Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T06:47:32.504Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Vigilance, alarm calling, pursuit deterrence, and predation inspection

from Part III - Related behaviors and other factors influencing escape

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2015

William E. Cooper, Jr
Affiliation:
Indiana University–Purdue University, Indianapolis
Daniel T. Blumstein
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Escaping From Predators
An Integrative View of Escape Decisions
, pp. 265 - 286
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barbour, M. A. & Clark, R. W. (2012). Ground squirrel tail-flag displays alter both predatory strike and ambush site selection behaviours of rattlesnakes. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 38273833.Google Scholar
Beauchamp, G. (2008). What is the magnitude of the group-size effect on vigilance? Behavioral Ecology, 19, 13611368.Google Scholar
Beauchamp, G. (2009a). How does food density influence vigilance in birds and mammals? Animal Behaviour, 78, 223231.Google Scholar
Beauchamp, G. (2009b). Sleeping gulls monitor the vigilance behaviour of their neighbours. Biology Letters, 5, 911.Google Scholar
Beauchamp, G. (2011). Collective waves of sleep in gulls (Larus spp.). Ethology, 117, 326331.Google Scholar
Beauchamp, G., Alexander, P. & Jovani, R. (2012). Consistent waves of collective vigilance in groups using public information about predation risk. Behavioral Ecology, 23, 368374.Google Scholar
Beauchamp, G. & Ruxton, G. D. (2012). Changes in anti-predator vigilance over time caused by a war of attrition between predator and prey. Behavioral Ecology, 23, 368374.Google Scholar
Bednekoff, P. A. & Lima, S. L. (2005). Testing for peripheral vigilance: do birds value what they see when not overtly vigilant? Animal Behaviour, 69, 11651171.Google Scholar
Bergstrom, C. T. & Lachmann, M. (2001). Alarm calls as costly signals of antipredator vigilance: The watchful babbler game. Animal Behaviour, 61, 535543.Google Scholar
Blanchard, P. & Fritz, H. (2007). Induced or routine vigilance while foraging. Oikos, 116, 16031608.Google Scholar
Blumstein, D. T. & Armitage, K. B. (1997). Does sociality drive the evolution of communicative complexity: A comparative test with ground-dwelling sciurid alarm calls. American Naturalist, 150, 179200.Google Scholar
Blumstein, D. T., Verneyre, L. & Daniel, J. C. (2004). Reliability and the adaptive utility of discrimination among alarm callers. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London – Series B: Biological Sciences, 271, 18511857.Google Scholar
Bradbury, J. W. & Vehrencamp, S. L. (2011). Principles of Animal Communication, 2nd edn. Sunderland: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Brown, G. E. & Dreier, V. M. (2002). Predator inspection behaviour and attack cone avoidance in a characin fish: The effects of predator diet and prey experience. Animal Behaviour, 63, 11751181.Google Scholar
Caraco, T. (1979). Time budgeting and group size: A theory. Ecology, 60, 611617.Google Scholar
Caro, T. M. (1986). The functions of stotting in Thomson’s gazelles: Some tests of the predictions. Animal Behaviour, 34, 663684.Google Scholar
Caro, T. M. (1994). Ungulate antipredator behaviour: Preliminary and comparative data from African bovids. Behaviour, 128, 189228.Google Scholar
Caro, T. M. (2005). Antipredator Defenses in Birds and Mammals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Caro, T. M., Lombardo, L., Goldizen, A. W. & Kelly, M. (1995). Tail-flagging and other antipredator signals in white-tailed deer: New data and synthesis. Behavioral Ecology, 6, 442450.Google Scholar
Caro, T. M., Graham, C. M., Stoner, C. J. & Vargas, J. K. (2004). Adaptive significance of antipredator behaviour in artiodactyls. Animal Behaviour, 67, 205228.Google Scholar
Carter, A. J., Pays, O. & Goldizen, A. W. (2009). Individual variation in the relationship between vigilance and group size in eastern grey kangaroos. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 64, 237245.Google Scholar
