Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T20:44:56.351Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Richard K. Larson
Affiliation:
State University of New York, Stony Brook
Viviane Déprez
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Hiroko Yamakido
Affiliation:
Lawrence University, Wisconsin
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Evolution of Human Language
Biolinguistic Perspectives
, pp. 232 - 262
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adcock, Greg J., Dennis, Elizabeth S., Easteal, Simon, Huttley, Gavin A., Jermiin, Lars S., Peacock, William J., and Thorne, Alan. 2001. Mitochondrial DNA sequences in ancient Australians: Implications for modern human origins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98: 537–542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Agrawal, Alka, Eastman, Quinn and Schatz, David. 1998. Implications of transposition mediated by V(D)J-recombination proteins RAG1 and RAG2 for origins of antigen-specific immunity. Nature 394: 744–751.Google Scholar
Wayne, Aldridge, J., Berridge, Kent C., Herman, Mark and Zimmer, Lee. 1993. Neuronal coding of serial order: Syntax of grooming in the neostratum. Psychological Science 4: 391–393.Google Scholar
Alexander, Garrett E. and Crutcher, Michael D.. 1990. Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: Neural substitutes of parallel processing. Trends in Neurosciences 13: 266–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, Garrett E., Delong, Mahlon R., and Strick, Peter L. 1986. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Revue of Neuroscience 9: 357–381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexander, Michael P., Naeser, Margaret A., and Palumbo, Carole L.. 1987. Correlations of subcortical CT lesion sites and aphasia profiles. Brain 110: 961–991.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexander, Richard. D. 1987. The Biology of Moral Systems. Hawthorne, NY: A. de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Alwin, Duane F. 1991. Family of origin and cohort differences in verbal ability. American Sociological Review 56(5): 625–638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambrose, Stanley H. 2001. Paleolithic technology and human evolution. Science 291: 1748–1752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbib, Michael. 2002. The mirror system, imitation and the evolution of language. In Imitation in Animals and Artifacts, eds. Dautenhahn, Kerstin and Nehaniv, Chrystopher L., 229–280. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Arbib, Michael 2005. From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(2): 105–124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armstrong, David F. 1999. Original Signs: Gesture, Sign, and the Source of Language. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Armstrong, David F., Stokoe, William C., and Wilcox, Sherman E.. 1995. Gesture and the Nature of Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, David F. and Wilcox, Sherman E.. 2007. The Gestural Origin of Language. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, Kate and Zuberbuhler, Klaus. 2006. Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature 441: 303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aronoff, Mark, Meir, Irit, Padden, Carol A., and Sandler, Wendy. 2008. The roots of linguistic organization in a new language. Interaction Studies 9: 133–153.Google Scholar
Atlas, Jay. 2005. Logic, Meaning, and Conversation: Semantical Underdeterminacy, Implicature, and Their Interface. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atran, Scott. 2002. In Gods We Trust. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Avital, Eytan and Jablonka, Eva. 2000. Animal Traditions: Behavioural Inheritance in Evolution. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baer, Thomas, Gore, John C., Gracco, L. Carol, and Nye, Patrick W.. 1991. Analysis of vocal tract shape and dimensions using magnetic resonance imaging: Vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 90: 799–828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, Mark C. 2001. The Atoms of Language. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, Yehoshua. 1953a. On recursive definitions in empirical sciences. Eleventh International Congress of Philosophy 5: 160–165.Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, Yehoshua. 1953b. A quasi-arithmetical notation for syntactic description. Language 29: 47–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-Hillel, Yehoshua. 1954. Logical syntax and semantics. Language 30: 230–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, Simon, Tager-Flusberg, Helen, and Cohen, Donald. 2000. Understanding Other Minds. Second edition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barron, Andrew B., Oldroyd, Benjamin P., and Ratnieks, Francis L. W.. 2001. Worker reproduction in honey-bees (Apis) and the anarchic syndrome: A review. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 50(3): 199–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baum, Shari. R., Blumstein, Sheila. E., Naeser, Margaret A., and Palumbo, Carole L.. 1990. Temporal dimensions of consonant and vowel production: An acoustic and CT scan analysis of aphasic speech. Brain and Language 39: 33–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beckman, Mary E., Jung, Tzyy-Ping, Lee, Sook-hyang, Jong, Kenneth, Krishnamurthy, Ashok K., Ahalt, Stanley C., Cohen, K. Bretonnel and Collins, Michael J.. 1995. Variability in the production of quantal vowels revisited. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 97: 471–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benson, Donald F. and Geschwind, Norman. 1985. Aphasia and related disorders: A clinical approach. In Principles of Behavioral Neurology, ed. Mesulam, M.-Marsel, 193–228. Philadelphia, PA: F. A. Davis.Google Scholar
Berridge, Kent C. and Whitshaw, Ian Q.. 1992. Cortex, striatum and cerebellum: Control of serial order in a grooming sequence. Experimental Brain Research 90: 275–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berwick, Robert. 1997. Syntax facit saltum. Journal of Neurolinguistics 10(2/3): 231–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bever, Thomas. C., Carrithers, Caroline, Cowart, Wayne, and Townsend, David J.. 1989. Language processing and familial handedness. In From Reading to Neurons: Issues in the Biology of Language and Cognition, ed. Galaburda, Albert M., 331–357. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bickerton, Derek. 1981. Roots of Language. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma Publishers.Google Scholar
Bickerton, Derek. 1990. Language and Species. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Bickerton, Derek. 1995. Language and Human Behavior. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Bickerton, Derek. 2000. How protolanguage became language. In The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: Social Function and the Origins of Linguistic Form, eds. Knight, Chris, Studdert-Kennedy, Michael and Hurford, James R, 264–284. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, Derek. 2002. Foraging versus social intelligence in the evolution of protolanguage. In The Transition to Language, ed. Wray, Alison, 207–225. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bickerton, Derek. 2003. Symbol and structure: A comprehensive framework for language evolution. In Language Evolution, eds. Christiansen, Morton H. and Kirby, Simon, 77–93. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, Derek. 2007. Language evolution: A brief guide for linguists. Lingua 117: 510–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biederman, Irving. 1987. Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review 94(2): 115–147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Binford, Lewis S. 1985. Human ancestors: changing views of their behavior. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 4: 292–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bingham, Paul M. 1999. Human uniqueness: A general theory. Quarterly Review of Biology 74(2): 133–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bingham, Paul M. 2000. Human evolution and human history: A complete theory. Evolutionary Anthropology 9(6): 248–257.3.0.CO;2-X>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bingham, Paul M. and Souza, Joanne. 2009. Death from a Distance and the Birth of a Humane Universe. Charleston, SC: BookSurge Press.Google Scholar
Bledsoe, Caroline H., Casterline, John B., Johnson-Kuhn, Jennifer A., and Haaga, John G. (eds.). 1999. Critical Perspectives on Schooling and Fertility in the Developing World. Washington, DC: The National Academy Press.
Blumenschine, Robert J. 1987. Characteristics of an early hominid scavenging niche. Current Anthropology 28: 383–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blumstein, Sheila E. 1994. The neurobiology of the sound structure of language. In The Cognitive Neurosciences, ed. Gazzaniga, Michael S., 915–929. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Blumstein, Sheila E. 1995. The neurobiology of language. In Speech, Language and Communication, eds. Miller, Joanne L. and Eimas, Peter D., 339–370. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blumstein, Sheila E., Cooper, William E., Goodglass, Harold, Statlender, Sheila, and Gottlieb, Jonathan. 1980. Production deficits in aphasia: a voice-onset time analysis. Brain and Language 9: 153–170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogin, Barry 1999. Patterns of Human Growth. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Gerhard C, Bosinski., Jöris, Olaf, Lumley, Marie-Antoinette, Majusuradze, Givi, and Mouskhelishvili, Aleksander. 2000. Earliest Pleistocene hominid cranial remains from Dmanisi, Republic of Georgia: Taxonomy, geological setting and age. Science 288: 1019–1025.Google Scholar
Boutla, Mrim, Supalla, Ted, Newport, Elizabeth L., and Bavelier, Daphne. 2004. Short-term memory span: Insights from sign language. Nature Neuroscience 7: 997–1002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradbury, Jack W. and Vehrencamp, Sandra L.. 1998. Principles of Animal Communication. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bricker, Dianne and Squires, Jane. 1999. Ages & Stages Questionnaire: A Parent-completed, Child-Monitoring System. Second edition. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Brinck, Ingar and Gärdenfors, Peter. 2003. Cooperation and communication in apes and humans. Mind and Language 18: 484–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briscoe, Ted. 2003. Grammatical assimilation. In Language Evolution, eds. Christiansen, Morton H. and Kirby, Simon, 295–316. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broca, Paul. 1861. Remarques sur le siege de la faculté de la parole articulée, suivies d'une observation d'aphemie (perte de parole). Bulletin de la Société d'Anatomie (Paris) 36: 330–357.Google Scholar
Browman, Catherine P. and Goldstein, Louis F.. 1995. Dynamics and articulatory phonology. In Mind as Motion, eds. Gelder, Timothy and Port, Robert F., 175–193. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brown, Roger. 1973. A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, Jerome. 1973. Going Beyond the Information Given. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Bruner, Jerome, Goodnow, Jacqueline, and Austin, George. 1956. A Study of Thinking. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bruner, Jerome and Postman, Leo. 1949. On the perception of incongruity: A paradigm. Journal of Personality 18: 206–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bunn, Henry and Kroll, Ellen M.. 1986. Systematic butchery by Plio-Pleistocene hominids at Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. Current Anthropology 27(5): 431–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burling, Robbins. 2005. The Talking Ape. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan and McClelland, James L.. 2005. Alternatives to the combinatorial paradigm of linguistic theory based on domain general principles of human cognition. The Linguistic Review 22: 381–410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, Richard. 1995. The Thinking Ape: Evolutionary Origins of Intelligence. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, Richard and Whiten, Andrew (eds.). 1988. Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Calvert, Gemma A. and Campbell, Ruth. 2003. Reading speech from still and moving faces: The neural substrates of visible speech. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15: 57–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calvin, William H. and Bickerton, Derek. 2000. Lingua ex Machina: Reconciling Darwin and Chomsky with the Human Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carnie, Andrew. 2008. Constituent Structure. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Carpenter, Malinda, Nagell, Katherine, Tomasello, Michael, Butterworth, George and Moore, Chris. 1998. Social Cognition, Joint Attention, and Communicative Competence from 9 to 15 Months of Age. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Carre, Rene, Lindblom, Bjorn, and MacNeilage, Peter. 1995. Acoustic factors in the evolution of the human vocal tract. C. R. Académie des Sciences Paris, 320(IIb), 471–476.Google Scholar
Carroll, Sean. 2005. Endless Forms Most Beautiful: The New Science of Evo Devo and the Making of the Animal Kingdom. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Carruthers, Peter and Smith, Peter K. (eds.). 1996. Theories of Theories of Mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Carston, Robyn. 2002. Thoughts and Utterances: The Pragmatics of Explicit Communication. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaitin, Gregory J. 2006. Meta-Math!: The Quest for Omega. New York: Vintage.Google Scholar
Changeux, Jean Pierre, Courrege, Philippe and Danchin, Antoine. 1973. Theory of epigenesis of neuronal networks by selective stabilization of synapses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 70: 2974–2978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheney, Dorothy and Seyfarth, Robert M.. 2005. Constraints and preadaptations in the earliest stages of language evolution. Linguistic Review 22: 135–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, Dorothy and Seyfarth, Robert M.. 2007. Baboon Metaphysics. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cherniak, Christopher. 2009. Brain wiring optimization and non-genomic nativism. In Of Minds and Language: The Basque Country Encounter with Noam Chomsky, eds. Piattelli-Palmarini, Massimo, Uriagereka, Juan and Salaburu, Pello, 108–119. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cherniak, Christopher, Mokhtarzada, Zekeria, Rodriguez-Esteban, Raul, and Changizi, Kelly. 2004. Global optimization of cerebral cortex layout. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101(4): 1081–1086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiba, Tsutomu and Kajiyama, Masato. 1941. The Vowel: Its Nature and Structure. Tokyo: Tokyo-Kaisekan.Google Scholar
Chimes, Gary. 2001. Factors associated with variation in overhand throwing performance by females. Ph.D. dissertation. Stony Brook University, NY.
