Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76dd75c94c-lntk7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T09:50:54.868Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 9 - Executive functions in learning disorders

from Section II - Executive Dysfunction in the Neurodevelopmental and Acquired Disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2012

Scott J. Hunter
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
Elizabeth P. Sparrow
Affiliation:
Sparrow Neuropsychology
Get access

Summary

Growing evidence supports the involvement of EF in academic performance and, consequently, EdF in learning disorders (commonly known as learning disabilities or LD). Often, the first indicator of LD is low academic achievement in areas of reading, math, or writing. The terminology used to describe different LD conditions varies by field and includes dyslexia and reading disorder/disability, dyscalculia and mathematics disorder/disability, and disorder of written expression/writing disability. Comorbidity among these learning disorders is common, as is the co-occurrence of LD with ADHD.

Recent US data indicate that almost 2.5 million public school students received special education and related services for LD in 2008, and 43% of all public school students receiving special education and related services were identified as having a specific learning disability. Federal guidelines used in educational settings indicate that a child may have a specific LD if, in the absence of any other disability or limiting condition and despite appropriate instruction and experiences, the child does not adequately achieve for age or state standards in one of the following eight academic areas: oral expression, listening comprehension, written expression, basic reading skills, reading fluency skills, reading comprehension, mathematics calculation, or mathematics problem solving. The DSM-IV-TR indicates diagnosis of specific disorders of reading, math, and written expression based on lower than expected performance on standardized tests “given the person’s chronological age, measured intelligence, and age-appropriate education.” The proposed changes for DSM-5 include defining these disorders based on low performance on standardized measures without requiring a significant discrepancy between measured intelligence and achievement.

