Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-tj2md Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T03:49:58.527Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part III - On the mechanics of affective social learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 August 2019

Daniel Dukes
Affiliation:
Université de Fribourg, Switzerland
Fabrice Clément
Affiliation:
Université de Neuchâtel, Switzerland
Get access

Summary

Interpersonally presented emotions help to calibrate people’s orientations to things happening in the shared environment. For example, social referencing involves one person seeking clarification of the appropriate appraisal of an object, event, or person, and another person responding with an emotional orientation that disambiguates things. However, this paradigmatic case represents only one of the possible ways in which emotions affect other people’s physical or mental attitudes. In other cases, emotion-related responses affect other people’s orientations independent of their explicit informational content. Further, emotional knowledge may be co-constructed dynamically rather than transmitted unidirectionally from one person to another. In these cases, affective social learning need not involve changes in the perceived meaning of emotional objects, but rather adjustments in interactants’ orientations to what is happening. This chapter suggests ways of extending and going beyond existing methodological and theoretical approaches to emotional influence and identifies some of the blindspots of previous research.

Type
Chapter
Information
Foundations of Affective Social Learning
Conceptualizing the Social Transmission of Value
, pp. 115 - 184
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion, 5, 311.Google Scholar
Bartel, C., & Saavedra, R. (2000). The collective construction of work group moods. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45, 197231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayliss, A. P., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Affective evaluations of objects are influenced by observed gaze direction and emotion expression. Cognition, 104, 644653.Google Scholar
Bruder, M., Dosmukhambetova, D., Nerb, J., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2012). Emotional signals in nonverbal interaction: Dyadic facilitation and convergence in expressions, appraisals, and feelings. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 480502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buck, R., Savin, V. J., Miller, R. E., & Caul, W. F. (1972). Communication of affect through facial expressions in humans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 362371.Google Scholar
Butler, E. A., & Randall, A. K. (2013). Emotional coregulation in close relationships. Emotion Review, 5, 202210.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J., & Stenberg, C. (1981). Perception, appraisal, and emotion: The onset of social referencing. In Lamb, M. E. & Sherrod, L. R. (Eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations (pp. 273314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893910.Google Scholar
Clément, F., & Dukes, D. (2017). Social appraisal and social referencing: Two components of affective social learning. Emotion Review, 9, 253261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Melo, C. M., Carnevale, P. J., Read, S. J., & Gratch, J. (2014). Reading people’s minds from emotion expressions in interdependent decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 7388.Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A. (2007). Emotion in organizations: A review and theoretical integration. Academy of Management Annals, 1, 371457.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (2005). Emotion experience. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 473497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H., & Philipszoon, E. (1963). Dimensions of recognition of emotion. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 4551.Google Scholar
Gendron, M., Crivelli, C., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Universality reconsidered: Diversity in meaning making about facial expressions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 211219.Google Scholar
Hareli, S. (2014). Making sense of the social world and influencing it by using a naïve attribution theory of emotions. Emotion Review, 6, 336343.Google Scholar
Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2010). What emotional reactions can tell us about the nature of others: An appraisal perspective on person perception. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 128140.Google Scholar
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 142157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2018). On the malleability of the meaning of contexts: The influence of another person’s emotion expressions on situation perception. Cognition and Emotion, 32, 185191.Google Scholar
Kalokerinos, E. K., Tamir, M., & Kuppens, P. (2017). Instrumental motives in negative emotion regulation in daily life: Frequency, consistency, and predictors. Emotion, 17, 648657.Google Scholar
Kokkinaki, T. S., Vasdekis, V. G. S., Koufaki, Z. E., & Trevarthen, C. B. (2017). Coordination of emotions in mother-infant dialogues. Infant and Child Development, 26, e1973.Google Scholar
Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1968). Group inhibition of bystander intervention in emergencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 215221.Google Scholar