Chomsky, Noam. 1955. The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1956. Three Models for the Description of Language. IRE Transactions on Information Theory IT-2(3): 113–124. Reprinted (1965, slightly emended) in Readings in Mathematical Psychology, eds. Luce, R. Duncan, Bush, Robert R., and Galanter, Eugene, 105–124. New York: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1957. Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1966. Cartesian Linguistics: A Chapter in the History of Rationalist Thought. New York: Harper & Row. New ZealandGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1968. Language and Mind. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1972. Remarks on Nominalization. In Studies on Semantics in Generative Grammar, 11–61. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1975a. Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory. New York: Plenum. Excerpted from 1956 revision of 1955 ms., Harvard University and MIT., IL.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1975b. Reflections on Language. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1980. Rules and Representations. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1985. Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1986a. Barriers. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1986b. Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin, and Use. New York: Praeger Scientific.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1987. Language and Problems of Knowledge: The Managua Lectures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1988. Language and Problems of Knowledge. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1992/1993. A minimalist program for linguistic theory. In A View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger, eds. Keyser, Samuel and Hale, Kenneth, 1–52. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1995a. Language and nature. Mind 104: 1–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1995b. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2000. New Horizons in the Study of Language and Mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2002. New Horizons in the Study of Language and Mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2005. Some simple evo-devo theses: how true might they be for language? Paper presented at the Morris Symposium on the Evolution of Language, Stony Brook University, NY, October 2005.
Chomsky, Noam. 2007. Symposium on Margaret Boden, Mind as Machine: A History of Cognitive Science. Artificial Intelligence 171: 1094–1103.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2008. Cartesian Linguistics: A Chapter in the History of Rationalist Thought. Third edition, ed. McGilvray, James. Christchurch, New Zealand: Cybereditions Corp.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam, Hauser, Marc, and Fitch, Tecumseh. 2005. Appendix: The Minimalist Program. unpublished manuscript. http://wjh.harvard.edu/%7Emnkylab/publications/recent/EvolAppendix.pdf.
Condillac, Étienne B.. 1971. An Essay on the Origin of Human Knowledge. Nugent, T. (tr.), Gainesville, FL: Scholars Facsimiles and Reprints. (Originally published 1746.)Google Scholar
Cools, Roshan, Barker, Roger A., Sahakian, Barbara J., and Robbins, Trevor W.. 2001. Mechanisms of cognitive set flexibility in Parkinson's disease. Brain 124: 2503–2512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coop, Graham, Bullaughey, Kevin, Luca, Francesca, and Przeworski, Molly. 2008. The timing of selection at the human FOXP2 gene. Molecular Biology and Evolution 25: 1257–1259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corballis, Michael C. 2002. From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Corballis, Michael C. 2003. From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26(2): 199–260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corballis, Michael C. 2004a. FOXP2 and the mirror system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(2): 95–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corballis, Michael C. 2004b. The origins of modernity: Was autonomous speech the critical factor? Psychological Review 111: 543–552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Correia, Sérgio P. C., Dickinson, Anthony, and Clayton, Nicola S.. 2007. Western scrub-jays anticipate future needs independently of their current motivational state. Current Biology 17: 856–861.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cretekos, Chris J., Wang, Ying, Green, Eric D., ,NISC Comparative Sequencing Program, Martin, James F., Rasweiler, John J., and Behringer, Richard R.. 2008. Regulatory divergence modifies limb length between mammals. Genes and Development 22: 141–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Critchley, MacDonald. 1975. Silent Language. London: Butterworth.Google Scholar
Crow, Timothy J. 2002. Sexual selection, timing, and an X-Y homologous gene: Did Homo sapiens speciate on the Y chromosome? In The Speciation of Modern Homo Sapiens, ed. Crow, Timothy J., 197–216. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, Peter W. and Jackendoff, Ray. 2005. Simpler Syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummings, Jeffrey L. 1993. Frontal-Subcortical circuits and human behavior. Archives of Neurology50: 873–880.Google ScholarPubMed
Cummings, Jeffrey L. and Benson, D. Frank. 1984. Subcortical dementia: Review of an emerging concept. Archives of Neurology 41: 874–879.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutland, Nigel. 1980. Computability: An Introduction to Recursive Function Theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dale, Philip S., Dionne, Ginette, Eley, Thalia C., and Plomin, Robert. 2000. Lexical and grammatical development: A behavioural genetic perspective. Journal of Child Language 27: 619–642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darwin, Charles. 1859. On the Origin of Species. Facsimile, ed. 1964 Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Davis, Martin. 1958. Computability and Unsolvability. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Dawkins, Richard. 1976. The Selfish Gene. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, Richard 1986. The Blind Watchmaker. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Dawkins, Richard 1996. Climbing Mount Improbable. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Day, Ruth S. 1979. Verbal fluency and the language-bound effect. In Individual Differences in Language Ability and Language Behavior, eds. Fillmore, Charles J., Kempler, Daniel, and Wang, William S.-Y., 57–84. New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, Terrence William. 1997. The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Decety, Jean, Grèzes, Julie, Costes, Nicolas, Perani, Daniela, Jeannerod, Marc, Procyk, Emmanuel, Grassi, Fabrizio, and Fazio, Ferruccio. 1997. Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy. Brain 120: 1763–1777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeGraff, Michel (ed.). 1999. Language Creation and Language Change: Creolization, Diachrony, and Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
DeGusta, David, Gilbert, W. Henry, and Turner, Scott P.. 1999. Hypoglossal canal size and hominid speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 96: 1800–1804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinzelin, Jean, Clark, Desmond J., White, Tim, Hart, William, Renne, Paul, WoldeGabriel, Giday, Beyene, Yonas, and Vrba, Elisabeth. 1999. Environment and behavior of 2.5-million-year-old bouri hominids. Science 284: 625–629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jong, Peter F. 1999. Hierarchical regression analysis in structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling 6: 198–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delong, Mahlon R. 1993. Overview of basal ganglia function. In Role of the Cerebellum and Basal Ganglia in Voluntary Movement, eds. Mano, Noriichi, Hamada, Ikuma, and DeLong, Mahlon R., 65–70. Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica.Google Scholar
D'Esposito, Mark and Alexander, Michael P.. 1995. Subcortical Aphasia: Distinct profiles following left putaminal hemorrhage. Neurology 45: 38–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dessalles, Jean-Louis. 2000. Aux origines du langage. Une histoire naturelle de la parole. Paris: Hermès.Google Scholar
Dessalles, Jean-Louis 2004. About the adaptiveness of syntactic recursion. Coevolution of Language and Theory of Mind. Interdisciplines: electronic conference.Google Scholar
Dessalles, Jean-Louis 2007. Why We Talk. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Di Sciullo, Anna Maria and Williams, Edwin. 1978. Words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dobzhansky, Theodosius. 1955. Evolution, Genetics, and Man. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Dobzhansky, Theodosius. 1973. Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. American Biology Teacher. 35: 125–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dor, Daniel and Jablonka, Eva. 2000. From cultural selection to genetic selection: a framework for the evolution of language. Selection 1(1–3): 33–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dor, Daniel and Jablonka, Eva. 2001. How language changed the genes: Towards an explicit account of the evolution of language. In New Essays on the Origin of Language, eds. Trabant, Jürgen and Ward, Sean, 149–175. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Dor, Daniel and Jablonka, Eva. 2004. Culture and genes in the evolution of human language. In Human Paleoecology in the Levantine Corridor, eds. Goren-Inbar, Naama and Speth, John D., 105–114. Oxford, UK: Oxbow Books.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Zoltán. 2005. The Psychology of the Language Learner: Individual Differences in Second Language Acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dronkers, Nina. F., Shapiro, Johnna K., Redfern, Brenda, and Knight, Robert T.. 1992 The role of Broca's area in Broca's aphasia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 14: 52–53.Google Scholar
Ducrot, Oswald 1972. Dire et ne pas dire. Paris: Hermann.Google Scholar
Dugatkin, Lee Alan. 1997. Cooperation Among Animals: An Evolutionary Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, Robin. 1996. Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language: London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Dunbar, Robin. 1998. Theory of mind and the evolution of language. In Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases, eds. Hurford, James R., Studdert-Kennedy, Michael, and Knight, Chris, 92–110. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dyer, Fred C. and Gould, James L.. 1983. Honey bee navigation. American Scientist 71: 587–597Google Scholar
Edelman, Gerald M. 1987. Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Egnor, S. E. Roian, Iguina, Carmen G., and Hauser, Marc D.. 2006. Perturbation of auditory feedback causes systematic perturbation in vocal structure in adult cotton-top tamarins. Journal of Experimental Biology 209: 3652–3663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egnor, S. E. Roian, Wickelgren, Jeanette Graham, and Hauser, Marc D.. 2007. Tracking silence: adjusting vocal production to avoid acoustic interference. Journal of Comparative Physiology 193: 477–483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellen, Paul and Thinus-Blanc, Catherine (eds.). 1987. Cognitive Processes and Spatial Orientation in Animal and Man, vol. I: Experimental Animal Psychology and Ethology. Leiden, The Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff Publishers.
Enard, Wolfgang, Przeworski, Molly, Fisher, Simon E., Lai, Cecilia S. L., Wiebe, Victor, Kitano, Takashi, and Pääbo, Svante. 2002. Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature 418: 869–871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, Richard L. and Carnielli, Walter A.. 2000. Computability: Computable Functions, Logic, and the Foundations of Mathematics. Second edition. London: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.Google Scholar
Erwin, Douglas. 2003. The Goldilocks hypothesis. Science 302: 1682–1683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everett, Daniel L. 1986. Pirahã. In Handbook of Amazonian Languages, vol. I, eds. Derbyshire, Desmond C. and Pullum, Geoffrey K, 200. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Everett, Daniel L. 1991. A Língua Pirahã e a Teoria da Sintaxe. São Paulo: Unicamp.Google Scholar
Everett, Daniel L. 1992. A língua pirahã e a teoria da sintaxe: Descrição, perspectivas e teoria. Dissertation. Editora Unicamp.
