Surányi, Z., Csépe, V., Richardson, U., Thomson, J. M., Honbolygó, F., & Goswami, U. (2009). Sensitivity to rhythmic parameters in dyslexic children: A comparison of Hungarian and English. Reading and Writing, 22, 41–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. L. (1978). Memory development in learning disabled children: Evidence from nonverbal tasks. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 100, 9–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. L. (1999). Reading research for students with LD: A meta-analysis of intervention outcomes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 504–532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. L. (2003). Age-related differences in learning disabled and skilled readers’ working memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85, 1–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. L. (2006). Working memory and reading disabilities: Both phonological and executive processing deficits are important. In Alloway, T. P. & Gathercole, S. E. (Eds.), Working memory and neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 59–88). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L. (2012). Meta-analysis and inadequate responders to intervention: A reply. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(6), 570–575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. L. (2013). Meta-analysis of research on children with learning disabilities. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 627–642). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L., Ashbaker, M. H., & Lee, C. (1996). Learning disable readers’ working memory as a function of processing demands. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61, 242–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. L., Hoskyn, M., & Lee, C. (1999). Interventions for students with learning disabilities: A meta-analysis of treatment outcomes. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L., Howard, C. B., & Sáez, L. (2006). Do different components of working memory underlie different subgroups of reading disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 252–269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. L., & Howell, M. (2001). Working memory, short-term memory, and speech rate as predictors of children's reading performance at different ages. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 720–734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. L., & Hsieh, C.-J. (2009). Reading disabilities in adults: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 79, 1362–1390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. L., & O’Connor, R. (2009). The role of working memory and fluency practice on the reading comprehension of students who are dysfluent readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 548–575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. L., Trainin, G., Necoechea, D. M., & Hammill, D. D. (2003). Rapid naming, phonological awareness, and reading: A meta-analysis of the correlation evidence. Review of Educational Research, 73, 407–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. L., & Zheng, X. (2013). Memory difficulties in children and adults with learning disabilities. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 214–238). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L., Zheng, X., & Jerman, O. (2009). Working memory, short-term memory, and reading disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 260–287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szenkovits, G., & Ramus, F. (2005). Exploring dyslexics’ phonological deficit I: Lexical vs. sub-lexical and input vs. output processes. Dyslexia, 11, 253–268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szwed, M., Ventura, P., Querido, L., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2012). Reading acquisition enhances an early visual process of contour integration. Developmental Science, 15, 139–149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taipale, M., Kaminen, N., Nopola-Hemmi, J., Haltia, T., Myllyluoma, B., Lyytinen, H., et al. (2003). A candidate gene for developmental dyslexia encodes a nuclear tetratricopeptide repeat domain protein dynamically regulated in brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 11553–11558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talcott, J. B., Hansen, P. C., Willis-Owen, C., McKinnell, I. W., Richardson, A. J., & Stein, J. F. (1998). Visual magnocellular impairment in adult developmental dyslexics. Neuro-ophthalmology, 20, 187–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talcott, J. B., Witton, C., Hebb, G. S., Stoodley, C. J., Westwood, E. A., France, S. J., et al. (2002). On the relationship between dynamic visual and auditory processing and literacy skills; results from a large primary-school study. Dyslexia, 8, 204–225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talcott, J. B., Witton, C., McLean, M. F., Hansen, P. C., Rees, A., Green, G. G. R., et al. (1999). Can sensitivity to auditory frequency modulation predict children's phonological and reading skills? NeuroReport, 10, 2045–2050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language, 9, 182–198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P., & Gaab, N. (2006). Dynamic auditory processing, musical experience and language development. Trends in Neurosciences, 29, 382–390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S. S., et al. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 81–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, L. H., Spinks, J. A., Eden, G., Perfetti, C. A., & Siok, W. T. (2005). Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 8781–8785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanaka, H., Black, J. M., Hulme, C., Stanley, L. M., Kesler, S. R., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., et al. (2011). The brain basis of the phonological deficit in dyslexia is independent of IQ. Psychological Science, 22, 1442–1451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tannock, R. (2013). Rethinking ADHD and LD in DSM-5: Proposed changes in diagnostic criteria. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(1), 5–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tansey, M. (1991). Wechsler (WISC-R) changes following treatment of learning disabilities via EEG biofeedback training in a private practice setting. Australian Journal of Psychology, 34, 147–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, E. (2011). Commentary: Reading and attention problems – how are they connected? Reflections on reading McGrath et al. (2011). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 558–559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, E., & Rutter, M. (2008). Classification. In Rutter, M., Bishop, D., Pine, D., Scott, S., Stevenson, J., Taylor, E. & Thapar, E. (Eds.), Rutter's child and adolescent psychiatry (5th ed., pp. 18–31). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, J., Roehrig, A. D., Soden Hensler, B., Connor, C. M., & Schatschneider, C. (2010). Teacher quality moderates the genetic effects on early reading. Science, 328, 512–514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Temple, E. (2002). Brain mechanisms in normal and dyslexic readers. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12, 178–183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Temple, E., Deutsch, G. K., Poldrack, R. A., Miller, S. L., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M., et al. (2003). Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 2860–2865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Temple, E., Poldrack, R. A., Salidis, J., Deutsch, G. K., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M., et al. (2001). Disrupted neural responses to phonological and orthographic processing in dyslexic children: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12, 299–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, C. J. (1905). Congenital “word-blindness” and its treatment. Ophthalmoscope, 3, 380–385.Google Scholar
Thompson, T. M., Sharfi, D., Lee, M., Yrigollen, C. M., Naumova, O. Yu., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2013). Comparison of whole-genome DNA methylation patterns in whole blood, saliva, and lymphoblastoid cell lines. Behavior Genetics, 43, 168–176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomson, J. M., Leong, V., & Goswami, U. (2013). Auditory processing interventions and developmental dyslexia: A comparison of phonemic and rhythmic approaches. Reading and Writing, 26(2), 139–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomson, M. (1990). Developmental dyslexia. London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Thomson, M. (2002). Dyslexia and diagnosis. The Psychologist, 15, 151.Google Scholar
Thomson, M. (2003). Monitoring dyslexics’ intelligence and attainments: A follow-up study. Dyslexia, 9, 3–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomson, M. (2009). The psychology of dyslexia: A handbook for teachers (2nd ed.). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Thornton, K. E., & Carmody, D. P. (2005). Electroencephalogram biofeedback for reading disability and traumatic brain injury. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 14, 137–162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tønnessen, F. E. (1995). On defining ‘dyslexia’. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 39, 139–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tønnessen, F. E. (1997). How can we best define ‘dyslexia’?Dyslexia, 3, 78–92.3.0.CO;2-2>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgerson, C. J., Brooks, G., & Hall, G. (2006). A systematic review of the research literature on the use of systematic phonics in the teaching of reading and spelling. London: Department for Education and Skills.Google Scholar
Torgesen, J. K. (2000). Individual differences in response to early interventions in reading: The lingering problem of treatment resisters. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 15, 55–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgesen, J. K. (2002). The prevention of reading difficulties. Journal of School Psychology, 40, 7–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgesen, J. K. (2004). Lessons learned from research on interventions for students who have difficulty learning to read. In McCardle, P. & Chhabra, V. (Eds.), The voice of evidence in reading research (pp. 355–382). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Torgesen, J. K. (2005). Recent discoveries on remedial interventions for children with dyslexia. In Snowling, M. J. & Hulme, C. (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 521–537). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgesen, J. K. (2007). Using an RTI model to guide early reading instruction: Effects on identification rates for students with learning disabilities. Retrieved February 26, 2012 from .
Torgesen, J. K., Alexander, A. W., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Voeller, K. K., & Conway, T. (2001). Intensive remedial instruction for children with severe reading disabilities: Immediate and long-term outcomes from two instructional approaches. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 33–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torgesen, J. K., Foorman, B. R., & Wagner, R. K. (n.d.). Dyslexia: A Brief for Educators, Parents, and Legislators in Florida – FCRR Technical Report #8. Florida Center for Reading Research. Retrieved August 9, 2012 from .
Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Alexander, A. W. (2001). Principles of fluency instruction in reading: Relationships with established empirical outcomes. In Wolf, M. (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain (pp. 333–355). Parkton, MD: York.Google Scholar
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R.K., Rashotte, C.A., Herron, J., & Lindamood, P. (2010). Computer-assisted instruction to prevent early reading difficulties in students at risk for dyslexia: Outcomes from two instructional approaches. Annals of Dyslexia, 60(1), 40–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Rose, E., Lindamood, P., Conway, T., et al. (1999). Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities: Group and individual responses to instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 579–593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torppa, M., Lyytinen, P., Erskine, J., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2010). Language development, literacy skills, and predictive connections to reading in Finnish children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 308–321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torppa, M., Parrila, R., Niemi, P., Lerkkanen, M.-K., Poikkeus, A.-M., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2013). The double deficit hypothesis in the transparent Finnish orthography: a longitudinal study from kindergarten to Grade 2. Reading and Writing, 26, 1353–1380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townend, J. (2000). Phonological awareness and other foundational skills of literacy. In Townend, J. & Turner, M. (Eds.), Dyslexia in practice: A guide for teachers (pp. 1–29). London: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tran, L., Sanchez, T., Arellano, B., & Swanson, H. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the RTI literature for children at risk of reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44(3), 283–295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tree, J. J., & Kay, J. (2006). Phonological dyslexia and phonological impairment: An exception to the rule?Neuropsychologia, 44, 2861–2873.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tressoldi, P. E., Lonciari, I., & Vio, C. (2000). Treatment of specific developmental reading disorders, derived from single- and dual-route models. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 278–285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troia, G. A., & Whitney, S. D. (2003). A close look at the efficacy of Fast ForWord Language for children with academic weaknesses. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 465–494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tunmer, W. (2011). Foreword. In Brady, S. A., Braze, D., & Fowler, C. A. (Eds.), Explaining individual differences in reading: Theory and evidence (pp. ix–xiii). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Tunmer, W. E. (2008). Recent developments in reading intervention research: Introduction to the special issue. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 299–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., & Chapman, J. W. (2003). The reading recovery approach to preventive early intervention. As good as it gets?Reading Psychology, 24, 337–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., & Greaney, K. (2010). Defining dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 229–243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tunmer, W. E., & Nicholson, T. (2011). The development and teaching of word recognition skill. In Kamil, M. L., Pearson, P. D., Moje, E. B., & Afflerbach, P. (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 4, pp. 405–431). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., & Prochnow, J. E. (2009). Cultural relativism and literacy education: Explicit teaching based on specific learning needs is not deficit theory. In Openshaw, R. & Rata, E. (Eds.), Thinking inside the square: Political and cultural conformity in New Zealand (pp. 154–190). Auckland: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Turkeltaub, P. E., Eden, G. F., Jones, K. M., & Zeffiro, T. A. (2002). Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading: method and validation. Neuroimage, 16, 765–780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, D. A. (2012). Education and neuroscience. Contemporary Social Science, 7(2), 167–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
U.S. Department of Education (2004). Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004 (IDEA), Pub. L. No. 108–446, 118 Stat. 2647.