Latané, B., & Wolf, S. (1981). The social impact of majorities and minorities. Psychological Review, 88, 438453.Google Scholar
Leach, C. W., Spears, R., Branscombe, N. R., & Doosje, B. (2003). Malicious pleasure: Schadenfreude at the suffering of an outgroup. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 932943.Google Scholar
Lee, D. H., Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Social transmission of the sensory benefits of eye widening in fear expression. Psychological Science, 24, 957965.Google Scholar
Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., … Brown, R. A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 7584.Google Scholar
Leventhal, H., & Scherer, K. R. (1987). The relationship of emotion and cognition: A functional approach to a semantic controversy. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2001). Social appraisal: The social world as object of and influence on appraisal processes. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 221232). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrath, J. E., & Kelly, J. R. (1986). Time and human interaction: Toward a social psychology of time. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mumenthaler, C., & Sander, D. (2012). Social appraisal influences recognition of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 11181135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mumenthaler, C., (2015). Automatic integration of social information in emotion recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 392399.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (1996). Emotions are social. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 663683.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2011). Interpersonal emotion transfer: Contagion and social appraisal. Personality and Social Psychology Compass, 5, 428439.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2013). Contextualizing facial activity. Emotion Review, 5, 97103.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2017). Comment: Respecifying emotional influence. Emotion Review, 9, 263265.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1993). Making sense of emotion in stories and social life. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 295323.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., Phiri, N., & Simons, G. (2012). Bursting with anxiety: Adult social referencing in an interpersonal Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Emotion, 12, 817826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parkinson, B., Shore, D., & Stephens, N. (unpublished). Expression regulation and perceived regulation and their interpersonal effects on risk behaviour in a dyadic Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART).Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Simons, G. (2009). Affecting others: Social appraisal and emotion contagion in everyday decision-making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 10711084.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., (2012). Worry spreads: Interpersonal transfer of problem-related anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 462479.Google Scholar
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Reddy, V. (2000). Coyness in early infancy. Developmental Science, 3, 186192.Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Kaiser, S., Wehrle, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2007). Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 470480.Google Scholar
Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., Mortillaro, M., Rotondi, I., Sergi, I., & Trznadel, S. (2018). Appraisal-driven facial actions as building blocks for emotion inference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114, 358379.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 233269.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Scott, H. S. (1997). A componential approach to the meaning of facial expressions. In Russell, J. A. & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 229254). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. N., Hofer, L., Haft, W., & Dore, J. (1985). Affect attunement: The sharing of feeling states between mother and infant by means of intermodal fluency. In Field, T. N. & Fox, N. (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 249268). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Totterdell, P. (2000). Catching moods and hitting runs: Mood linkage and subjective performance in professional sport teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 848859.Google Scholar
Totterdell, P., Kellett, S., Teuchmann, K., & Briner, R. B. (1998). Evidence of mood linkage in work groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 15041515.Google Scholar
van Doorn, E. A., van Kleef, G. A., & van der Pligt, J. (2015). Deriving meaning from others’ emotions: Attribution, appraisal, and the use of emotions as social information. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1077.Google Scholar
van Kleef, G. A. (2009). How emotions regulate social life: The Emotions as Social Information (EASI) model. Current Directions in Psychological Research, 18, 184188.Google Scholar
Walle, E. A., Reschke, P. J., Camras, L. A., & Campos, J. J. (2017). Infant differential behaviour responding to discrete emotions. Emotion, 17, 10781091.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., Russell, D., & Lerman, D. (1979). The cognition-emotion process in achievement-related contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 12111220.Google Scholar

