Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T06:41:18.126Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Hermit Crabs – Information Gathering by the Hermit Crab, Pagurus bernhardus

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 July 2018

Nereida Bueno-Guerra
Affiliation:
Comillas Pontifical University
Federica Amici
Affiliation:
Universität Leipzig
Get access

Summary

hermit crab
Type
Chapter
Information
Field and Laboratory Methods in Animal Cognition
A Comparative Guide
, pp. 222 - 243
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Balshine, S., Verma, A., Chant, V., and Theysmeyer, T. (2005). Competitive interactions between round gobies and logperch. Journal of Great Lakes Research, 31, 6877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belanger, R. M., and Corkum, L. D. (2003). Susceptibility of tethered round gobies (Neogobius melanostomus) to predation in habitats with and without shelters. Journal of Great Lakes Research, 29, 588593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kornis, M. S., Mercado-Silva, N., and Vander Zanden, M. J. (2012). Twenty years of invasion: a review of round goby Neogobius melanostomus biology, spread and ecological implications. Journal of Fish Biology, 80, 235285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCallum, E. S., Gulas, S. T., and Balshine, S. (2017). Accurate resource assessment requires experience in a territorial fish. Animal Behaviour, 123, 249257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Bates, K. M., and Laidre, M. E. (2018). When to socialize: perception of time-sensitive social structures among social hermit crabs. Animal Behaviour, 138, 1927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bracken-Grissom, H. D., Cannon, M. E., Cabezas, P., et al. (2013). A comprehensive and integrative reconstruction of evolutionary history for Anomura (Crustacea: Decapoda). BMC Evolutionary Biology, 13, 128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laidre, M. E. (2010). How rugged individualists enable one another to find food and shelter: field experiments with tropical hermit crabs. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 277, 13611369.Google ScholarPubMed
Laidre, M. E. (2012a). Homes for hermits: temporal, spatial and structural dynamics as transportable homes are incorporated into a population. Journal of Zoology, 288, 3340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laidre, M. E. (2012b). Niche construction drives social dependence in hermit crabs. Current Biology, 22, 861863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laidre, M. E. (2013a). Foraging across ecosystems: diet diversity and social foraging spanning aquatic and terrestrial ecosystems by an invertebrate. Marine Ecology, 34, 8089.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laidre, M. E. (2013b). Eavesdropping foragers use level of collective commotion as public information to target high quality patches. Oikos, 122, 15051511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laidre, M. E. (2014). The social lives of hermits. Natural History, 122, 2429.Google Scholar
Laidre, M. E. (2017). Ruler of the atoll. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment, 15, 527528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laidre, M. E. (2018). Coconut crabs. Current Biology, 28, R58R60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laidre, M. E., and Trinh, R. (2014). Unlike terrestrial hermit crabs, marine hermit crabs do not prefer shells previously used by conspecifics. Crustaceana, 87, 856865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laidre, M. E., and Vermeij, G. J. (2012). A biodiverse housing market in hermit crabs: proposal for a new biodiversity index. Cuadernos de Investigación UNED, 4, 175179.Google Scholar
Laidre, M. E., Patten, E., and Pruitt, L. (2012). Costs of a more spacious home after remodelling by hermit crabs. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 9, 35743577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noë, R., von Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., and Hammerstein, P. (2001). Economics in nature: social dilemmas, mate choice and biological markets. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Cognition, evolution, and behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