Type
Chapter
Information
Executive Function and Dysfunction
Identification, Assessment and Treatment
, pp. 131 - 140
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Fuchs, DCompton, DLFuchs, LSBryant, JDavis, GN.Making “secondary intervention” work in a three-tier responsiveness-to-intervention model: findings from the first-grade longitudinal reading study of the National Research Center on Learning DisabilitiesRead Writ 2008 21 413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMaster, KLFuchs, DFuchs, LSCompton, DL.Responding to nonresponders: an experimental field trial of identification and intervention methodsExcept Child 2005 4 445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, ESHumphrey, MMellard, DFWoods, KSwanson, HL.Cognitive processing deficits and students with specific learning disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literatureLearn Disab Quart 2010 33 3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kavale, KA.Identifying specific learning disability: Is responsiveness to intervention the answer?J Learn Disab 2005 38 553CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, LSFuchs, D.Mathematical problem-solving profiles of students with mathematics disabilities with and without comorbid reading disabilitiesJ Learn Disab 2002 35 564CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katusic, SKColligan, RCWeaver, ALBarbaresi, WJ.The forgotten learning disability: Epidemiology of written-language disorder in a population-based birth cohort (1976–1982), Rochester, MinnesotaPediatrics 2009 123 1306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Data Accountability Centerhttp://www.ideadata.org/PartBChildCount.asp 2010
Identification of specific learning disabilities 2006 http://idea.ed.gov/object/fileDownload/model/TopicalBrief/field/PdfFile/primary_key/23
American Psychiatric AssociationDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental DisordersWashington, DCAmerican Psychiatric Publ 2000Google Scholar
Fletcher, JMFrancis, DJRourke, BPShaywitz, SEShaywitz, BA.The validity of discrepancy-based definitions of reading disabilitiesJ Learn Disab 1992 25 555CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vellutino, FRScanlon, DMLyon, GR.Differentiating between difficult-to-remediate and readily remediated poor readers: More evidence against the IQ-achievement discrepancy definition of reading disabilityJ Learn Disab 2000 33 223CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, LS.IQ is irrelevant to the definition of learning disabilitiesJ Learn Disab 1989 22 469CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuebing, KFletcher, JMLeDoux, JMLyon, GRShaywitz, SEShaywitz, BA.Validity of IQ-discrepancy classification of reading difficulties: a meta-analysisAm Educ Res J 2002 39 469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reschly, DJHosp, JL.State SLD identification policies and practicesLearn Disabi Quart 2004 27 197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, JNBoyle, JMEKelly, SW.Do tasks make a difference? Accounting for heterogeneity of performance of children with reading difficulties on tasks of executive function: Findings from a meta-analysisBr J Dev Psychol 2010 28 133CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, JJCutting, LERyan, MZilioli, MDenckla, MMahone, EM.Response variability in rapid automatized naming predicts reading comprehensionJ Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2009 7 877CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locascio, GMahone, EMEason, SHCutting, LE.Executive dysfunction among children with reading comprehension deficitsJ Learn Disabil 2010 43 441CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutting, LEMaterek, ACole, CASLevine, TMMahone, EM.Effects of fluency, oral language, and executive function on reading comprehension performanceAnn Dyslexia 2009 59 34CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sesma, HWMahone, EMLevine, TEason, SHCutting, LE.The contribution of executive skills to reading comprehensionChild Neuropsychol 2009 15 232CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, HL.Age-related differences in learning disabled and skilled readers’ working memoryJ Exp Child Psychol 2003 85 1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, HLSachse-Lee, C.Mathematical problem solving and working memory in children with learning disabilities: Both executive and phonological processes are importantJ Exp Child Psychol 2001 79 294CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clair-Thompson, HLGathercole, SE.Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memoryQuart J Exp Psychol 2006 59 745CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vellutino, FRFletcher, JMSnowling, MJScanlon, DM.Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades?J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004 45 2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, SEAlloway, TPWillis, CAdams, A-M.Working memory in children with reading disabilitiesJ Exp Child Psychol 2006 93 265CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menghini, DFinzi, ABenassi, RDifferent underlying neurocognitive deficits in developmental dyslexia: a comparative studyNeuropsychologia 2010 48 863CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borella, ECarretti, BPelegrina, S.The specific role of inhibition in reading comprehension in good and poor comprehendersJ Learn Disab 2010 43 541CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, CACPritchard, VEWoodward, LJ.Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievementDev Psychol 2010 46 1176CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bull, REspy, KAWiebe, SA.Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 yearsDev Neuropsychol 2008 33 205CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bull, RScerif, G.Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memoryDev Neuropsychol 2001 19 273CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Amico, ADGuarnera, M.Exploring working memory in children with low arithmetical achievementLearn Individ Differ 2005 15 189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Passolunghi, MCPazzaglia, F.A comparison of updating processes in children good or poor in arithmetic word problem-solvingLearn Individ Differ 2005 15 257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, LSGeary, DCCompton, DLFuchs, DHamlett, CLBryant, JD.The contributions of numerosity and domain-general abilities to school readinessChild Dev 2010 81 1520CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, LSHamlett, CLFuchs, D.Curriculum-based math computation and concepts/applicationsVanderbilt UniversityNashville, TN37203 1990Google Scholar