References

Adams, R. B., Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Perceived gaze direction and the processing of facial displays of emotion. Psychological Science, 14, 644647.Google Scholar
Adams, R. B., (2005). Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facial communicated emotion. Emotion, 5, 311.Google Scholar
Aviezer, H., Bentin, S., Dudarev, V., & Hassin, R. R. (2011). The automaticity of emotional face–context integration. Emotion, 11, 14061414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aviezer, H., Hassin, R. R., Bentin, S., & Trope, Y. (2008). Putting facial expressions back in context. In Ambady, N. & Skowronsky, J. (Eds.), First impressions (pp. 255286). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Aviezer, H., Hassin, R., Ryan, J., Grady, C., Susskind, J., Anderson, A., … Bentin, S. (2008). Angry, disgusted, or afraid? Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychological Science, 19, 724732.Google Scholar
Aviezer, H., Trope, Y., & Todorov, A. (2012). Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions. Science, 338(6111), 12251229.Google Scholar
Bayliss, A. P., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Affective evaluations of objects are influenced by observed gaze direction and emotional expression. Cognition, 104, 644653.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., & Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 286290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bodenhausen, G. V., & Todd, A. R. (2010). Social cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(2), 160171. doi:10.1002/wcs.28Google Scholar
Bruder, M., Fischer, A., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2014). Social appraisal as a cause of collective emotions. In von Scheve, C. & Salmela, M. (Eds.), Collective emotions (pp. 141155). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clément, F., Bernard, S., Grandjean, D., & Sander, D. (2013). Emotional expression and vocabulary learning in adults and children. Cognition and Emotion, 27(3), 539548Google Scholar
Clément, F., & Dukes, D. (2013). The role of interest in the social transmission of values. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 349.Google Scholar
Adams, R. B., (2017). Social appraisal and social referencing: Two components of affective social learning. Emotion Review, 9(3), 253261.Google Scholar
Cristinzio, C., N’Diaye, K., Seeck, M., Vuilleumier, P., & Sander, D. (2010). Integration of gaze direction and facial expression in patients with unilateral amygdala damage. Brain, 133, 248261.Google Scholar
Cline, M. G. (1956). The influence of social context on the perception of faces. Journal of Personality, 25(2), 142157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daw, N. D., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2014). Multiple systems for value learning. In Glimcher, P. W. & Fehr, E (Eds.), Neuroeconomics (2nd ed., pp. 393410). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
de Gelder, B., Meeren, H. K. M., Righart, R., Stock, J., van de Riet, W. A. C., & Tamietto, M. (2006). Beyond the face: Exploring rapid influences of context on face processing. Progress in Brain Research, 155, 3748.Google Scholar
de Gelder, B., & van den Stock, J. (2012). Real faces, real emotions: Perceiving facial expressions in naturalistic contexts of voices, bodies and scenes. In Calder, A. J., Rhodes, G., Haxby, J. V., & Johnson, M. H. (Eds.), The handbook of face perception. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
de Melo, C. M., Carnevale, P. J., Read, S. J., & Gratch, J. (2014). Reading people’s minds from emotion expressions in interdependent decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(1), 7388.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In Cole, J. K. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 207283). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression of emotion. American Psychologist, 48, 384392.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Freisen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face. New York, NY: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Evers, C., Fischer, A., Mosquera, P., & Manstead, A. (2005). Anger and social appraisal: A “spicy” sex difference? Emotion, 5, 258266.Google Scholar
Feinman, S. (1992). In the broad valley: An integrative look at social referencing. In Feinman, S. (Ed.), Social referencing and the social construction of reality in infancy (pp. 1554). New York, NY: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117140.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. H., Rotteveel, M., Evers, C., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004). Emotional assimilation: How we are influenced by others’ emotions. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition, 22, 223245.Google Scholar
Gendron, M., Mesquita, B., & Barrett, L. F. (2013). Emotion perception: Putting the face in context. In Reisberg, D. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology (pp. 379389). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Unpacking the cognitive architecture of emotion processes. Emotion, 8, 341351. doi:10.1037/1528- 3542.8.3.341Google Scholar
Haaker, J., Golkar, A., Selbing, I., & Olsson, A (2017). Assessment of social transmission of threats in humans using observational fear conditioning. Nature Protocols, 12(7), 13781386.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 852870). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hassin, R. R. (2013). Yes it can: On the functional abilities of the human unconscious. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 195207.Google Scholar