Everett, Daniel L. 2005. Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã. Current Anthropology 46(4): 621–646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falconer, Douglas S. 1960. Introduction to Quantitative Genetics. New York: Ronald Press Company.Google Scholar
Fant, Gunnar. 1960. Acoustic Theory of Speech Production. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Feinberg, Michael J. and Ekberg, Olle. 1990. Deglutition after near-fatal choking episode: radiologic evaluation. Radiology 176: 637–640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, Larry, Dale, Philip S., Reznick, J. Steven, Bates, Elizabeth, Thal, Donna J., and Pethick, Stephen J.. 1994. Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59, Serial No. 242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischman, Josh. 2005. The Pathfinders. National Geographic 207(4): 16–27.Google Scholar
Fisher, S. E., and Marcus, G. F.. 2006. The eloquent ape: genes, brains and the evolution of language. Nature Reviews, Genetics 7: 9–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, Simon. E., Vargha-Khadem, Faraneh, Watkins, Kate E., Monaco, Anthony P., and Pembry, Marcus E.. 1998. Localization of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature Genetics 18: 168–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 1997. Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in macaque monkeys. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 102: 1213–1222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2000a. The evolution of speech: A comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 258–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2000b. Skull dimensions in relation to body size in nonhuman mammals: The causal bases for acoustic allometry. Zoology 103: 40–58.Google Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2005. The evolution of language: A comparative review. Biology and Philosophy 20: 193–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2006. Production of vocalizations in mammals. In Brown, Keith, (ed.) Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, 115–121. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh. in press. Prolegomena to a science of biolinguistics. In Learning from Animals?: Examining the Nature of Human Uniqueness, eds. Roeska-Hardy, Louise and Neumann-Held, Eva M., 15–44. London: Psychology Press.
Fitch, W. Tecumseh and Hauser, Marc D.. 2002. Unpacking honesty: generating and extracting information from acoustic signals. In Acoustic Communication, eds. Megala-Simmons, Andrea and Popper, Arthur N., 65–137. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh and Hauser, Marc D.. 2004. Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science 303: 377–380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh, Hauser, Marc, and Chomsky, Noam. 2005. The evolution of the language faculty: clarifications and implications. Cognition 97. 179–210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitch, W. Tecumseh and Reby, David. 2001. The descended larynx is not uniquely human. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 268: 1669–1675.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flowers, Kenneth A. and Robertson, Colin. 1985. The effects of Parkinson's disease on the ability to maintain a mental set. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, Psychiatry 48: 517–529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, Jerry. 2003. Hume Variations. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry and Piattelli-Palmarini, Massimo. in press. What Darwin Got Wrong. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Foley, Robert. 1987. Another Unique Species: Patterns in Human Evolutionary Ecology. Harlow: Longman Scientific and Technical.Google Scholar
Fox, Danny and Lasnik, Howard. 2003. Successive-cyclic movement and island repair: The difference between Sluicing and VP-ellipsis. Linguistic Inquiry 34(1): 143–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friederici, Angela D. 2004. Processing local transitions versus long-distance syntactic hierarchies. Trends in Cognitive Science 8(5): 245–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frisch, Karl. 1967. Honeybees: Do they use direction and distance information provided by their dancers? Science 158: 1072–1076.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabunia, L., Vekua, A., Lordkipanidze, D., Swisher, C. C., Ferring, R., Justus, A., Nioradze, M., Tvalchrelidze, M., Anton, S. C., Bosinski, G., Jöris, O., Lumley, M. A., Majsuradze, G., and Mouskhelishvili, A.. 2000. Earliest Pleistocene hominid cranial remains from Dmanisi, Republic of Georgia: Taxonomy, geological setting, and age. Science 288: 1019–1025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leo, Gabunia, Vekua, Abesalom, Lordkipanidze, David, Swisher, Carl C., Ferring, Reid, Justus, Antje, Nioradze, Medea, Tvalcrelidze, Merab, Anton, Susan, and Gallistel, Charles Randy. 1990. Representations in animal cognition: An introduction. Cognition 37: 1–22.Google Scholar
Gallistel, Charles Randy (ed.). 1990. Animal Cognition, Cognition (Special Issue), 37 (1–2).PubMed
Gallistel, Charles Randy. 1997. Neurons and Behavior. In Conversations in the Cognitive Neurosciences, ed. Gazzaniga, Michael S., 71–89. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallistel, Charles Randy. 1998. Symbolic processes in the brain: the case of insect navigation. In Invitation to Cognitive Science, eds. Scarborough, Don and Sternberg, Saul vol. IV: Methods, Models and Conceptual Issues, 1–51. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallistel, Charles Randy. 2000. The replacement of general-purpose learning models with adaptively specialized learning modules. In The New Cognitive Neuroscience, Second edition, ed. Gazzaniga, Michael, 1179–1191. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallistel, Charles Randy. 2002. Frequency, contingency and the information processing theory of conditioning. In Frequency Processing and Cognition, eds. Sedlmeier, Peter and Betsch, Tilmann, 153–171. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ganger, Jennifer and Stromswold, Karin. 1998. The innateness, evolution and genetics of language. Human Biology 70: 199–213.Google ScholarPubMed
Gärdenfors, Peter. 1996. Cued and detached representations in animal cognition. Behavioural Processes 36: 263–273.Google Scholar
Gärdenfors, Peter. 2003. How Homo Became Sapiens: On the Evolution of Thinking. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gärdenfors, Peter 2004. Cooperation and the evolution of symbolic communication. In The Evolution of Communication Systems, eds. Oller, D. Kimbrough and Griebel, Ulrike, 237–256. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gärdenfors, Peter 2007. The cognitive and communicative demands of cooperation. Hommage à Wlodek: Philosophical Papers Dedicated to Wlodek Rabinowicz <http://www.fil.lu.se/hommageawlodek>.
Gardner, R. Allen and Gardner, Beatrice T.. 1969. Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science 165: 664–672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, R. Allen and Gardner, Beatrice T.. 1980. Two comparative psychologists look at language acquisition. In Children's Language, vol. II., ed. Nelson, Keith E., 309–369. New York: Halsted.Google Scholar
Gardner, Andy and West, Stuart A.. 2004. Cooperation and punishment, especially in humans. American Naturalist 164(6): 753–764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gazzaniga, Micheal S. 1985. The Social Brain: Discovering the Networks of the Mind. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gentilucci, Maurizio, Benuzzi, Francesca, Gangitano, Massimo and Grimaldi, Silvia. 2001. Grasp with hand and mouth: A kinematic study on healthy subjects. Journal of Neurophysiology 86: 1685–1699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentilucci, Maurizio and Corballis, Michael C.. 2006. From manual gesture to speech: A gradual transition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30: 949–960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, Timothy Q., Fenn, Kimberly M., Margoliash, Daniel, and Nusbaum, Howard C.. 2006. Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature 440: 1204–1207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerardin, Emmanuel, Sirigu, Angela, Lehéricy, Stéphanie, Poline, Jean-Baptiste, Gaymard, Bertrand, Marsault, Claude, Agid, Yves, and Bihan, Denis. 2000. Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. Cerebral Cortex 10: 1093–1104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerhart, John and Kirschner, Mark. 1997. Cells, Embryos, and Evolution: Towards a Cellular and Developmental Understanding of Phenotypic Variation and Evolutionary Adaptability. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gersting, Judith L. 1999. Mathematical Structures for Computer Science. Fourth edition. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Geschwind, Norman. 1965. Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man: Part I. Brain 88: 237–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbons, Ann. 2006. The First Human: The Race to Discover Our Earliest Ancestors. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Wayt. 2003. The unseen genome: Beyond DNA. Scientific American December: 107–113.
Gibson, Kathleen R. and Jessee, Stephen. 1999. Language evolution and expansions of multiple neurological processing areas. In The Origins of Language: What Nonhuman Primates Can Tell Us, ed. King, Barbara J., 189–227. Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research Press.Google Scholar
Gilbert, Daniel T. and Wilson, Timothy D.. 2007. Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science 317: 1351–1354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, Scott F. 2003. The morphogenesis of evolutionary developmental biology. International Journal of Developmental Biology 47: 467–477.Google ScholarPubMed
Gintis, Herbert. 2000. Strong reciprocity and human sociality. Journal of Theoretical Biology 206(2): 169–179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleitman, Henry and Gleitman, Lila. 1970. Phrase and Paraphrase. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Glenberg, Arthur M. 1997. What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20: 1–19.Google ScholarPubMed
Gold, Mark. 1967. Language identification in the limit. Information and Control 10: 447–474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 2003. Constructions: a new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Science 7(5): 219–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 2005. Constructions at Work: Constructionist Approaches in Context. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan and McNeill, David 1999. The role of gesture and mimetic representation in making language the province of speech. In The Descent of Mind, eds. Corballis, Michael C. and Lea, Stephen E. G., 155–172. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldstein, Kurt. 1948. Language and Language Disturbances: Aphasic Symptom Complexes and Their Significance for Medicine and Theory of Language. New York, Grune and Stratton.Google Scholar
Gomez, Juan Carlos. 2004. Apes, Monkeys, Children, and the Growth of Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gould, James L. 1976. The dance-language controversy. Quarterly Review of Biology 51: 211–4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, Stephen and Lewontin, Richard. 1979. The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society London B 205: 581–598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graffi, Giorgio. 2001. 200 Years of Syntax: A Critical Survey. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing Co.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grafton, Scott T., Arbib, Michael A., Fadiga, Leonida, and Rizzolatti, Giacomo. 1996. Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography. 2. Observation compared with imagination. Experimental Brain Research 112: 103–111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graybiel, Ann M. 1995. Building action repertoires: memory and learning functions of the basal ganglia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 5: 733–741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graybiel, Ann M. 1997. The basal ganglia and cognitive pattern generators. Schizophrenia Bulletin 23: 459–469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graybiel, Ann M. 1998. The basal ganglia and chunking of action repertoires. Neurobiology Memory Learning 70: 119–136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, Benjamin D., Murphy, Dennis L., and Rasmussen, Steven A.. 2000. Neuroanatomically based approaches to obsessive-compulsive disorder: Neurosurgery and transcranial magnetic stimulation. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 23: 671–685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, Patricia M. 1991. Language, tools and brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14: 531–595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, Patricia and Savage-Rumbaugh, E. Susan. 1990. Grammatical combination in Pan paniscus: Process of learning and invention. In Language and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes: Comparative Developmental Perspectives, eds. Parker, Sue Taylor, and Gibson, Kathleen Rita, 540–578. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grewal, Shiv and Moazed, Danesh. 2003. Heterochromatin and epigenetic control of gene expression. Science 301: 798–802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grice, Paul 1957. Meaning. Philosophical Review 66: 377–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, Paul 1989. Studies in the Ways of Words, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grossman, Murray, Carvell, Susan, Gollomp, Stephen, Stern, Matthew B., Vernon, Gwyn, and Hurtig, Howard I.. 1991. Sentence comprehension and praxis deficits in Parkinson's disease. Neurology 41: 1620–1628.Google ScholarPubMed
Grossman, Murray, Carvell, Susan, Gollomp, Stephen, Stern, Matthew B., Reivich, Martin, Morrison, Donald, Alavi, Abass, and Hurtig, Howard L.. 1993. Cognitive and physiological substrates of impaired sentence processing in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 5: 480–498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosz, Barbara J. and Sidner, Candace. 1990. Plans for discourse. In Intentions in Communications, eds. Cohen, Phillip, Morgan, Jerry, and Pollack, Martha, 417–444. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Grush, Rick. 1997. The architecture of representation. Philosophical Psychology 10: 5–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gulz, Agneta. 1991. The Planning of Action as a Cognitive and Biological Phenomenon. Lund, Sweden: Lund University Cognitive Studies 2.Google Scholar
Hale, Kenneth and Keyser, Samuel Jay. 1993. On argument structure and the lexical representation of semantic relations. In The View from Building 20, eds. Keyser, Samuel Jay and Hale, Kenneth, 53–109. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hale, Kenneth and Keyser, Samuel Jay 2002. Prolegomenon to a Theory of Argument Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Halle, Morris and Marantz, Alec. 1993. Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In The View from Building 20, eds. Keyser, Samuel and Hale, Kenneth, 111–176. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hamilton, William D. 1964a. Genetical evolution of social behaviour I. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7(1): 1–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, William D. 1964b. Genetical evolution of social behaviour II. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7(1): 17–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, William D. 1996. Narrow Roads of Gene Land: the Collected Papers of W. D. Hamilton. Oxford/New York: W. H. Freeman/Spektrum.Google Scholar
Hanakawa, Takashi, Immisch, Ilka, Toma, Keiichiro, Dimyan, Michael A., Gelderen, Peter, and Hallett, Mark. 2003. Functional properties of brain areas associated with motor execution and imagery. Journal of Neurophysiology 89: 989–1002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hangartner, Walter. 1969. Trail-laying in the subterranean ant Acanthomyops interjectus. Journal of Insect Physiology 15: 1–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, Brian, Call, Josep and Tomasello, Michael. 2001. Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour 61: 139–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, Zelig. 1986/1951. Structural Linguistics. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hauser, Marc D. 1996. The Evolution of Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hauser, Marc D. 2000. Wild Minds: How Animals Really Think. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Hauser, Marc D. 2001. What's so special about speech? In Language, Brain, and Cognitive Development: Essays in Honor of Jacques Mehler, ed. Dupoux, Emmanuel, 417–434. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hauser, Marc D. 2006. Moral Minds: How Natured Designed a Universal Sense of Right and Wrong. New York: HarperCollins/Ecco.Google Scholar
Hauser, Marc D., Barner, David, and O'Donnell, Tim J.. 2007. Evolutionary linguistics: a new look at an old landscape. Language, Learning and Development 3: 101–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, Marc D., Chomsky, Noam, and Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2002. The faculty of language: What it is, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298: 1569–1579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, Marc D. and Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2003. What are the uniquely human components of the language faculty? In Language Evolution, eds. Christiansen, Morton H. and Kirby, Simon, 158–181. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, Marc D. and McDermott, Josh. 2003. The evolution of the music faculty: a comparative perspective. Nature Neuroscience 6: 663–668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, Marc D., Newport, Elissa L., and Aslin, Richard N.. 2001. Segmenting a continuous acoustic speech stream: Serial learning in cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Cognition 78: B53–B64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Catherine. 1952. The Ape in Our House. London: Gollancz.Google Scholar
Heinrich, Bernd. 1989. Ravens in Winter. New York: Summit Books.Google Scholar
Hellwag, Christoph. 1781. De Formatione Loquelae, Dissertation, Tübingen.