Vadasy, P. F., & Sanders, E. A. (2010). Efficacy of supplemental phonics-based instruction for low-skilled kindergarteners in the context of language minority status and classroom phonics instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 786–803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vadasy, P. F., Sanders, E. A., & Abbott, R. D. (2008). Effects of supplemental early reading intervention at 2-year follow-up: Reading skill growth patterns and predictors. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 51–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaessen, A., & Blomert, L. (2010). Long-term cognitive dynamics of fluent reading development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 213–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaessen, A., Gerretsen, P., & Blomert, L. (2009). Naming problems do not reflect a second independent core deficit in dyslexia: Double deficits explored. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 202–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valdois, S., Bidet-Ildei, C., Lassus-Sangosse, D., Reilhac, C., N’guyen-Morel, M., Guinet, E., et al. (2011). A visual processing but no phonological disorder in a child with mixed dyslexia. Cortex, 47, 1197–1218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valdois, S., Lassus-Sangosse, D., & Lobier, M. (2012a). The visual nature of the visual attention span disorder in developmental dyslexia. In Stein, J. & Kapoula, Z. (Eds.), Visual aspects of dyslexia (pp. 111–121). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valdois, S., Lassus-Sangosse, D., & Lobier, M. (2012b). Impaired letter-string processing in developmental dyslexia: What visual-to-phonology code mapping disorder? Dyslexia, 18, 77–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valencia, S. W. (2011). Reader profiles and reading disabilities. In Allington, R. & McGill-Franzen, A. (Eds.), Handbook of research on reading disabilities (pp. 25–35). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
van Atteveldt, N., Formisano, E., Goebel, R., & Blomert, L. (2004). Integration of letters and speech sounds in the human brain. Neuron, 43, 271–282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Bergen, E., de Jong, P. F., Maassen, B., Krikhaar, E., Plakas, A., & van der Leij, A. (2012). Child and parental literacy levels within families with a history of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 28–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Bergen, E., de Jong, P. F., Plakas, A., Maassen, B., & van der Leij, A. (in press). IQ of four-year-olds who go on to develop dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
van der Leij, A. (2013). Dyslexia and early intervention: What did we learn from the Dutch Programme?Dyslexia, 19, 241–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Leij, A., van Bergen, E., van Zuijen, T., de Jong, P., Maurits, N., & Maassen, B. (2013). Precursors of developmental dyslexia: An overview of the Longitudinal Dutch Dyslexia Programme Study. Dyslexia, 19, 191–213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Sluis, S., Verhage, M., Posthuma, D., & Dolan, C. V. (2010). Phenotypic complexity, measurement bias, and poor phenotypic resolution contribute to the missing heritability problem in genetic association studies. PLoS ONE, 5(11), e13929. .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Ingelghem, M. C. A., van Wieringen, A. W. J., Vandenbussche, E., Onghena, P., & Ghesquiere, P. (2001). Psychophysical evidence for a general temporal processing deficit in children with dyslexia. Neuroreport, 12, 3603–3607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanDerHeyden, A. M. (2011). Technical adequacy of response to intervention decisions. Exceptional Children, 77(3), 335–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandermosten, M., Boets, B., Luts, H., Poelmans, H., Golestani, N., Wouters, J., et al. (2010). Adults with dyslexia are impaired in categorizing speech and nonspeech sounds on the basis of temporal cues. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 10389–10394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandermosten, M., Boets, B., Wouters, J., & Ghesquière, P. (2012). A qualitative and quantitative review of diffusion tensor imaging studies in reading and dyslexia. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 1532–1552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Zuijen, T., Plakas, A., Maassen, B., Maurits, N. M., & van der Leij, A. (2013). Infant ERPs separate children at risk of dyslexia who become good readers from those who become poor readers. Developmental Science, 16(4), 554–563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vargo, F. E., Grossner, G. S., & Spafford, C. S. (1995). Digit span and other WISC-R scores in the diagnosis of dyslexic children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 80, 1219–1229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaughn, S., Cirino, P. T., Wanzek, J., Wexler, J., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., et al. (2010). Response to intervention for middle school students with reading difficulties: Effects of a primary and secondary intervention. School Psychology Review, 39, 3–21.Google ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, S., Denton, C. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (2010). Why intensive interventions are necessary for students with severe reading difficulties. Psychology in the Schools, 47, 432–444.Google ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, S., & Fletcher, J. M. (2010). Thoughts on rethinking response to intervention with secondary students. School Psychology Review, 39, 296–299.Google ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, S., & Fletcher, J. M. (2012). Response to intervention with secondary school students with reading difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45, 244–256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, S., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Denton, C. A., Wanzek, J., Wexler, J., et al. (2008). Response to intervention with older students with reading difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 338–345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, S., & Linan-Thompson, S. (2003). What is special about special education for students with learning disabilities? Journal of Special Education, 37, 140–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaughn, S., & Roberts, G. (2007). Secondary interventions in reading: Providing additional instruction for students at risk. Teaching Exceptional Children, 39, 40–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaughn, S., Wanzek, J., Murray, C. S., Scammacca, N., Linan-Thompson, S., & Woodruff, A. L. (2009). Response to early reading intervention: Examining higher and lower responders. Exceptional Children, 75, 165–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaughn, S., Wanzek, J., Wexler, J., Barth, A., Cirino, P. T., Fletcher, J., et al. (2010). The relative effects of group size on reading progress of older students with reading difficulties. Reading and Writing, 23, 931–956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, S., Wexler, J., Leroux, A, Roberts, G., Denton, C., Barth, A., & Fletcher, J. (2012). Effects of intensive reading intervention for eighth-grade students with persistently inadequate response to intervention. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(6), 515–525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, S., Wexler, J., Roberts, G., Barth, A., Cirino, P. T., Romain, M. A., et al. (2011). Effects of individualized and standardized interventions on middle school students with reading disabilities. Exceptional Children, 77, 391–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vellutino, F. R. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vellutino, F. R. (1987). Dyslexia. Scientific American, 256, 34–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vellutino, F. R., & Fletcher, J. M. (2007). Developmental dyslexia. In Snowling, M. J. & Hulme, C. (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 362–378). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 45, 2–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Jaccard, J. (2003). Toward distinguishing between cognitive and experiential deficits as primary sources of difficulty in learning to read: A two year follow-up of difficult to remediate, readily remediated poor readers. In Foorman, B. R. (Ed.), Preventing and remediating reading difficulties: Bringing science to scale (pp. 73–120). Baltimore, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Lyon, G. R. (2000). Differentiating between difficult-to-remediate and readily remediated poor readers: More evidence against the IQ-achievement discrepancy definition for reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 223–238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., Sipay, E. R., Small, S. G., Pratt, A., Chen, R., et al. (1996). Cognitive profiles of difficult-to-remediate and readily remediated poor readers: Early intervention as a vehicle for distinguishing between cognitive and experiential deficits as basic causes of specific reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 601–638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., Small, S., & Fanuele, D. P. (2006). Response to intervention as a vehicle for distinguishing between children with and without reading disabilities: Evidence for the role of kindergarten and first grade interventions. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 157–169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Spearing, D. (1995). Semantic and phonological coding in poor and normal readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 76–123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Tanzman, M. S. (1998). The case for early intervention in diagnosing specific reading disability. Journal of School Psychology, 36, 367–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., Zhang, H., & Schatschneider, C. (2008). Using response to kindergarten and first grade intervention to identify children at-risk for long-term reading difficulties. Reading and Writing, 21, 437–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vidyasagar, T. R. (1999). A neuronal model of attentional spotlight: Parietal guiding the temporal. Brain Research Reviews, 30, 66–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vidyasagar, T. R. (2012). Aetiology of dyslexia: A visual perspective on a phonological marker. In Stein, J. & Kapoula, Z. (Eds.), Visual aspects of dyslexia (pp. 151–170). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vidyasagar, T. R., & Pammer, K. (2010a). Dyslexia: A deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14, 57–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vidyasagar, T. R., & Pammer, K. (2010b). Letter-order encoding is both bottom-up and top-down: Response to Whitney. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14, 238–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vigneau, M., Beaucousin, V., Herve, P. Y., Duffau, H., Crivello, F., Houde, O., et al. (2006). Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. Neuroimage, 30, 1414–1432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viholainen, H., Ahonen, T., Cantell, M., Lyytinen, P., & Lyytinen, H. (2002). Development of early motor skills and language in children at risk for familial dyslexia. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 44, 761–769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vinckenbosch, E., Robichon, F., & Eliez, S. (2005). Gray matter alteration in dyslexia: converging evidence from volumetric and voxel-by-voxel MRI analyses. Neuropsychologia, 43, 324–331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vineis, P., & Pearce, N. (2010). Missing heritability in genome-wide association study research. Nature Reviews Genetics, 11, 589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Bastian, C. C., & Oberaver, K. (in press). Effects and mechanisms of working memory training: A review. Psychological Research.