Hassin, R. R., Aviezer, H., & Benton, S. (2013). Inherently ambiguous: Facial expressions of emotions, in context. Emotion Review, 5, 6065.Google Scholar
Hassin, R. R., Bargh, J. A., & Uleman, J. S. (2002). Spontaneous causal inferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 515522.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Jakobs, E., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1997). Emotional experience as a function of social context: The role of the other. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21, 103130.Google Scholar
Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., Burriss, R. P., & Feinberg, D. R. (2007). Social transmission of face preferences among humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 274, 899903.Google Scholar
Klinnert, M. D., Campos, J. J., Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., & Svejda, M. (1983). Emotions as behavior regulators. In Plutchik, R. & Kellerman, H. (Eds.), Emotions: Theory, research, and experience (Vol. 2, pp. 5786). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
LaBar, K., & Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(1), 5464.Google Scholar
Landis, C. (1929). The interpretation of facial expression in emotion. Journal of General Psychology, 2(1), 5972.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 259289.Google Scholar
Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2001). Social appraisal: The social world as object of and influence on appraisal processes. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, method, research (pp. 221232). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Masuda, T., Ellsworth, P. C., Mesquita, B., Leu, J., Tanida, S., & de Veerdonk, E. V. (2008). Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 365381.Google Scholar
Moors, A. (2017). Integration of two skeptical emotion theories: Dimensional appraisal theory and Russell’s psychological construction theory. Psychological Inquiry, 28(1), 119.Google Scholar
Mudrik, L., Faivre, N., & Koch, C. (2014). Information integration without awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 488496.Google Scholar
Mumenthaler, C., & Sander, D. (2012). Social appraisal influences recognition of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 11181135.Google Scholar
Mumenthaler, C., (2015). Automatic integration of social information in emotion recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 392399.Google Scholar
Mumenthaler, C., Sander, D., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2018). Emotion recognition in simulated social interactions. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing. https://doi.org/10.1109/TAFFC.2018.2799593Google Scholar
Olkkonen, M., & Ekroll, V. (2016) Color constancy and contextual effects on color appearance. In Kremers, J., Baraas, R, & Marshall, N. (Eds.), Human color vision (Springer Series in Vision Research, Vol. 5, pp. 221232). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.Google Scholar
Olsson, A., Nearing, K. I., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Learning fears by observing others: The neural systems of social fear transmission. Social, Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 2, 210.Google Scholar
Olsson, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2004). Learned fear of unseen faces after Pavlovian, observational and instructed fear. Psychological Science, 15, 822828.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., Phiri, N., & Simons, G. (2012). Bursting with anxiety: Adult social referencing in an interpersonal Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Emotion, 12(4), 817826. doi:10.1037/a0026434Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Simons, G. (2009). Worry spreads: Interpersonal transfer of problem-related anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 26(3), 462479.Google Scholar
Pool, E. R., Brosch, T., Delplanque, S., & Sander, D. (2016). Attentional bias for positive emotional stimuli: A meta-analytic investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 79106.Google Scholar
Righart, R., & de Gelder, B. (2008). Recognition of facial expressions is influenced by emotional scene gist. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 264272.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Fehr, B. (1987). Relativity in the perception of emotion in facial expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116(3), 223237.Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Kaiser, S., Wehrle, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2007). Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 470480.Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2005). A systems approach to appraisal mechanisms in emotion. Neural Networks, 18, 317352.Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2018). An appraisal-driven componential approach to the emotional brain. Emotion Review, 10(3), 219231.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., Mortillaro, M., Rotondi, I., Sergi, I., & Trznadel, S. (2018). Appraisal-driven facial actions as building blocks for emotion inference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(3), 358379.Google Scholar