Henke, William L. 1966. Dynamic articulatory model of speech production using computer simulation. Ph.D. Dissertation. MIT.
Henshilwood, Christopher S., d'Errico, Francesco, Marean, Curtis W., Milo, Richard G., and Yates, Royden. 2001. An early bone tool industry from the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa: Implications for the origins of modern human behaviour, symbolism and language. Journal of Human Evolution 41: 631–678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henshilwood, Christopher S., d'Errico, Francesco, Yates, Royden, Jacobs, Zenobia, Tribolo, Chantal, Duller, Geoff A., Mercier, Norbert, Sealy, Judith C., Valladas, Helene, Watts, Ian, and Wintle, Ann G.. 2003. Emergence of modern human behavior: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa. Science 295: 1278–1280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herman, Louis. M. 2002. Language learning. In Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals, eds. Perrin, William F., Wursig, Bernd, and Thewissen, J. G. M. Hans, 685–689. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hermisson, Joachim and Wagner, Günter P.. 2004. The population genetic theory of hidden variation and genetic robustness. Genetics 168: 2271–2284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewes, Gordon W. 1973. Primate communication and the gestural origins of language. Current Anthropology 14: 5–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, Cecilia M. and Galef, Bennett G.. 1996. Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hochstadt, Jesse. 2004. The Nature and causes of sentence comprehension deficits in Parkinson's disease: Insights from eye tracking during sentence picture matching. Ph.D. dissertation. Brown University.
Hockett, Charles F. 1960. Logical considerations in the study of animal communication. In Animal Sounds and Communication, eds. Lanyon, Wesley E. and Tavolga, William N., 392–430. Washington, DC: American Institute of Biological Sciences.Google Scholar
Hockett, Charles F. 1960. The origin of speech. Scientific American 203(3): 88–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstadter, Douglas R. 1979. Gödel, Escher, Bach: An Eternal Golden Braid. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Holldobler, Bert. 1978. Ethological aspects of chemical communication in ants. Advances in the Study of Behavior 8: 75–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopcroft, John E., Motwani, Rajeev, and Ullman, Jeffrey D.. 2000. Introduction to Automata Theory, Languages and Computation. Second edition. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Horn, Laurence. 1989. A Natural History of Negation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Horn, Laurence and Ward, Gregory. 2004. The Handbook of Pragmatics. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Horwitz, Sarah Mccue, Irwin, Julia R., Briggs-Gowan, Margaret J., Heenan, Joan M. Bosson, Mendoza, Jennifer, and Carter, Alice S.. 2003. Language delay in a community cohort of young children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 42: 932–940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubel, David and Wiesel, Torsten. 1959. Receptive fields of single neurons in the cat's striate cortex. Journal of Physiology 148: 574–591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubel, David and Wiesel, Torsten 1962. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. Journal of Physiology 160: 106–154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, Claire, Russell, James, and Robbins, Trevor W.. 1994. Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia 32: 477–492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphrey, Nicholas K. 1976. The social function of intellect. In Growing Points in Ethology, eds. Bateson, Paul P. G. and Hinde, Robert A., 303–317. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Humphrey, Nicholas K. 1993. A History of the Mind. London: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Hurford, James R., Studdert-Kennedy, Michael, and Knight, Chris (eds.). 1998. Approaches to the Evolution of Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hurley, Susan L. and Chater, Nick. 2005. Perspectives on Imitation: From Neuroscience to Social Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ike-uchi, Masayuki. 2003. Predication and Modification: A Minimalist Approach. Tokyo: Liber Press.Google Scholar
Illes, Judy, Metter, E., Jeffrey, Hanson, William R., and Iritani, Shuji. 1988. Language production in Parkinson's disease: Acoustic and linguistic considerations. Brain and Language 33: 146–160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irons, David B., Anthony, Robert G., and Estes, James A.. 1986. Foraging strategies of glaucous-winged gulls in a rocky intertidal community. Ecology 67: 1460–1474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isaac, Llywelyn Glynn. 1982. The earliest archaeological traces. In Cambridge History of Africa, vol. I, ed. Clark, J. Desmond, 157–247. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isaac, Llywelyn Glynn 1984. The archaeology of human origins: Studies of the lower Pleistocene in East Africa 1971–1981. Advances in World Archaeology 3: 1–87.Google Scholar
Iverson, Jana M. and Goldin-Meadow, Susan 2005. Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science 16: 367–371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jablonka, Eva and Lamb, Marion J.. 1995. Epigenetic Inheritance and Evolution: The Lamarckian Dimension. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jablonka, Eva and Rechav, Geva. 1996. The evolution of language in light of the evolution of literacy. In Origins of Language (Collegium Budapest Workshop Series No.2), ed. Trabant, Jürgen, 70–88. Budapest: Collegium Budapest.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray. 2002. Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray. 2007. Language, Consciousness, Culture: Essays on Mental Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray and Lerdahl, Fred. 2006. The capacity for music: What's special about it? Cognition 100: 33–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray and Pinker, Steven. 2005. The nature of the language faculty and its implications for the evolution of language (Reply to Fitch, Hauser, and Chomsky). Cognition 97: 211–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacob, François. 1982. The Possible and the Actual. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Jaenisch, Rudolf and Bird, Adrian. 2003. Epigenetic regulation of gene expression: How the genome integrates intrinsic and environmental signals. Nature Genetics 33: 245–254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jellinger, Kurt A. 1990. New developments in the pathology of Parkinson's disease. In Advances in Neurology, vol. LIII: Parkinson's Disease: Anatomy, Pathology and Therapy, ed. Streifler, Max B., 1–15. New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
Johansson, Sverker. 2005. Origins of Language: Constraints on Hypotheses. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johansson, Sverker, Zlatev, Jordan, and Gärdenfors, Peter. 2006. Why don't chimps talk and humans sing like canaries? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29(3): 287–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joos, Martin (ed.). 1957. Readings in Linguistics: The Development of Descriptive Linguistics in America since 1925. Washington, DC: American Council of Learned Societies.