Cooper, W. E. (2010). Timing during predatorprey encounters, duration and directedness of a putative pursuit-deterrent signal by the zebra-tailed lizard, Callisaurus draconoides. Behaviour, 147, 16751691.Google Scholar
Cresswell, W., Hilton, G. M. & Ruxton, G. D. (2000). Evidence for a rule governing the avoidance of superfluous escape flights. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 267, 733737.Google Scholar
Croft, D., James, R., Thomas, P. et al. (2006). Social structure and co-operative interactions in a wild population of guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 59, 644650.Google Scholar
Curio, E. (1978). The adaptive significance of avian mobbing: I. Teleonomic hypothesess and predictions. Zeitscrift fur Tierpsychologie, 48, 175183.Google Scholar
Devereux, C. L., Whittingham, M. J., Fernández-Juricic, E., Vickery, J. A. & Krebs, J. R. (2006). Predator detection and avoidance by starlings under differing scenarios of predation risk. Behavioral Ecology, 17, 303309.Google Scholar
Dimond, S. & Lazarus, J. (1974). The problem of vigilance in animal life. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 9, 6079.Google Scholar
Evans, C. S., Evans, L. & Marler, P. (1993). On the meaning of alarm calls: Functional reference in an avian vocal system. Animal Behaviour, 46, 2338.Google Scholar
Favreau, F.-R., Goldizen, A. W. & Pays, O. (2010). Interactions among social monitoring, anti-predator vigilance and group size in eastern grey kangaroos. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277, 20892095.Google Scholar
Fernández-Juricic, E., Erichsen, J. T. & Kacelnik, A. (2004). Visual perception and social foraging in birds. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 19, 2531.Google Scholar
Fernández-Juricic, E., Gall, M. D., Dolan, T., Tisdale, V. & Martin, G. R. (2008). The visual fields of two ground-foraging birds, House Finches and House Sparrows, allow for simultaneous foraging and anti-predator vigilance. Ibis, 150, 779787.Google Scholar
FitzGibbon, C. D. (1994). The costs and benefits of predator inspection behaviour in Thomson’s gazelles. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 34, 139148.Google Scholar
FitzGibbon, C. D. & Fanshawe, J. (1988). Stotting in Thomson’s gazelles: An honest signal of condition. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 23, 6974.Google Scholar
Fortin, D., Boyce, M. S., Merrill, E. H. & Fryxell, J. M. (2004). Foraging costs of vigilance in large mammalian herbivores. Oikos, 107, 172180.Google Scholar
Ge, C., Beauchamp, G. & Li, Z. (2011). Coordination and synchronisation of anti-predation vigilance in two crane species. Plos One, 6, e26447.Google Scholar
Godin, J.-G. J. & Davis, S. A. (1995). Who dares, benefits: Predator approach behaviour in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata) deters predator pursuit. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 259, 193200.Google Scholar
Goodale, E., Beauchamp, G., Magrath, R. D., Nieh, J. C. & Ruxton, G. D. (2010). Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 25, 354361.Google Scholar
Grafen, A. (1990). Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 144, 517546.Google Scholar
Griesser, M. (2008). Referential calls signal predator behaviour in a group living bird. Current Biology, 18, 6973.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1971). Geometry for the selfish herd. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 31, 295311.Google Scholar
Kaby, U. & Lind, J. (2003). What limits predator detection in blue tits (Parus caeruleus): Posture, task or orientation? Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 54, 534538.Google Scholar
Kenward, R. E. (1978). Hawks and doves: Factors affecting success and selection in goshawk attacks on woodpigeons. Journal of Animal Ecology, 47, 449460.Google Scholar
Külling, D. & Milinski, M. (1992). Size-dependent predation risk and partner quality in predator inspection of sticklebacks. Animal Behaviour, 44, 949955.Google Scholar
Landeau, L. & Terborgh, J. (1986). Oddity and the “confusion effect” in predation. Animal Behaviour, 34, 13721380.Google Scholar
Leger, D. W., Owings, D. H. & Gelfand, D. L. (1980). Single-note vocalizations of California ground squirrels: Graded signals and situation-specificity of predator and socially evoked calls. Zeitscrift fur Tierpsychologie, 52, 227246.Google Scholar