von Plessen, K., Lundervold, A., Duta, N., Heiervang, E., Klauschen, F., Smievoll, A., et al. (2002). Less developed corpus callosum in dyslexic subjects – a structural MRI study. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1035–1044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vourkas, M., Micheloyannis, S., Simos, P. G., Rezaie, R., Fletcher, J. M., Cirino, P. T., et al. (2011). Dynamic task-specific brain network connectivity in children with severe reading difficulties. Neuroscience Letters, 488, 123–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vukovic, R. K., Lesaux, N. K., & Siegel, L. S. (2010). The mathematics skills of children with reading difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 639–643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vukovic, R. K., & Siegel, L. S. (2006). The double-deficit hypothesis: A comprehensive analysis of the evidence. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 25–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vukovic, R. K., Wilson, A. M., & Nash, K. K. (2004). Naming speed deficits in adults with reading disabilities: A test of the double-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 440–450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wadlington, E. M., Elliot, C., & Kirylo, J. (2008). The dyslexia simulation: Impact and implications. Literacy Research and Instruction, 47, 264–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wadlington, E. M., & Wadlington, P. L. (2005). What educators really believe about dyslexia. Reading Improvement, 42, 16–33.Google Scholar
Wadsworth, S. J., Olson, R. K., & DeFries, J. C. (2010). Differential genetic etiology of reading difficulties as a function of IQ: An update. Behavior Genetics, 40, 751–758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, R. K. (2008). Rediscovering dyslexia: New approaches for identification and classification. In Reid, G., Fawcett, A., Manis, F., & Siegel, L. (Eds.), The Sage handbook of dyslexia (pp. 174–191). London: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Brown Waesche, J. B., Schatschneider, C., Maner, J. K., & Ahmed, Y. (2011). Using response to intervention for identification and classification. In McCardle, P., Lee, J., Miller, B., & Tzeng, O. (Eds.), Dyslexia across languages: Orthography and the brain-gene-behavior link (pp. 202–213). Baltimore: Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Muse, A. (2006). Short-term memory deficits in developmental dyslexia. In Alloway, T. P. & Gathercole, S. E. (Eds.), Working memory and neurodevelopmental disorders. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A five-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, J. E. (2010). Recent advances in quantitative EEG as an aid to diagnosis and as a guide to neurofeedback training for cortical hypofunctions, hyperfunctions, disconnections, and hyperconnections: Improving efficacy in complicated neurological and psychological disorders. Applied psychophysiological selective review with implications for neurofeedback remediation and results of treatment in twelve consecutive patients. Journal of Neurotherapy, 10, 45–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, J. E., & Norman, C. A. (2006). The neurophysiology of dyslexia: A selective review with implications for neurofeedback remediation and results of treatment in twelve consecutive patients. Journal of Neurotherapy: Investigations in Neuromodulation Neurofeedback and Applied Neuroscience, 10, 45–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, K., Hall, S., Klein, R. G., & Phillips, D. (2006). Development of perceptual correlates of reading performance. Brain Research, 1124(1), 126–141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallace, M. T. (2009). Dyslexia: Bridging the gap between hearing and reading. Current Biology, 19, R260–R262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, J. J., Bi, H. Y., Gao, L. Q., & Wydell, T. N. (2010). The visual magnocellular pathway in Chinese-speaking children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3627–3633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2013). Working memory deficits in children with reading difficulties: Memory span and dual task coordination. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 188–197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wanzek, J., & Roberts, G. (2012). Reading interventions with varying instructional emphases for fourth graders with reading difficulties. Learning Disability Quarterly, 35(2), 90–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wanzek, J., & Vaughn, S. (2007). Research-based implications from extensive early reading interventions. School Psychology Review, 36, 541–561.Google Scholar
Wanzek, J., & Vaughn, S. (2008). Response to varying amounts of time in reading intervention for students demonstrating insufficient response to intervention. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 126–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wanzek, J., Vaughn, S., Roberts, G., & Fletcher, J. M. (2011). Efficacy of a reading intervention for middle school students with learning disabilities. Exceptional Children, 78, 73–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wanzek, J., Vaughn, S., Scammacca, N. K., Metz, K., Murray, C. S., Roberts, G., & Danielson, L. (2013). Extensive reading interventions for students with reading difficulties after grade 3. Review of Educational Research, 83(2), 163–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wanzek, J., Wexler, J., Vaughn, S., & Ciullo, S. (2010). Reading interventions for struggling readers in the upper elementary grades: A synthesis of 20 years of research. Reading and Writing, 23, 889–912.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, S. B., Ward, T. J., Hatt, C. V., Young, D. L., & Molner, N. R. (1995). The incidence and utility of the ACID, ACIDS and SCAD profiles in a referred population. Psychology in the Schools, 32, 267–276.3.0.CO;2-Q>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warnke, A., Schulte-Körne, G., & Ise, E. (2012). Developmental dyslexia. In Garralda, M. E. & Raynaud, J. (Eds.), Brain, mind, and developmental psychopathology in childhood (pp. 173–198). Lanham, MD: Jason Aronson Publishing.Google Scholar