Joshi, Aravind K., Vijay-Shanker, K., and Weir, David J.. 1991. The convergence of Mildly Context-Sensitive formalisms. In Processing of Linguistic Structure, eds. Sells, Peter, Shieber, Stuart M., and Wasow, Thomas, 31–81. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Judson, Horace F. 1979. The Eighth Day of Creation: Makers of the Revolution in Biology, Plainview, NY: Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press. (New expanded edition 1996.) New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Just, Marcel A., Carpenter, Patricia A., Keller, Timothy A., Eddy, William F., and Thulborn, Keith R.. 1996. Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science 274: 114–116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, Daniel. 2003. A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality (Nobel Lecture). American Psychologist 58(9): 697–720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamp, Yves and Hasler, Martin. 1990. Recursive Neural Networks for Associative Memory. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Kandel, Eric R. and Squire, Larry R.. 2000. Neuroscience: Breaking down scientific barriers to the study of brain and mind. Science 290: 1113–1120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, Hillard, Hill, Kim, Lancaster, Jane, and Hurtado, Magdalena. 2000. A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology 9: 156–185.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kauffman, Stuart. 1993. The Origins of Order. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kauffman, Stuart 1995. At Home in the Universe: The Search for Laws of Self-organization and Complexity. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kay, Richard F., Cartmill, Matt, and Barlow, Michelle. 1998. The hypoglossal canal and the origin of human vocal behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA) 95: 5417–5419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keeley, H. Lawrence and Toth, Nicholas. 1981. Microwear polishes on early stone tools from Koobi Fora, Kenya. Nature 293: 464–465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kegl, Judy, Senghas, Anne and Coppola, Maria. 1999. Creations through contact: Sign language emergence and sign language change in Nicaragua. In Language Creation and Language Change: Creolization, Diachrony, and Development, ed. DeGraff, Michel, 179–237. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Key, Catherine A. 2000. The evolution of human life history. World Archaeology 31(3): 329–350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, Jonathan and Just, Marcel A.. 1991. Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language 30: 580–602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirby, Simon. 1999. Function, Selection and Innateness: The Emergence of Language Universals. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kirby, Simon 2002. Natural language from artificial life. Artificial Life 8(2): 185–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirschner, Marc W. and Gerhart, John C.. 2005. The Plausibility of Life: Resolving Darwin's Dilemma. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Klein, Denise, Zatorre, Robert J., Milner, Brenda, Meyer, Ernst, and Evans, Alan C.. 1994. Left putaminal activation when speaking a second language: Evidence from PET. NeuroReport 5: 2295–2297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, Richard G. 1999. The Human Career. Second edition. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Klein, Richard G., Avery, Graham, Cruz-Uribe, Kathryn, Halkett, David, Parkington, John E., Steele, Teresa, Volman, Thomas P., and Yates, Royden. 2004. The Ysterfontein 1 Middle Stone Age site, South Africa, and early human exploitation of coastal resources. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101: 5708–5715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knight, Chris. 1998a. Introduction: Grounding language function in social cognition. In Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases, eds. Hurford, James R., Studdert-Kennedy, Michael, and Knight, Chris, 9–16. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Knight, Chris 1998b. Ritual/speech coevolution: A solution to the problem of deception. In Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases, eds. Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Knight, C., 68–91. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Knuth, Donald E. 1973. The Art of Computer Programming: Fundamental Algorithms. Second edition. London: Addison Wesley.Google Scholar
Kobayashi, Hiromi and Kohshima, Shiro. 2001. Unique morphology of the human eye and its adaptive meaning: comparative studies on external morphology of the primate eye. Journal of Human Evolution 40(5): 419–435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohler, Evelyne, Keysers, Christian, Umiltà, M. Allessandra, Fogassi, Leonardo, Gallese, Vittorio, and Rizzolatti, Giacomo. 2002. Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science 297: 846–848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Köhler, Wolfgang. 1921. Zur Psychologie des Schimpansen. Psychologische Forschung 1: 2–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köhler, Wolfgang 1925. The mentality of apes, London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Komarova, Natalia and Nowak, Martin. 2001. Natural selection of the critical period for language acquisition. Proceedings of the Royal Society London B 268: 1189–1196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konner, Melvin. 1982. The Tangled Wing: Biological Constraints on the Human Spirit. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Kotz, Sonja. A., Meyer, Martin, Alter, Kai, Besson, Mireilli, Cramon, D. Yves, and Frederici, Angela D.. 2003 On the lateralization of emotional prosody: An fMRI investigation. Brain and Language, 96: 366–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krause, Johannes, Lalueza-Fox, Carles, Orlando, Ludovic, Enard, Wolfgang, Green, Richard E., Burbano, Hernan A., Hublin, Jean-Jacques, Hänni, Catherine, Fortea, Javier, Rasilla, Marco, Bertranpetit, Jaime, Rosas, Antonio, and Pääbo, Svante. 2007. The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neanderthals. Current Biology 17: 1908–1912.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, John R. and Davies, Nicholas B.. 1993. An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Oxford, UK/Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
Krebs, John R. and Dawkins, Richard. 1984. Animal signals: mind reading and manipulation. In Behavioral Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, eds. Krebs, John R. and Davies, Nicholas B., 380–402. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Lai, Cecilia S. L., Fisher, Simon. E., Hurst, Jane A., Vargha-Khadem, Faraneh, and Monaco, Anthony P.. 2001. A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature 413: 519–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lai, Cecilia S. L., Gerrelli, Dianne, Monaco, Anthony P., Fisher, Simon E., and Copp, Andrew J.. 2003. FOXP2 expression during brain development coincides with adult sites of pathology in a severe speech and language disorder. Brain 126: 2455–2462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lange, Klaus W., Robbins, Trevor W., Marsden, C. David, James, M., Owen, Adrian M., and Paul, Geraldine M.. 1992. L-Dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. Psychopharmacology 107: 394–404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larson, Richard K. 1991. The projection of DP (and DegP). Syntax Colloquium Series. University of Indiana – Bloomington.
Larson, Richard K. and Yamakido, Hiroko. 2008. Ezafe and the deep position of nominal modifiers. In Adjectives and Adverbs: Syntax, Semantics, and Discourse, eds. McNally, Louise and Kennedy, Christopher, 43–70. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Larson, Susan 2007. Evolutionary transformation of the hominin shoulder. Evolutionary Anthropology 16: 172–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lashley, Karl S. 1951. The problem of serial order in behavior. In Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior, ed. Jefress, Lloyd A., 112–146. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lasnik, Howard, Uriagereka, Juan, and Boeckx, Cedric. 2005. A Course In Minimalist Syntax: Foundations and Prospects (Generative Syntax). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lehericy, Stephanie, Ducros, Mathieu, Moortele, Pierre-Francois, Francois, Chantal, Thivard, Lionel, Poupon, Cyril, Swindale, Nick, Ugerbil, Kamil, and Kim, Dae-Shik. 2004. Diffusion tensor tracking shows distinct corticostriatal circuits in humans. Annals of Neurology 55: 522–529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leiber, Justin. 2001. Turing and the fragility and insubstantiality of evolutionary explanations. Philosophical Psychology 14.1: 83–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, William and Robertson, L. Marcia. 1997. Comparative primate energetics and hominind evolution. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 102: 265–281.3.0.CO;2-X>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, William and Robertson, L. Marcia 2000. Ecological correlates for home range variation in primates: Implications for human evolution. In On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups, eds. Boinski, S. and Garber, P. A., 628–648. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lerdahl, Fred and Jackendoff, Ray. 1983. A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Leslie, Alan. M., Friedman, Ori, and German, Tamsin P.. 2004. Core mechanisms in “theory of mind.”Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 528–533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lettvin, Jerome, Maturana, Humberto, McCulloch, Warren, and Pitts, Walter. 1959. What the frog's eye tells the frog's brain. Proceedings of the Institute of Radio Engineers 47: 1940–1951.Google Scholar
Levinson, Stephen C. 1983. Pragmatics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, Stephen C. 2000. Presumptive Meanings: The Theory of Generalized Conversational Implicature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, Mark E. 1997. Carnivorean paleoguilds of Africa: Implications for hominid food procurement strategies. Journal of Human Evolution 32: 257–258CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewontin, Richard. 1998. The evolution of cognition: Questions we will never answer. In Invitation to Cognitive Science, eds. Scarborough, Don and Sternberg, Saul Vol. IV: Methods, Models and Conceptual Issues, 107–132. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Liberman, Alvin M., Cooper, Franklin S., Shankweiler, Donald P., and Studdert-Kennedy, Michael. 1967. Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review 74: 431–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtheim, Ludwig. 1885. On aphasia. Brain 7: 433–484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, Daniel E. 1998. Sphenoid shortening and the evolution of modern cranial shape. Nature 393: 158–162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, Daniel E., McBratney, Brandeis M. and Krovitz, Gail. 2002. The evolution and development of cranial form in Homo sapiens. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 99: 1134–1139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, Daniel E., Ross, Callum F., and Ravosa, Matthew J.. 2000. The primate cranial base: Ontogeny, function and integration. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 43: 117–169.3.3.CO;2-9>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, Philip. 1968. Primate vocalizations and human linguistic ability. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 44: 1157–1164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, Philip 1984. The Biology and Evolution of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, Philip 1998. Eve Spoke: Human Language and Human Evolution. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Lieberman, Philip 2000. Human Language and Our Reptilian Brain: The Subcortical Bases of Speech, Syntax, and Thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, Philip 2002. On the nature and evolution of the neural bases of human language. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 45: 36–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, Philip 2006. Toward an Evolutionary Biology of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, Philip and Crelin, Edmund S.. 1971. On the speech of Neanderthal man. Linguistic Inquiry 2: 203–222.Google Scholar
Lieberman, Philip, Crelin, Edmund S., and Klatt, Dennis H.. 1972. Phonetic ability and related anatomy of the newborn, adult human, Neanderthal man, and the chimpanzee. American Anthropologist 74: 287–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, Philip, Friedman, Joseph and Feldman, Lianne S.. 1990. Syntactic deficits in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 178: 360–365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, Philip, Kako, Edward T., Friedman, Joseph, Tajchman, Gary, Feldman, Lianne S., and Jiminez, Eleonora B.. 1992. Speech production, syntax comprehension, and cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease. Brain and Language 43: 169–189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, Philip, Klatt, Dennis H. and Wilson, William H.. 1969. Vocal tract limitations on the vowel repertoires of rhesus monkey and other nonhuman primates. Science 164: 1185–1187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, Philip, Morey, Angie, Hochstadt, Jesse, Larson, Mara, and Mather, Sandra. 2005. Mount Everest: A space-analog for speech monitoring of cognitive deficits and stress. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine 76: 198–207.Google Scholar
Liégeois, Frédérique, Baldeweg, Torsten, Connelly, Alan, Gadian, David G., Mishkin, Mortimer, and Vargha-Khadem, Faraneh. 2003. Language fMRI abnormalities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation. Nature Neuroscience 6: 1230–1237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lightfoot, David. 2000. The spandrels of the linguistic genotype. In The Evolutionary Emergence of Language, eds. Knight, Chris, Studdert-Kennedy, Michael, and Hurford, James R., 231–247. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linz, Peter. 2001. An Introduction to Formal Languages and Automata. Sudbury, MA: Jones & Bartlett.Google Scholar
Lisker, Leigh and Abramson, Arthur S.. 1964. A cross language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements. Word 20: 384–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, John F. 1998. Social sound-making as a precursor to spoken language. In Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases, eds. Hurford, James R., Studdert-Kennedy, Michael, and Knight, Chris, 190–201. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lotka, Alfred J. 1956/1924. Elements of Mathematical Biology. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
MacLarnon, Ann and Hewitt, Gwen. 2004. Increased breathing control: Another factor in the evolution of human language. Evolutionary Anthropology 13: 181–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLean, Paul D. and Newman, Joseph D.. 1988. Role of midline frontolimbic cortex in the production of the isolation call of squirrel monkeys. Brain Research 450: 111–123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacSwan, Jeff. 2005. Codeswitching and generative grammar: A critique of the MLF model and some remarks on “modified minimalism.”Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 8(1): 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marantz, Alec. 1997. No escape from syntax: don't try morphological analysis in the privacy of your own lexicon. In Proceedings of the 21st Annual Penn Linguistics Colloquium, eds. Dimitriadis, Alexis, Siegel, Laura, Surek-Clark, Clarissa, and Williams, Alexander, 201–225. Philadelphia, PA: The University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics.Google Scholar
Marín, Oscar, Smeets, Wilhelmus J. A. J., and González, Agustín. 1998. Evolution of the basal ganglia in tetrapods: A new perspective based on recent studies in amphibians. Trends in Neurosciences 21: 487–494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marr, David. 1982. Vision. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Marsden, C. David and Obeso, Jose A.. 1994. The functions of the basal ganglia and the paradox of sterotaxic surgery in Parkinson's disease. Brain 117: 877–897.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maynard Smith, John. 1982. Evolution and the Theory of Games. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maynard Smith, John, Burian, R., Kauffman, S., Alberch, P., Campbell, J., Goodwin, B., Lande, R., Raup, D., and Wolpert, L.. 1985. Developmental constraints and evolution. The Quarterly Review of Biology 60.3: 265–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maynard Smith, John and Szathmáry, Eörs. 1995. The Major Transitions in Evolution. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mayr, Ernst. 1950. Taxonomic categories in fossil hominids. Cold Spring Harbor Symposium on Quantitative Biology 15: 109–118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBrearty, Sally and Brooks, Alison S.. 2000. The revolution that wasn't: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution 39: 453–563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, Robert C., Strait, David S., Yates, Fredrich, and Lieberman, Philip. forthcoming. The Recent Origin of Human Speech.