Lima, S. L. (1988). Vigilance during the initiation of daily feeding in dark-eyed juncos. Oikos, 53, 1216.Google Scholar
Lima, S. L. (1992). Strong preferences for apparently dangerous habitats? A consequence of differential escape from predators. Oikos, 64, 597600.Google Scholar
Lima, S. L. (1995). Collective detection of predatory attack by social foragers: Fraught with ambiguity? Animal Behaviour, 50, 10971108.Google Scholar
Lima, S. L. & Bednekoff, P. A. (1999). Back to the basics of antipredatory vigilance: can nonvigilant animals detect attack? Animal Behaviour, 58, 537543.Google Scholar
Lima, S. L., Rattenborg, N. C., Lesku, J. A. & Amlaner, C. J. (2005). Sleeping under the risk of predation. Animal Behaviour, 70, 723736.Google Scholar
Loughry, W. J. & McDonough, C. M. (1988). Calling and vigilance in California ground squirrels: A test of the tonic communication hypothesis. Animal Behaviour, 36, 15331540.Google Scholar
Magurran, A. E. (1986). Predator inspection behaviour in minnow shoals: Differences between populations and individuals. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 19, 267273.Google Scholar
Magurran, A. E. (1990). The adaptive significance of schooling as antipredator defence in fish. Annals Zoologi Fennici, 27, 5166.Google Scholar
Manser, M. B., Seyfarth, R. M. & Cheney, D. L.(2002). Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and urgency. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 5557.Google Scholar
Mathot, K. J., van den Hout, P. J., Piersma, T. et al. (2011). Disentangling the roles of frequency vs. state-dependence in generating individual differences in behavioural plasticity. Ecology Letters, 14, 12541262.Google Scholar
McNamara, J. M. & Houston, A. I. (1992). Evolutionarily stable levels of vigilance as a function of group size. Animal Behaviour, 43, 641658.Google Scholar
Michelena, P. & Deneubourg, J.-L. (2011). How group size affects vigilance dynamics and time allocation patterns: The key role of imitation and tempo. Plos One, 6, e18631.Google Scholar
Milinski, M. (1987). Tit-for-tat in sticklebacks and the evolution of cooperation. Nature, 325, 433437.Google Scholar
Milinski, M., Lüthi, J. H., Eggler, R. & Parker, G. A. (1997). Cooperation under predation risk: Experiments on costs and benefits. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 264, 831837.Google Scholar
Murphy, T. G. (2006). Predator-elicited visual signal: Why the turquoise-browed motmot wag-displays its racketed tail. Behavioral Ecology, 17, 547553.Google Scholar
Murphy, T. G. (2007). Dishonest “preemptive” pursuit-deterrent signal? Why the turquoise-browed motmot wags its tail before feeding nestlings. Animal Behaviour, 73, 965970.Google Scholar
Neill, S. R. S. J. & Cullen, J. M. (1974). Experiments on whether schooling by their prey affects the hunting behaviour of cephalopods and fish predators. Journal of Zoology, 172, 549569.Google Scholar
Öst, M. & Tierala, T. (2011). Synchronized vigilance while feeding in common eider brood-rearing coalitions. Behavioral Ecology, 22, 378384.Google Scholar
Owens, N. W. & Goss-Custard, J. D. (1976). The adaptive significance of alarm calls given by shorebirds on their winter feeding grounds. Evolution, 30, 397398.Google Scholar
Pays, O., Beauchamp, G., Carter, A. J. & Goldizen, A. W. (2013). Foraging in groups allows collective predator detection in a mammal species without alarm calls. Behavioral Ecology, 24, 12291236.Google Scholar
Pays, O., Sirot, E. & Fritz, H. (2012). Collective vigilance in the Greater Kudu: Towards a better understanding of synchronization patterns. Ethology, 118, 19.Google Scholar
Périquet, S., Valeix, M., Loveridge, A. J. et al. (2010). Individual vigilance of African herbivores while drinking: the role of immediate predation risk and context. Animal Behaviour, 79, 665671.Google Scholar
Pitcher, T. J., Green, D. A. & Magurran, A. E. (1986). Dicing with death: Predator inspection behaviour in minnow shoals. Journal of Fish Biology, 28, 439448.Google Scholar
Pravosudov, V. V. & Grubb, T. C.(1998). Body mass, ambient temperature, time of day, and vigilance in tufted titmice. Auk, 115, 221223.Google Scholar