Washburn, E. K., Joshi, R. M., & Cantrell, E. B. (2011). Are preservice teachers prepared to teach struggling readers? Annals of Dyslexia, 61, 21–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, E. (1922). How to teach silent reading to beginners. Philadelphia and London: Lippincott.Google Scholar
Watkins, M. W., Kush, J. C., & Glutting, J. J. (1997). Discriminant and predictive validity of the WISC-III ACID profile among children with learning disabilities. Psychology in the Schools, 34, 309–319.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, C., & Willows, D. M. (1995). Information-processing patterns in specific reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 216–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westendorp, M., Hartman, E., Houwen, S., Smith, J., & Visscher, C. (2011). The relationship between gross motor skills and academic achievement in children with learning disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 2773–2779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wexler, J., Vaughn, S., Edmonds, M., & Reutebuch, C. L. (2008). A synthesis of fluency interventions for secondary struggling readers. Reading and Writing, 21, 317–347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wexler, J., Vaughn, S., Roberts, G., & Denton, C. A. (2010). The efficacy of repeated reading and wide reading practice for high school students with severe reading disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 25, 2–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
What Works Clearinghouse (2007). Fast ForWord. Retrieved February 7, 2012 from .
White, S., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Hansen, P., Swettenham, J., Frith, U., et al. (2006). The role of sensorimotor impairments in dyslexia: A multiple case study of dyslexic children. Developmental Science, 9, 237–269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitney, C. (2010). Serial letter encoding is bottom-up, not top-down: Comment on Vidyasagar and Pammer. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 237–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, A., Garside, S., & Enfield, M. L. (1993). Basic facts about dyslexia: What everyone ought to know. Baltimore, MD: The Orton Dyslexia Society.Google Scholar
Wilkins, A. J. (1995). Visual stress. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, A. J. (2003). Reading through colour. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wilkins, A. J. (2012). Origins of visual stress. In Stein, J. & Kapoula, Z. (Eds.), Visual aspects of dyslexia (pp. 63–77). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willburger, E., & Landerl, K. (2010). Anchoring the deficit of the anchor deficit: Dyslexia or attention? Dyslexia, 16, 175–182.Google ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, E. G., Betjemann, R. S., McGrath, L. M., Chhabildas, N. A., Olson, R. K., DeFries, J. C., et al. (2010). Etiology and neuropsychology of comorbidity between RD and ADHD: The case for multiple-deficit models. Cortex, 46, 1345–1361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, E. G., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Comorbidity of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Differences by gender and subtype. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 179–191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., & DeFries, J. C. (2007). Understanding comorbidity: A twin study of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 144B, 709–714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, E. G., Petrill, S. A., Wu, S., Boada, R., DeFries, J. C., Olson, R. K., & Pennington, B. F. (2013). Comorbidity between reading disability and math disability: Concurrent psychopathology, functional impairment, and neuropsychological functioning. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(6), 500–516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willcutt, E. G., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Nigg, J. T., & Sergeant, J. A. (2008). Recent developments in neuropsychological models of childhood disorders. Advances in Biological Psychiatry, 24, 195–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, H., & Mayringer, H. (2001). Is the reading-rate problem of German dyslexic children caused by slow visual processes? In Wolf, M. (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain (pp. 93–102). Timonium, MD: York.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668–680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, H., Schurz, M., Sturm, D., Richlan, F., Klackl, J., Kronbichler, M., et al. (2010). A dual-route perspective on poor reading in a regular orthography: An fMRI study. Cortex, 46, 1284–1298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wise, B. W., Ring, J., & Olson, R. K. (1999). Training phonological awareness with and without explicit attention to articulation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 271–304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, M. (2007). Proust and the squid: The story and science of the reading brain. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexia. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, M., Gottwald, S., Galante, W., Norton, E., & Miller, L. (2009). How the origins of the reading brain instruct our knowledge of reading intervention. In Pugh, K. & McCardle, P. (Eds.), How children learn to read: Current issues and new directions in the integration of cognition, neurobiology and genetics of reading and dyslexia research and practice (pp. 289–299). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Wolf, M., & Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 211–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M., Miller, L., & Donnelly, K. (2000). Retrieval, Automaticity, Vocabulary Elaboration, Orthography (RAVE-O): A comprehensive, fluency-based reading intervention program. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 375–386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, M., O’Brien, B., Adams, K. D., Joffe, T., Jeffrey, J., Lovett, M., et al. (2003). Working for time: Reflections on naming speed, reading fluency,and intervention. In Foorman, I. B. R. (Ed.), Preventing and remediating reading difficulties (pp. 355–380). Baltimore, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Wolf, M., O’Rourke, A. G., Gidney, C., Lovett, M., Cirino, P., & Morris, R. (2002). The second deficit: An investigation of the independence of phonological and naming-speed deficits in developmental dyslexia. Reading & Writing, 15, 43–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolff, P. H., & Melngailis, I. (1994). Family patterns of developmental dyslexia. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 54, 122–131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P. H., Melngailis, I., Obregon, M., & Bedrosian, M. (1995). Family patterns of developmental dyslexia, part II: Behavioral phenotypes. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 60, 494–505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, C. (2006). Metrical stress sensitivity in young children and its relationship to phonological awareness and reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 270–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
World Federation of Neurology. (1968). Report of research group on dyslexia and world illiteracy. Dallas, TX: World Federation of Neurology.Google Scholar
Wright, C. M., & Conlon, E. G. (2009). Auditory and visual processing in children with dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34, 330–355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, C. M., Conlon, E. G., & Dyck, M. (2012). Visual search deficits are independent of magnocellular deficits in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 62, 53–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeatman, J. D., Ben-Shachar, M., Bammer, R., & Feldman, H. M. (2009). Using diffusion tensor imaging and fiber tracking to characterize diffuse perinatal white matter injury: A case report. Journal of Child Neurology, 24, 795–800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeatman, J. D., Dougherty, R. F., Ben-Shachar, M., & Wandell, B. A. (2012). Development of white matter and reading skills. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(44), E3045–E3053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yule, W. (1976). Dyslexia. Psychological Medicine, 6, 165–167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zane, T. (2005). Fads in special education: An overview. In Jacobson, J. W., Foxx, R. M., & Mulick, J. A. (Eds.), Controversial therapies for developmental disabilities: Fad, fashion and science in professional practice (pp. 175–191). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Zappulla, D. C., & Cech, T. R. (2006). RNA as a flexible scaffold for proteins: Yeast telomerase and beyond. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology, 71, 217–224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeri, F., De Luca, M., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2011). Ocular dominance stability and reading skill: A controversial relationship. Optometry & Vision Science, 88(11), 1353–1362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, J., & McBride-Chang, C. (2010). Auditory sensitivity, speech perception, and reading development and Impairment. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 323–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J. C. (2008). Better to lose the anchor than the whole ship. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 244–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., et al. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading. Psychological Science, 21, 551–559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ziegler, J. C., Pech-Georgel, C., Dufau, S., & Grainger, J. (2010). Rapid processing of letters, digits and symbols: What purely visual-attentional deficit in developmental dyslexia? Developmental Science, 13, F8–F14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zirkel, P. A. (2013). The trend in SLD enrollments and the role of RTI. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(5), 473–479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Judica, A., & Martelli, M. (2009). Reading development in an orthographically regular language: Effects of length, frequency, lexicality and global processing ability. Reading and Writing, 22, 1053–1079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zoccolotti, P., & Friedmann, N. (2010). From dyslexia to dyslexias, from dysgraphia to dysgraphias, from a cause to causes: A look at current research on developmental dyslexia and dysgraphia. Cortex, 46, 1211–1215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zorzi, M., Barbiero, C., Facoetti, A., Lonciari, I., Carrozzi, M., et al. (2012). Extra-large letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 11455–11459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zumeta, R. O., Compton, D. L., & Fuchs, L. S. (2012). Using word identification fluency to monitor first-grade reading development. Exceptional Children, 78, 201–220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×