McCulloch, Warren, and Pitts, Walter. 1943. A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activity. Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics 5: 115–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDougall, Ian, Brown, Francis H., Fleagle, John G.. 2005. Stratigraphic placement and age of modern humans from Kibish, Ethiopia. Nature 433: 733–736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGurk, Harry and MacDonald, John. 1976. Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature 264: 746–748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medsker, Larry R. and Jain, Lakhmi C. (eds.). 2000. Recurrent Neural Networks: Design and Applications. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Mellars, Paul A. 2006. Going east: New genetic and archaeological perspectives on the modern human colonization of Eurasia. Science 313: 796–800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellars, Paul A. and Stringer, Chris B. (eds.). 1989. The human revolution: Behavioural and Biological Perspectives on the Origins of Modern Humans. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Merchant, Jason. 2001. The Syntax of Silence: Sluicing, Islands, and the Theory of Ellipsis. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Merchant, Jason. 2004. Fragments and ellipsis. Linguistics and Philosophy 27: 661–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Middleton, Frank A. and Strick, Peter L.. 1994. Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognition. Science 266: 458–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Cory T., Dibble, Elizabeth, and Hauser, Marc D.. 2001. Amodal completion of acoustic signals in a nonhuman primate. Nature Neuroscience 4: 783–784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Cory T., Iguina, Carmen Gloria and Hauser, Marc D.. 2005 Processing vocal signals for recognition during antiphonal calling in tamarins. Animal Behaviour 69: 1387–1398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Geoffrey F. 1999. Sexual selection for cultural displays. In The Evolution of Culture, eds. Dunbar, Robin I. M., Knight, Chris and Power, Camilla, 71–91. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Minsky, Marvin. 1986. The Society of Mind. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Mirenowicz, Jacques and Schultz, Wolfram. 1996. Preferential activation of midbrain dopamine neurons by appetitive rather than aversive stimuli. Nature 379: 449–451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moglich, Michael and Holldobler, Bert. 1975. Communication and orientation during foraging and emigration in the ant Formica fusca. Journal of Comparative Physiology 101: 275–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moglich, Michael, Maschwitz, Ulrich, and Holldobler, Bert. 1974. Tandem calling: A new kind of signal in ant communication. Science 186: 1046–1047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monahan, Christopher M. 1996. New zooarchaeological data from Bed II, Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania: Implications for hominid behavior in the early Pleistocene. Journal of Human Evolution 31: 93–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monchi, Oury, Petrides, Michael, Petre, Valentina, Worsley, Keith, and Dagher, Alain. 2001. Wisconsin Card Sorting Revisited: Distinct neural circuits participating in different stages of the task identified by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience 21: 7733–7741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
,Mouse Genome Sequencing Consortium. 2002. Initial sequencing and comparative analysis of the mouse genome. Nature 420: 520–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulcahy, Nicholas J. and Call, Josep. 2006. Apes save tools for future use. Science 312: 1038–1040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Musso, Mariacristina, Moro, Andrea, Glauche, Volkmar, Rijntjes, Michel, Reichenbach, Jürgen, Büchel, Christian, and Weiller, Cornelius. 2003. Broca's area and the language instinct. Nature Neuroscience 6(7): 774–781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadel, Lynn and Piattelli-Palmarini, Massimo. 2003. What is cognitive science? In Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science, vol. I, ed. Nadel, Lynn, xiii–xli. London, UK: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Naeser, Margaret A., Alexander, Michael P., Helms-Estabrooks, Nancy, Levine, Harvey L., Laughlin, Susan A., and Geschwind, Norman. 1982. Aphasia with predominantly subcortical lesion sites: Description of three capsular/putaminal aphasia syndromes. Archives of Neurology 39: 2–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nance, Water E. and Kearsey, Michael J.. 2004. Relevance of connexin deafness (DFNB1) to human evolution. American Journal of Human Genetics 74: 1081–1087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Natsopoulos, Demetrios, Grouios, George, Bostantzopoulou, Sevasti, Mentenopoulos, Georges, Katsarou, Zoe, and Logothetis, John. 1993. Algorithmic and heuristic strategies in comprehension of complement clauses by patients with Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 31: 951–964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nevins, Andrew, Pesetsky, David, and Rodrigues, Cilene. 2007. Piraha? exceptionality: A reassessment [electronic version]. LingBuzz 1–58. http://129.242.176.75:9091/lingbuzz/©wrsYmjShqyDRdYVI/6Fc Retrieved 8 March 2007.
Newmeyer, Frederick. 1991. Functional explanation in linguistics and the origins of language. Language and Communications 11: 3–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowak, Martin, Komarova, Natalia, and Niyogi, Partha. 2001. Evolution of universal grammar. Science 291: 114–118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowak, Martin, Komarova, Natalia and Niyogi, Partha. 2002. Computational and evolutionary aspects of language. Nature 417: 611–617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowak, Martin, Plotkin, Joshua, and Jansen, Vincent. 2000. The evolution of syntactic communication. Nature 404: 495–498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connell, James F., Hawkes, Kristen, and Blurton-Jones, Nicholas. G.. 1999. Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution 36: 461–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Donnell, Tim J., Hauser, Marc D., and Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2005. Using mathematical models of language experimentally. Trends in Cognitive Science 9: 284–289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okada, Daijiro and Bingham, Paul M.. 2008. Human uniqueness-self-interest and social cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology 253: 261–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oppenheimer, Stephen. 2003. Out of Eden: The Peopling of the World. London: Constable.Google Scholar
Origgi, Gloria. 2001. Interpretare il linguaggio e interpretare gli altri: una o due teorie? Sistemi Intelligenti 13: 171–188.Google Scholar
Origgi, Gloria and Sperber, Dan. 2000. Evolution, communication and the proper function of language. In Evolution and the Human Mind: Language, Modularity and Meta-cognition, eds. Carruthers, Peter and Chamberlain, Andrew, 140–169. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osvath, Mathias. 2009a. In the search of inner worlds: Are humans alone in the mental world of possible futures? In Human Characteristics: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Mind and Kind, eds. Høgh-Olesen, Henrik, Tønnesvang, Jan, and Bertelsen, Preben, 44–64. Cambridge University Scholars.Google Scholar
Osvath, Mathias. 2009b. Spontaneous planning for future stone throwing in a male chimpanzee. Current Biology 19(5): R190–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osvath, Mathias and Gärdenfors, Peter. 2005. Oldowan culture and the evolution of anticipatory cognition. Lund University Cognitive Studies 122.Google Scholar
Osvath, Mathias and Gärdenfors, Peter. 2007. What are the evolutionary causes of mental time travel? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30: 329–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osvath, Mathias and Osvath, Helena. 2008. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: Self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use. Animal Cognition 11(4): 661–674(14).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palleroni, Alberto, Miller, Cory T., Hauser, Marc D., and Marler, Peter. 2005. Prey plumage adaptation against falcon attack. Nature 434: 973–974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, Jeffrey B., Rudin, Nathan J., Lara, Gustavo, and Crompton, Alfred W.. 1992. Coordination of mastication and swallowing. Dysphagia 7: 187–200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parent, Andre. 1986. Comparative Neurobiology of the Basal Ganglia. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Parker, Anna R. 2006. Evolving the narrow language faculty: Was recursion the pivotal step? Paper presented at the 6th International Conference on the Evolution of Language, Rome, April 2006.
Patterson, Francine and Cohn, Ronald H.. 1999. Koko-love!: Conversations with a Signing Gorilla. New York: Dutton Children's Books.Google Scholar
Patterson, Nick, Richter, Daniel J., Gnerre, Sante, Lander, Eric S., and Reich, David. 2006. Genetic evidence for complex speciation of humans and chimpanzees. Nature 441: 1103–1108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peirce, Charles Saunders. 1931–1935. The Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce. vols. I–IV. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Penrose, Roger. 1989. The Emperor's New Mind. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, Irene M. 1987. Acquisition of the same/different concept by an African Grey parrot. Animal Behavior and Learning 15: 423–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, Irene M. 1999. The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Perruchet, Pierre and Rey, Arnaud. 2005. Does the mastery of center-embedded linguistic structures distinguish humans from nonhuman primates? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 12: 307–313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petitto, Laura-Ann. 2005. How brain begets language: On the neural tissue underlying human language acquisition. In The Cambridge Companion to Chomsky, ed. McGilvray, James, 84–101. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petraglia, Michael, Korisettar, Ravi, Boivin, Nicole, Clarkson, Christopher, Ditchfield, Peter, Jones, Sacha, Koshy, Jinu, Lahr, Marta Mirazón, Oppenheimer, Clive, Pyle, David, Roberts, Richard, Schwenninger, Jean-Luc, Arnold, Lee, and White, Kevin. 2007. Middle Paleolithic assemblages from the Indian subcontinent before and after the Toba super-eruption. Science 317: 114–116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrides, Michael, Cadoret, Geneviève, and Mackey, Scott. 2005. Orofacial somatomotor responses in the macaque monkey homologue of Broca's area. Nature 435: 1235–1238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petronis, Arturas. 2001. Human morbid genetics revisited: Relevance of epigenetics. Trends in Genetics 17(3): 142–146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piattelli-Palmarini, Massimo (ed.). 1980. Language and Learning: The Debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Piattelli-Palmarini, Massimo. 1981. Equilibria, crystals, programs, energetic models and organizational models. In Italian Studies in the Philosophy of Science, ed. Chiara, Maria Luisa Dalla, 341–359. Dordrecht, Holland: Reidel.Google Scholar
Piattelli-Palmarini, Massimo. 1989. Evolution, selection and cognition: From “learning” to parameter setting in biology and in the study of language. Cognition 31: 1–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piattelli-Palmarini, Massimo. 2008. Novel tools at the service of old ideas. Biolinguistics 2(2): 237–246.Google Scholar
Piattelli-Palmarini, Massimo and Uriagereka, Juan. 2004. The immune syntax: The evolution of the language virus. In Variations and Universals in Biolinguistics, ed. Jenkins, Lyle, 341–377. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Piattelli-Palmarini, Massimo and Uriagereka, Juan. 2005. The evolution of the narrow faculty of language: The skeptical view and a reasonable conjecture. Lingue e Linguaggio 4(1): 27–79.Google Scholar
Piattelli-Palmarini, Massimo and Piattelli-Palmarini, Massimo. 2008. Still a bridge too far? Biolinguistic questions for grounding language on brains. Physics of Life Reviews 5(4): 207–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piattelli Palmarini, Massimo and Uriagereka, Juan. in press. A geneticist's dream, a linguist's nightmare: The case of FOXP2. In Biolinguistics Investigations, eds. Di Sciullo, Anna Maria and Boeckx, Cedric. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Pietroski, Paul M. 2005. Meaning before truth. In Contextualism in Philosophy: Knowledge, Meaning and Truth, eds. Preyer, Gerhard and Peter, Georg, 255–302. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 1979. Formal models of language learning. Cognition 7: 217–283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, Steven. 1984. Language Learnability and Language Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 1994. The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. New York: William Morrow.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 2003. Language as an adaptation to the cognitive niche. In Language Evolution, eds. Christiansen, Morton H. and Kirby, Simon, 16–37. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Steven and Bloom, Paul. 1990. Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 707–784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Steven and Jackendoff, Ray. 2005. The faculty of language: What's special about it? Cognition 95: 201–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ploog, Detlev. 2002. Is the neural basis of vocalisation different in non-human primates and Homo sapiens? In The Speciation of Modern Homo Sapiens, ed. Crow, Timothy J., 121–135. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Plummer, Thomas. 2004. Flaked stones and old bones: Biological and cultural evolution at the dawn of the dawn of technology. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 47: 118–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, David. 2004. Is language the key to human intelligence?Science 303: 318–320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, David. 2007. Human and animal cognition: Continuity and discontinuity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104: 13861–13867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polit, Andres and Bizzi, Emil. 1978. Processes controlling arm movements in monkeys. Science 201: 1235–1237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Povinelli, Daniel. 2000. Folk Physics for Apes. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pray, Leslie. 2004. Epigenetics: Genome, meet your environment. The Scientist 18(13): 1–10.Google Scholar
Price, Thomas S., Eley, Thalia C., Dale, Philip S., Stevenson, Jim, Saudino, Kim, and Plomin, Robert. 2000. Genetic and environmental covariation between verbal and nonverbal cognitive development in infancy. Child Development 71: 948–959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pylkkänen, L. and Marantz, A.. 2003. Tracking the time course of word recognition with MEG. Trends in Cogntive Science 7(5): 187–189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raby, Caroline, Alexis, Dean, Dickinson, Anthony, and Clayton, Nicola. S.. 2007. Planning for the future by western scrub-jays. Nature445: 919–921.Google ScholarPubMed
Ramus, Franck, Hauser, Marc D., Miller, Cory T., Morris, Dylan, and Mehler, Jacques. 2000. Language discrimination by human newborns and cotton-top tamarins. Science 288: 349–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raymond, Eric Steven. 2004. The Art of Unix Programming. Boston, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Recanati, François. 2004. Literal Meaning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Reed, Kaye E. 1997. Early hominid evolution and ecological change through the African Plio-Pleistocene. Journal of Human Evolution 32: 289–322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhodes, Jill and Churchill, Steven. 2008. Throwing in the middle and upper Paleolithic: Inferences from an analysis of humeral retroversion. Journal of Human Evolution 30: 1–11.Google Scholar
Rissman, J., Eliasesen, J. C., and Blumstein, S. E.. 2003. Am event-related fMRI study of implicit semantic priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.15: 1160–1175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rissman, J., Eliassen, J. C. and Blumstein, S. E.. 2003. An event-related fMRI study of implicit semantic priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15: 1160–1175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, Giacomo and Arbib, Michael A.. 1998. Language within our grasp. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21: 188–194.Google ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, Giacomo, Camarda, Roselino, Fogassi, Leonardo, Gentilucci, Maurizio, Luppino, Giuseppi and Matelli, Massimo. 1988. Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Experimental Brain Research 71: 491–507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, Giacomo L., Fogassi, Leonardo and Gallese, Vittorio. 2000. Mirror neurons: Intentionality detectors?International Journal of Psychology 35(3–4): 205–205.Google Scholar
Roberts, A., William. 2002.” Are animals stuck in time?Psychological Bulletin 128: 473–489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, A. William. 2006. The questions of temporal and spatial displacement in animal cognition. In Comparative Cognition: Experimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence, eds. Wasserman, Edward A. and Zentall, Thomas R., 145–163. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rogers, James, Pullum, Geoffrey K., and Hauser, Marc D.. In review. Evolving linguistic computation. Trends in Cognitive Science.