Pulliam, H. R. (1973). On the advantages of flocking. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 38, 419422.Google Scholar
Quinn, J. L., Whittingham, M. J., Butler, S. J. & Cresswell, W. (2006). Noise, predation risk compensation and vigilance in the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. Journal of Avian Biology, 37, 601608.Google Scholar
Rattenborg, N. C., Lima, S. L. & Amlaner, C. J. (1999). Half-awake to the risk of predation. Nature, 397, 397398.Google Scholar
Reale, D., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T. & Dingemanse, N. J. (2007). Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews, 82, 291318.Google Scholar
Ridgway, S., Carder, D., Finneran, J. et al. (2006). Dolphin continuous auditory vigilance for five days. Journal of Experimental Biology, 209, 36213628.Google Scholar
Ruxton, G. D., Sherratt, T. N. & Speed, M. P. (2004). Avoiding Attack: The Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Warning Signals and Mimicry. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. & Marler, P. (1980). Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Animal Behaviour, 28, 10701094.Google Scholar
Shelley, E. L. & Blumstein, D. T. (2005). The evolution of vocal alarm communication in rodents. Behavioral Ecology, 16, 169177.Google Scholar
Sherman, P. W. (1985). Alarm calls of Belding’s ground squirrels to aerial predators: Nepotism or self-preservation? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 17, 313323.Google Scholar
Shultz, S., Faurie, C. & Noë, R. (2003). Behavioural responses of Diana monkeys to male long-distance calls: Changes in ranging, association patterns and activity. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 53, 238245.Google Scholar
Sirot, E. & Pays, O. (2011). On the dynamics of predation risk perception for a vigilant forager. Journal of theoretical Biology, 276, 17.Google Scholar
Sirot, E. & Touzalin, F. (2009). Coordination and synchronization of vigilance in groups of prey: The role of collective detection and predators’ preference for stragglers. American Naturalist, 173, 4759.Google Scholar
Stang, A. T. & McRae, S. B. (2009). Why some rails have white tails: The evolution of white undertail plumage and anti-predator signaling. Evolutionary Ecology, 23, 943961.Google Scholar
Tan, K., Wang, Z., Li, H. et al. (2012). An “I see you” prey–predator signal between the Asian honeybee, Apis cerana, and the hornet, Vespa velutina. Animal Behaviour, 83, 879882.Google Scholar
Templeton, C. N., Greene, E. & Davis, K. (2005). Allometry of alarm calls: Black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science, 308, 19341937.Google Scholar
Thomas, P. O. R., Croft, D. P., Morrell, L. J. et al. (2008). Does defection during predator inspection affect social structure in wild shoals of guppies? Animal Behaviour, 75, 4353.Google Scholar
Tosh, C. R., Jackson, A. L. & Ruxton, G. D. (2006). The confusion effect in predatory neural networks. American Naturalist, 167, E52E65.Google Scholar
Vega-Redondo, F. & Hasson, O.(1993). A game-theoretic model of predator–prey signaling. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 162, 309319.Google Scholar
Wallace, D. J., Greenberg, D. S., Sawinski, J. et al. (2013). Rats maintain an overhead binocular field at the expense of constant fusion. Nature, 498, 6569.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. R. & Evans, C. S. (2012). Fowl communicate the size, speed and proximity of avian stimuli through graded structure in referential alarm calls. Animal Behaviour, 83, 535544.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A. & Zahavi, A. (1997). The Handicap Principle: A Missing Piece of Darwin’s Puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Survivor signals: The biology and psychology of animal alarm calling. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 40, 277322.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K., Noë, R. & Seyfarth, R. M. (1997). Diana monkey long-distance calls: Messages for conspecifics and predators. Animal Behaviour, 53, 589604.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×