References

Appel, M., and Elwood, R. W. (2009). Gender differences, responsiveness and memory of a potentially painful event in hermit crabs. Animal Behaviour, 78, 13731379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnott, G., and Elwood, R. W. (2007). Fighting for shells: how private information about resource value changes hermit crab pre-fight displays and escalated fight behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 274, 30113017.Google ScholarPubMed
Bertness, M. D. (1981). Conflicting advantages in resource utilization: the hermit crab housing dilemma. The American Naturalist, 118(3), 432437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bracken-Grissom, H. D., Cannon, M. E., Cabezas, P., et al. (2013). A comprehensive and integrative reconstruction of evolutionary history for Anomura (Crustacea: Decapoda). BMC Evolutionary Biology, 13, 128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridge, A., Elwood, R. W., and Dick, J. T. A. (2000). Imperfect assessment and limited information preclude optimal strategies in male:male fights in the orb-web spider, Metellina mengei. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 267, 273280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briffa, M., and Elwood, R. W. (2000a). The power of rapping influences eviction during hermit crab shell fights. Behavioural Ecology, 11, 288293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briffa, M., and Elwood, R. W. (2000b). Cumulative or sequential assessment during hermit crab shell fights: effects of oxygen on decision rules. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 267, 24452452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briffa, M., and Elwood, R. W. (2001). Decision rules, energy metabolism and vigour in hermit crab fights. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 268, 18411847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briffa, M., and Elwood, R. W. (2002). Power of signals influences physiological costs and subsequent decisions during hermit crab fights. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 269, 23312336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briffa, M., and Elwood, R. W. (2005). Rapid change in energetic status in fighting animals: causes and effects of strategic decisions. Animal Behaviour, 70, 119124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briffa, M., Elwood, R. W., and Dick, J. T. A. (1998). Analyses of repeated signals during hermit crab shell fights. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 265, 14671474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chase, I. D., Weissburg, M., and Dewitt, T. H. (1988). The vacancy chain process: a new mechanism of resource distribution in animals with application to hermit crabs. Animal Behaviour, 36, 12651274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Contreras-Garduño, J., and Córdoba-Aguilar, A. (2006). Sexual selection in hermit crabs: a review and outlines of future research. Journal of Zoology, 270, 595605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Culshaw, A. D., and Broom, D. M. (1980). The imminence of behavioural change and the startle response. Behaviour, 73, 6476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dick, J. T. A., and Elwood, R. W. (1989). Assessments and decisions during mate choice in Gammarus pulex (Amphipoda). Behaviour, 109, 235246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doake, S., and Elwood, R. W. (2011). How resource quality differentially affects motivation and ability to fight in hermit crabs. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 278, 567573.Google ScholarPubMed
Doake, S., Scantlebury, M., and Elwood, R. W. (2010). The cost of bearing arms and armour in the hermit crab, Pagurus bernhardus. Animal Behaviour, 80, 637642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dowds, B. M., and Elwood, R. W. (1983). Shell wars: assessment strategies and the timing of decisions in hermit crab fights. Behaviour, 85, 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edmonds, E., and Briffa, M. (2017). Weak rappers rock more: hermit crabs assess their own agonistic behaviour. Biology Letters, 12, 20150884.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, R. W. (1995). Motivational change during resource assessment in hermit crabs. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 193, 4155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, R. W., and Adams, P. M. (1990). How hermit crabs (Pagurus bernhardus) deal with obstructions in the apertures of shells. Irish Naturalists’ Journal, 23, 180185.Google Scholar
Elwood, R. W., and Appel, M. (2009). Pain in hermit crabs? Animal Behaviour, 77, 12431246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, R. W., and Arnott, G. (2012). Understanding how animals fight with Lloyd Morgan’s canon. Animal Behaviour, 84, 10951102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, R. W., and Briffa, M. (2001). Information gathering during agonistic and non-agonistic shell aquistion by hermit crabs. Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 30, 5397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, R. W., and Glass, C. W. (1981). Negotiation or aggression during shell fights of the hermit crab, Pagurus bernhardus. Animal Behaviour, 29, 12391244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, R. W., and Kennedy, H. F. (1988). Sex differences in shell preferences in the hermit crab, Pagurus bernhardus L. Irish Naturalists’ Journal, 22, 436440.Google Scholar
Elwood, R. W., and Neil, S. J. (1992). Assessments and decisions: a study of information gathering by hermit crabs. London: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar
Elwood, R. W., and Stewart, A. (1985). The timing of decisions during shell investigation by the hermit crab, Pagurus bernhardus. Animal Behaviour, 33, 620627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, R. W., and Stewart, A. (1987). Reproduction in the littoral hermit crab, Pagurus bernhardus. Irish Naturalists’ Journal, 22, 252255.Google Scholar
Elwood, R. W., McClean, A., and Webb, L. (1979). The development of shell preferences by the hermit crab, Pagurus bernhardus. Animal Behaviour, 27, 940946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, R.W., Wood, K., Gallagher, M., and Dick, J. T. A. (1998). Probing motivational state during agonistic encounters in animals. Nature, 393, 6668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hazlett, B. A. (1978). Shell exchanges in hermit crabs: aggression, negotiation, or both? Animal Behaviour, 26, 12781279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herreid, C. F., and Full, R. J. (1986). Energetics of hermit crabs during locomotion: the cost of carrying a shell. Journal of Experimental Biology, 120, 297308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imafuku, M. (1994). Response of hermit crabs to sinistral shells. Journal of Ethology, 12, 107114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, N. W., and Elwood, R. W. (1989a). How animals make assessments: information gathering by the hermit crab, Pagurus bernhardus. Animal Behaviour, 38, 951957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, N. W., and Elwood, R. W. (1989b). Memory of shells in the hermit crab, Pagurus bernhardus. Animal Behaviour, 37, 529534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, N. W., and Elwood, R. W. (1990). Interrupting an assessment process to probe changes in the motivational state. Animal Behaviour, 39, 10681077.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinosita, H., and Okajima, A. (1968). Analysis of shell-searching behaviour of the land hermit crab, Coenobita rugosus H. Milne Edwards. Journal of the Faculty of Science, University of Tokyo, Section IV, 11, 293358.Google Scholar
Laidre, M. E. (2007). Vulnerability and reliable signaling in conflicts between hermit crabs. Behavioural Ecology, 18, 736741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancaster, I. (1990). Reproduction and life history strategy of the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus. Journal of the Marine Biological Association UK, 70, 129142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magee, B., and Elwood, R. W. (2016). Trade-offs between predator avoidance and electric shock avoidance in hermit crabs demonstrate a non-reflexive response to noxious stimuli consistent with prediction of pain. Behavioural Processes, 130, 3135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesce, K. A. (1982). Calcium-bearing objects elicit shell selection behaviour in a hermit crab. Science, 215, 993995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mowles, S. L., Cotton, P. A., and Briffa, M. (2009). Aerobic capacity influences giving-up decisions in fighting hermit crabs: does stamina constrain contests? Animal Behaviour, 78, 735740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neil, S. J., and Elwood, R. W. (1986). Factors influencing shell selection in the hermit crab, Pagurus bernhardus. Ethology, 73, 225234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pechenik, J. A., Diederich, C. M., Burns, R., Pancheri, F. Q., and Dorfmann, L. (2015). Influence of the commensal gastropod Crepidula plana on shell choice by the marine hermit crab Pagurus longicarpus, with an assessment of the degree of stress caused by different eviction techniques. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 469, 1826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pike, R. B., and Williamson, D. I. (1959). Observations on the distribution and breeding of British hermit crabs and the stone crab (Crustacea: Diogenidae, Paguridae and Lithodiddae). Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London, 132, 551567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ping, X., Lee, J. S., Garlick, D., Jiang, Z., and Blaisdell, A. P. (2015). Behavioral evidence illuminating the visual abilities of the terrestrial Caribbean hermit crab Coenobita clypeatus. Behavioural Processes, 118, 4758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prenter, J., Elwood, R. W., and Montgomery, W. I. (1994). Assessments and decisions in mate guarding in Metellina segmentata (Areneae: Metidae). Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 35, 3943.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E. S. (1963). The behavioural mechanisms underlying shell selection by hermit crabs. Behaviour, 21, 78126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ritschoff, D. (1980). Chemical attraction of hermit crabs and other attendants to simulated gastropod predation sites. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 6, 103118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, L., Cheesman, S., Elliott, M., and Breithaupt, T. (2016). Sensitivity of Pagurus bernhardus (L.) to substrate-borne vibration and anthropogenic noise. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 474, 185194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonoda, K., Asakura, A., Minoura, M., Elwood, R. W., and Gunji, P. (2012). Hermit crabs perceive the extent of their virtual bodies. Biology Letters, 8, 495497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J. D., and McDermott, J. J. (2004). Hermit crab biocoenoses: a worldwide review of the diversity and natural history of hermit crab associates. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 305, 1128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yasuda, C., Takeshita, F., and Wada, S. (2012). Assessment strategy in male-male contests of the hermit crab Pagurus middendorffi. Animal Behaviour, 84, 385390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×