Rohl, Jeffrey S. 1984. Recursion via Pascal. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roitblat, Herbert L. 1982. The meaning of representation in animal memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5: 353–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ronshaugen, Matthew, McGinnis, Nadine, and McGinnis, William. 2002. Hox protein mutation and macroevolution of the insect body plan. Nature 415: 914–917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, Bridget A., Cerny, Frank J., and Stathopoulos, Elaine T.. 1998. Effects of varied vocal intensity on ventilation and energy expenditure in women and men. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 41: 239–248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sander, Erik K. 1972. When are speech sounds learned?Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 37: 55–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandler, Wendy, Meir, Irit, Padden, Carol, and Aronoff, Mark. 2005. The emergence of grammar: Systematic structure in a new language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102: 2661–2665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanes, Jerome N, Donoghue, John P., Thangaraj, Venkatesan, Edelman, Robert R., and Warach, Steven. 1995. Shared neural substrates controlling hand movements in human motor cortex. Science. 268: 1775–1777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sauerland, Uli, and Gärtner, Hans-Martin (eds.). 2007. Interfaces + Recursion = Language?Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. Sue. 1994. Hominid evolution: Looking to modern apes for clues. In Hominid Culture in Primate Perspective, eds. Quiatt, Duane and Itani, Junichiro, 7–49. Niwot, CO: University Press of Colorado.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. Sue, McDonald, Kelly, Sevcik, Rose A., Hopkins, William D., and Rubert, Elizabeth. 1986. Spontaneous symbol acquisition and use by pygmy chimpanzees (Pan panicus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 115: 211–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. Sue, Shanker, Stuart G., and Taylor, Talbot J.. 1998. Apes, Language and the Human Mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schick, Kathy D. and Toth, Nicholas. 1993. Making Silent Stones Speak: Human Evolution and the Dawn of Technology. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Schmitz, Oswald J. 1992. Optimal diet selection by white-tailed deer: Balancing reproduction with starvation risk. Evolutionary Ecology 6: 125–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schusterman, Ronald J. and Krieger, Kathy. 1984. California sea lions are capable of semantic interpretation. The Psychological Record 34: 3–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schutzemberger, Marco. 1961. On the definition of a family of automata. Information and Control 4: 245–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searcy, William A. and Nowicki, Stephen. 2005. The Evolution of Animal Communication: Reliability and Deception in Signaling Systems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Semaw, Sileshi, Renne, Paul R., Harris, John W. K., Feibel, Craig S., Bernor, Raymond L., Fesseha, N., and Mowbray, Ken. 1997. 2.5-million-year-old stone tools from Gona, Ethiopia. Nature 385: 333–336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semaw, Sileshi, Michael, J. Rogers, Quade, Jay, Renne, Paul, Butler, Robert, Domínguez-Rodrigo, Manuel, Stout, Dietrich, Hart, William, Pickering, Travis, and Simpson, Scott. 2003. 2.6-Million-year-old stone tools and associated bones from OGS-6 and OGS-7, Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. Journal of Human Evolution 45: 169–177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seyfarth, Robert and Cheney, Dorothy. 2003. The structure of social knowledge in monkeys. In Animal Social Complexity: Intelligence, Culture, and Individualized Societies, eds. Waal, F. and Tyack, P., 207–229. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Sherman, Michael Y. 2007. Universal genome in the origin of metazoa. Cell Cycle 6.15: 1873–1877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shipley, Elizabeth, Smith, Carlota, and Gleitman, Lila. 1969. A study in the acquisition of Language. Language 45: 322–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simeone, Antonio, Acampora, Dario, Gulisano, Massimo, Stornaiuolo, Anna and Boncinelli, Edoardo. 1992. Nested expression domains of four homeobox genes in developing rostral brain. Nature 358: 687–690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sipser, Michael. 1997. Introduction to the Theory of Computation. Boston, MA: PWS Publishing.Google Scholar
Skiena, Steven S. 1998. The Algorithm Design Manual. New York: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Soare, Robert I. 1996. Computability and Recursion. The Bulletin of Symbolic Logic 2: 284–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sober, Elliot and Wilson, David Sloan. 1998. Unto Others: The Evolution and Psychology of Unselfish Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, Dan 2000. Metarepresentations in an Evolutionary Perspective. In Metarepresentations: A Multidisciplinary Perspective, ed. Sperber, Dan, 117–137. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, Dan and Wilson, Deirdre. 1986. Relevance Communication and Cognition. Second edition. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Second revised edition 1995.Google Scholar
Sperber, Dan and Wilson, Deirdre. 2002. Pragmatics, Modularity and Mind-reading. Mind and Language 17: 3–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spurzheim, Johann Gaspar. 1815. The Physiognomical System of Drs. Gall and Spurzheim: Founded on an Anatomical and Physiological Examination of the Nervous System in General, and of the Brain in Particular, and Indicating the Dispositions and Manifestations of the Mind. London: Baldwin, Cradock and Joy.Google Scholar
Stabler, Edward P. 2004. Varieties of crossing dependencies: Structure dependence and mild context sensitivity. Cognitive Science 28: 699–720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stabler, Edward P. 2009. Computational models of language universals: Expressiveness, learnability and consequences. In Language Universals, eds. Christiansen, Morten, Collins, Christopher, and Edelman, Shimon. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stalnaker, Robert. 1999. Context and Content: Essays on Intentionality in Speech and Thought. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephens, David W. and Krebs, John R.. 1986. Foraging Theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Stevens, Kenneth N. 1972. Quantal nature of speech. In Human Communication: A Unified View, eds. David, Edward E. and Denes, Peter B., 51–66. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Stoianov, Ivelin. 2000. Recurrent autoassociative networks: Developing distributed representation of hierarchically structured sequences by autoassociation. In Recurrent Neural Networks: Design and Applications, eds. Medsker, Larry R. and Jain, Lakhmi C., 205–241. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
Story, Brad H., Titze, Ingo R., and Hoffman, Eric A.. 1996. Vocal tract area functions from magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 100: 537–554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stowe, Laurie A., Paans, Anne M. J., Wijers, Albertus A., and Zwarts, Frans. 2004. Activation of “motor” and other non-language structures during sentence comprehension. Brain and Language 89: 290–299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Striedter, Georg. 2006. Précis and multiple book review of Principles of Brain Evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29: 1–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stringer, Christopher. B. 1998. Chronological and biogeographic perspectives on later human evolution. In Neanderthals and Modern Humans in Western Asia, eds. Akazawa, Takeru, Aoki, Kenichi, and Bar-Yosef, Ofer, 29–38. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Stromswold, Karin. 1997. Specific language impairments. In Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology, eds. Feinberg, Todd E. and Farah, Martha J., 755–772. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Stromswold, Karin. 1998. The genetics of spoken language disorders. Human Biology 70: 297–324.Google ScholarPubMed
Stromswold, Karin. 2000. The cognitive neuroscience of language acquisition. In The New Cognitive Neurosciences, Second edition, ed. Gazzaniga, Michael, 909–932. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stromswold, Karin. 2001a. The heritability of language: A review and meta-analysis of twin, adoption and linkage studies. Language 77: 647–723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stromswold, Karin. 2001b. The Parent Assessment of Language (PAL) Tests. unpublished ms.