Fuchs, LSCompton, DLFuchs, DPaulsen, KBryant, JDHamlett, CL.The prevention, identification, and cognitive determinants of math difficultyJ Ed Psychol 2005 97 493CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, LSFuchs, D.Mathematical problem-solving profiles of students with mathematics disabilities with and without comorbid reading disabilitiesJ Learn Disab 2002 35 564CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, LSHamlett, CLPowell, SR.Grade 3 Math BatteryVanderbilt UniversityNashville, TN37203 2003Google Scholar
Seethaler, PMFuchs, LS.The cognitive correlates of computational estimation skill among third-grade studentsLearn Disab Res Pract 2006 21 233CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersson, U.The contribution of working memory to children’s mathematical word problem solvingAppl Cogn Psychol 2007 21 1201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altemeier, LJones, JAbbott, RDBerninger, VW.Executive functions in becoming writing readers and reading writers: note taking and report writing in third and fifth gradersDev Neuropsychol 2006 29 161CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berninger, VWAbbott, RDJones, JEarly development of language by hand: Composing, reading, listening, and speaking connections; three-letter writing modes; and fast mapping in spellingDev Neuropsychol 2006 29 61CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altemeier, LEAbbott, RDBerninger, VW.Executive functions for reading and writing in typical literacy development and dyslexiaJ Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2008 30 588CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooper, SRSwartz, CWWakely, MBde Kruif, RELMontgomery, JW.Executive functions in elementary school children with and without problems in written expressionJ Learn Disabil 2002 35 57CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourke, LAdams, AM.The relationship between working memory and early writing assessed at the word, sentence and text levelEduc Child Psychol 2003 20 19Google Scholar
Gathercole, SEBrown, LPickering, SJ.Working memory assessments at school entry as longitudinal predictors of national curriculum attainment levelsEduc and Child Psychol 2003 20 109Google Scholar
Houdé, ORossi, SLubin, AJoliot, M.Mapping numerical processing, reading, and executive functions in the brain: an fMRI meta-analysis of 52 studies including 842 childrenDev Sci 2010 13 876CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dove, APollmann, SSchubert, TWiggnes, CJvon Cramon, DY.Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI studyCogn Brain Res 2000 9 103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menon, VUddin, LQ.Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula functionBrain Struct Funct 2010 214 655CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maisog, JMEinbinder, ERFlowers, DLTurkeltaub, PEEden, GF.A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of dyslexiaAnn NY Acad Sci 2008 1145 237CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, RCSambataro, FLohr, CSteinbrink, CMartin, CVasic, N.Functional brain network abnormalities during verbal working memory performance in adolescents and young adults with dyslexiaNeuropsychologia 2010 48 309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dumontheil, IKlingberg, T.Brain activity during a visuospatial working memory task predicts arithmetical performance 2 years laterCereb Cortex 2011Google ScholarPubMed
Richards, TBerninger, VWinn, WDifferences in fMRI activation between children with and without spelling disability on 2-back/0-back working memory contrastJ Writing Res 1 93CrossRef
Meyler, AKeller, TACherkassky, VLGabrieli, JDEJust, MA.Modifying the brain activation of poor readers during sentence comprehension with extended remedial instruction: A longitudinal study of neuroplasticityNeuropsychologia 2008 46 2580CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chein, JMSchneider, W.Neuroimaging studies of practice-related change: fMRI and meta-analytic evidence of a domain-general control network for learningCogn Brain Res 2005 25 607CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pugh, KRFrost, SJSandak, REffects of stimulus activity and repetition on printed work identification: An fMRI comparison of nonimpaired and reading-disabled adolescent cohortsJ Cog Neurosci 2008 20 1146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrison, ABChein, JM.Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by working memoryPsychoneurol Bull Rev 2011 18 46CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chein, JMMorrison, AB.Expanding the mind’s workspace: Training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span taskPsychoneurol Bull Rev 2010 17 193CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, JDunning, DGathercole, SE.Can working memory training improve children’s reading skills? 2011 St. Pete BeachFloridaGoogle Scholar
Van der Molen, MJVan Luit, JEHVan der Molen, MWKlugkist, IJongmans, MJ.Effectiveness of a computerised working memory training in adolescents with mild to borderline intellectual disabilitiesJ Intell Disab Res 2010 54 433CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, JGathercole, SEDunning, DL.Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in childrenDev Science 2009 12 F9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clair-Thompson, HLStevens, RHunt, ABolder, E.Improving children’s working memory and classroom performanceEduc Psychol 2010 30 203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, SHarris, KROlinghouse, N.Addressing executive function problems in writing: an example from the self-regulated strategy development modelLynn, MeltzerExecutive Function in Education from Theory to PracticeNYThe Guilford Press 2007Google Scholar
Harris, KRGraham, SMason, LHFriedlander, B.Powerful Writing Strategies for All StudentsBaltimorePaul H Brookes Publishing Co 2008Google Scholar
Best, JRMiller, PHJones, LL.Executive functions after age 5: changes and correlatesDev Rev 2009 29 180CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×