Stromswold, Karin. 2005. Genetic specificity of linguistic heritability. In Twenty-first Century Psycholinguistics: Four Cornerstones, ed. Cutler, Anne, 121–139. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Stromswold, Karin. 2006. Why aren't identical twins linguistically identical? Genetic, prenatal and postnatal factors. Cognition 101: 333–384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stromswold, Karin, Schramm, Kathleen, Molnar, Diane, Holodak, Scott, and Sheffield, Ellyn. 2005. The role of specific and non-specific genetic factors in language development. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
Stromswold, Karin, Sheffield, Ellyn, Truit, Debra, and Molnar, Diane. 2006. Parents can test preschool children's language: The Parent Assessment of Language (PAL) test. Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science Technical Report 84: 1–31.Google Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, Michael. 1998. The particulate origins of language generativity: From syllable to gesture. In Approaches to the Evolution of Language, eds. Hurford, James R., Studdert-Kennedy, Michael, and Knight, Chris, 169–176. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stuss, Donald T. and Benson, D. Frank. 1986. The Frontal Lobes. New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
Suc, Jean-Pierre, Bertini, Adele, Leroy, Suzanne A. G., and Suballyova, Danica 1997. Towards the lowering of the Pliocene/Pleistocene boundary to the Gauss-Matuyama reversal. Quaternary International 40: 37–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sudd, John H. and Franks, Nigel R.. 1987. The Behavioral Ecology of Ants. New York: Chapman & Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, Thomas and Corballis, Michael C.. 1997. Mental time travel and the evolution of human mind. Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monographs 123: 133–167.Google ScholarPubMed
Suddendorf, Thomas and Corballis, Michael C.. 2007. The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30(3): 299–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szabó, Zoltan (ed.). 2005. Semantics versus Pragmatics. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Takahashi, Kaoru, Liu, Fu-Chin, Hirokawa, Katsuiku and Takahashi, Hiroshi. 2003. Expression of FoxP2, a gene involved in speech and language in the developing and adult striatum. Journal of Neuroscience Research 73: 62–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tattersall, Ian. 2009. The Fossil Trail: How We Know What We Think We Know About Human Evolution. Second edition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tattersall, Ian. 1998. The Origin of the Human Capacity. New York: The American Museum of Natural History.Google ScholarPubMed
Tattersall, Ian. 2000. Once we were not alone. Sci American 282(1): 56–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tattersall, Ian. 2002. The Monkey in the Mirror: Essays on the Science of What Makes Us Human. New York: Harcourt.Google Scholar
Tattersall, Ian. 2004. What happened in the origin of human consciousness? The Anatomical Record. Part B, New Anatomist 276B: 19–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tattersall, Ian. 2005. Patterns of innovation in human evolution. Nova Acta Leopoldinia 93: 145–157.Google Scholar
Terkel, Joseph. 1996. Cultural transmission of feeding behavior in the black rat. In Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, eds. Heyes, Cecilia M. and Galef, Bennett G., 17–47. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terrace, Herbert S. 1979. Nim. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Thompson, D'Arcy and Bonner, John. 1917/1992. On Growth and Form. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobias, Philip V. 1987. The brain of Homo habilis: A new level of organization in cerebral evolution. Journal of Human Evolution 16: 741–761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobias, Phillip V. 1994. The evolution of early hominids. In Companion Encyclopedia of Anthropology, ed. Ingold, Tim, 33–78. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 1999. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Unversity Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 2000. Acquiring syntax is not what you think. In Speech and Language Impairments in Children: Causes, Characteristics, Intervention, and Outcome, eds. Bishop, Dorothy V. M. and Leonard, Laurence, 1–15. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 2003. On the different origins of symbols and grammar. In Language Evolution, eds. Christiansen, Morton H. and Kirby, Simon, 94–110. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 2004. What kind of evidence could refute the UG hypothesis? Commentary on Wunderlich. Studies in Language 28: 642–645.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 2005. Beyond formalities: The case of language acquisition. The Linguistic Review 22: 183–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Carpenter, Malinda, Call, Josep, Behne, Tanva and Moll, Henrike. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 675–691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooby, John and Cosmides, Leda. 2000. Toward mapping the evolved functional organization of mind and brain. In The New Cognitive Neurosciences, ed. Gazzaniga, Michael, 1167–1178. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, John and DeVore, Irving. 1987. The reconstruction of hominid evolution through strategic modeling. In The Evolution of Human Behavior: Primate Models, ed. Kinzey, Warren G., 183–238. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Toro, Juan M. and Trobalon, Josep B.. 2005. Statistical computations over a speech stream in a rodent. Perception and Psychophysics 67: 867–875.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toro, Juan M. and Trobalon, Josep B. and Sebastian-Galles, Núria. 2003. The use of prosodic cues in language discrimination tasks by rats. Animal Cognition 6: 131–136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toro, Juan M. and Trobalon, Josep B. and Sebastian-Galles, Núria. 2005. Effects of backward speech and speaker variability in language discrimination by rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 31: 95–100.Google ScholarPubMed
Toth, Nicholas. 1985. The Oldowan reassessed: A close look at early stone artifacts. Journal of Archeological Science 12: 101–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trauth, Martin H., Maslin, Mark A., Deino, Alain, and Strecker, Manfred R.. 2005. Late Cenozoic moisture history of East Africa. Science 309: 2051–2053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treves, Adrian and Naughton-Treves, Lisa. 1999. Risk and opportunity for humans co- existing with large carnivores. Journal of Human Evolution 36: 275–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treves, Alessandro. 2005. Frontal latching networks: A possible neural basis for infinite recursion. Cognitive Neuropsychology 22(3–4): 276–291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
True, Heather, Berlin, Ilana, and Lindquist, Susan. 2004. Epigenetic regulation of translation reveals hidden genetic variation to produce complex traits. Nature 431: 184–189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, Endel. 2005. Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In The Missing Link in Cognition: Evolution of Self-Knowing Consciousness, eds. Terrace, Herbert and Janet, Metcalfe, 3–56. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ujhelyi, Mária. 1998. Long call structure in apes as a possible precursor for language. In Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases, eds. Hurford, James R., Studdert-Kennedy, Michael, and Chris, Knight, 171–189. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Uriagereka, Juan. 1998. Rhyme and Reason: An Introduction to Minimalist Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gelderen, Elly and MacSwan, Jeff. 2008. Interface conditions and code-switching: Pronouns, lexical DPs, and checking theory. Lingua 118(6): 765–776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanhaeren, Marian, d'Errico, Francesco, Stringer, Chris, James, Sarah L., Todd, Jonathan A., and Mienis, Henk K.. 2006. Middle Paleolithic shell beads in Israel and Algeria. Science 312: 1785–1788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaik, Carel P., Ancrenaz, Marc, Borgen, Gwendolyn, Galdikas, Birute, Knott, Cheryl D., Singleton, Ian, Suzuki, Akira, Utamia, Sri Suci, and Merrill, Michelle. 2003. Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science 299(5603): 102–105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vargha-Khadem, Faraneh, Watkins, Kate E., Alcock, Katherine J., Fletcher, Paul, and Passingham, Richard. 1995. Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 92: 930–933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vargha-Khadem, Fararneh, Watkins, Kate E., Price, Cathy J., Ashbruner, John, Friston, Karl J., Frackowiak, Richard S. J., Mishkin, Mortimer, Gadian, David G., and Passingham, Richard E.. 1998. Neural basis of an inherited speech and language disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95: 12695–12700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vauclair, Jacques. 1990. Primate cognition: From representation to language. In “Language” and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes, eds. Parker, Sue Taylor and Gibson, Kathleen Rita, 312–329. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velasco, Juan and Millan, Andres. 1998. Feeding habits of two large insects from a desert stream: Abedus herberti (Hemiptera: Belostomatidae) and Thermonectus marmoratus (Coleoptera: Dytiscidae). Aquatic Insects 20: 85–96.Google Scholar
Vouloumanos, Athena and Werker, Janet F.. 2004. Tuned to the signal: The privileged status of speech for young infants. Developmental Science 7: 270–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrba, Elizabeth S. 1995. Paleoclimate and Evolution, with Emphasis on Human Origins. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, Günter P. 2000. What is the promise of developmental evolution? Part I: why is developmental biology necessary to explain evolutionary innovations?Journal of Experimental Zoology Part A: Ecological Genetics and Physiology 288: 95–98.3.0.CO;2-5>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, Alan and Leakey, Richard E. F. (eds.). 1993. The NariokotomeHomo erectus skeleton. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRef
Wallace, Alfred Russel. 1889. Darwinism: An Exposition of the Theory of Natural Selection, with Some of Its Applications. London, UK: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Watanabe, Shigeru, Yamamoto, Eriko, and Uozumi, Midori. 2006. Language discrimination by java sparrows. Behavioural Processes 73: 114–116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, Kate E., Dronkers, Nina F., and Vargha-Khadem, Faraneh. 2002. Behavioural analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: Comparison with acquired aphasia. Brain 125: 452–464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, Kate E., Strafella, Antonio P., and Paus, Tomas. 2003. Seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production. Neuropsychologia 41: 989–994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, Kate E., Vargha-Khadem, Faraneh, Ashbruner, John, Passingham, Richard E., Connelly, Alan, Friston, Karl J., Frackowiak, Richard S. J., Mishkin, Mortimer, and Gadian, David G.. 2002. MRI analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: Structural brain abnormalities. Brain 125: 465–478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinreich, Daniel M., Delaney, Nigel F., DePristo, Mark A., and Hartl, Daniel L.. 2006. Darwinian evolution can follow only very few mutational paths to fitter proteins. Science 312: 111–114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wernicke, Carl. 1874/1967. The aphasic symptom complex: A psychological study on a neurological basis. In Proceedings of the Boston Colloquium for the Philosophy of Science, vol. IV, eds. Cohen, Robert S. and Wartofsky, Marx W., 34–97. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, Mary Jane. 2003. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wexler, Kenneth and Culicover, Peter. 1980. Formal Principles of Language Acquisition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
White, Randall. 1989. Visual thingking in the Ice Age. Scientific American 260(7): 92–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, Timothy D., Asfaw, Berhane, DeGusta, David, Gilbert, Henry, Richards, Gary D., Suwa, Gen, and Howell, F. Clark. 2003. Pleistocene Homo sapiens from Middle Awash, Ethiopia. Nature 423: 742–747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, Andrew. 2002. The imitator's representation of the imitated: Ape and child. In The Imitative Mind: Development, Evolution, and Brain Bases, eds. Meltzoff, Andrew N. and Prinz, Wolfgang, 98–121. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, Andrew and Byrne, Richard W. (eds.). 1997. Machiavellian Intelligence II: Evaluations and Extensions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Whiten, Andrew, Goodall, Jane, McGrew, William C., Nishida, Toshisada, Reynolds, Vernon, Sugiyama, Yukimaru, Tutin, Caroline E. G., Wrangham, Richard W., and Boesch, Christopher. 1999. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399(6737): 682–685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, George C. 1966. Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, Edward O. 1962. Chemical communication in the fire ant Solenopsis Saevissima. Animal Behavior 10: 134–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolpert, Daniel M., Doya, Kenji, and Kawato, Mitsuo. 2003. A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London, B 358: 593–602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, Bernard J. 2002. Hominid revelations from Chad. Nature 418: 134–135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, Bernard and Collard, Mark. 1999. The human genus. Science 284: 65–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Worden, Robert. 1998. The evolution of language from social intelligence. In Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases, eds. Hurford, James R., Studdert-Kennedy, Michael, and Knight, Chris, 148–166. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wrangham, Richard W., McGrew, William C., Waal, Frans B. M., and Heltne, Paul. 1994. Chimpanzee Cultures. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press (in cooperation with the Chicago Academy of Sciences)Google Scholar
Wundt, Wilhelm M. 1921. Elements of Folk Psychology. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Yang, Charles. 2002. Knowledge and Learning in Natural Language. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Yang, Charles. 2004. Universal Grammar, statistics, or both. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (10): 451–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, Charles. forthcoming Three factors in language variation. Lingua.
Yip, Moira. 2006. The search for phonology in other species. Trends in Cognitive Science 10: 442–446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahavi, Amotz. 1975. Mate selection – a selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology 53: 205–214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zlatev, Jordan, Persson, Tomas, and Gärdenfors, Peter. 2005. Bodily mimesis as the “missing link” in human cognitive evolution. Lund University Cognitive Studies 121.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Edited by Richard K. Larson, State University of New York, Stony Brook, Viviane Déprez, Rutgers University, New Jersey, Hiroko Yamakido, Lawrence University, Wisconsin
  • Book: The Evolution of Human Language
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511817755.018
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Edited by Richard K. Larson, State University of New York, Stony Brook, Viviane Déprez, Rutgers University, New Jersey, Hiroko Yamakido, Lawrence University, Wisconsin
  • Book: The Evolution of Human Language
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511817755.018
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Edited by Richard K. Larson, State University of New York, Stony Brook, Viviane Déprez, Rutgers University, New Jersey, Hiroko Yamakido, Lawrence University, Wisconsin
  • Book: The Evolution of Human Language
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511817755.018
Available formats
×