Sennwald, V., Pool, E., & Sander, D. (2017). Considering the influence of the Pavlovian system on behavior: Appraisal and value representation. Psychological Inquiry, 28(1), 5255.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Scott, H. S. (1997). A componential approach to the meaning of facial expressions. In Russell, J. A., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 229254). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 195.Google Scholar
Stussi, Y., Pourtois, G., & Sander, D. (2018). Enhanced Pavlovian aversive conditioning to positive emotional stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147, 905923.Google Scholar
Tang, Y., Harris, P. L., Zou, H., & Xu, Q. (2019). The impact of emotional expressions on children’s trust judgments. Cognition and Emotion, 33(2), 318331. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2018.1449735Google Scholar
Todorov, A. (2017). Face value: The irresistible influence of first impressions. Princeton, NJ and Oxford, UK: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Trope, Y. (1986). Identification and inferential processes in dispositional attribution. Psychological Review, 93, 239257.Google Scholar
van den Stock, J., Righart, R., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Whole body expressions influence recognition of facial expressions and emotional prosody. Emotion, 7, 487494.Google Scholar
van Doorn, E. A., Heerdink, M. W., & van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Emotion and the construal of social situations: Inferences of cooperation versus competition from expressions of anger, happiness, and disappointment. Cognition & Emotion, 26(3), 442461.Google Scholar
van Reekum, C., Johnstone, T., Banse, R., Etter, A., Wehrle, T., & Scherer, K. (2004). Psychophysiological responses to appraisal dimensions in a computer game. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 663688.Google Scholar
Wehrle, T., Kaiser, S., Schmidt, S., & Scherer, K. R. (2000). Studying the dynamics of emotional expression using synthesized facial muscle movements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 105119.Google Scholar
Wieser, M. J., & Brosch, T. (2012). Faces in context: a review and systematization of contextual influences on affective face processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 471.Google Scholar

References

Aktar, E., Majdandžić, M., de Vente, W., & Bögels, S. M. (2013). The interplay between expressed parental anxiety and infant behavioural inhibition predicts infant avoidance in a social referencing paradigm. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(2), 144156.Google Scholar
Aktar, E., Majdandžic̈, M., de Vente, W., & Bögels, S. M. (2014). Parental social anxiety disorder prospectively predicts toddlers’ fear/avoidance in a social referencing paradigm. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(1), 7787.Google Scholar
Baeyens, F., Crombez, G., van den Bergh, O., & Eelen, P. (1988). Once in contact always in contact: Evaluative conditioning is resistant to extinction. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 10(4), 179199.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. Oxford, UK: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1971). Social Learning Theory. New York, NY: General Learning Press.Google Scholar
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1961). Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63(3), 575582.Google Scholar
Bandura, A., Ross, D., (1963). Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66(1), 3.Google Scholar
Beardslee, W. R., Gladstone, T. R., & O’Connor, E. E. (2011). Transmission and prevention of mood disorders among children of affectively ill parents: A review. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(11), 10981109.Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B. I., & Campos, J. J. (1984). A reexamination of fear and its determinants on the visual cliff. Psychophysiology, 21(4), 413417.Google Scholar
Brody, L. R. (2000). The socialization of gender differences in emotional expression: Display rules, infant temperament, and differentiation. In Fischer, A. H. (Eds.), Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 324). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brody, L. R., & Hall, J. A. (2010). Gender, emotion, and socialization. In Chrisler, J. C. & McCreary, D. R. (Eds.), Handbook of gender research in psychology (pp. 429459). New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Bruder, M., Fischer, A., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2014). Social appraisal as a cause of collective emotions. In von Scheve, C. & Salmela, M. (Eds.), Collective emotions (pp. 141155). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clément, F., & Dukes, D. (2017). Social appraisal and social referencing: Two components of affective social learning. Emotion Review, 9(3), 253261.Google Scholar
Darley, J. M., & Latane, B. (1970). Norms and normative behavior: Field studies of social interdependence. In Macaulay, J. & Berkowitz, L. (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior (pp. 83–102). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
de Houwer, J., Thomas, S., & Baeyens, F. (2001). Associative learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 127(6), 853869.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., & Grout, L. (1993). Socialization of emotion: Pathway to preschoolers’ emotional and social competence. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17(3), 205227.Google Scholar
Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L. (1991). Family talk about feeling states and children’s later understanding of others’ emotions. Developmental Psychology, 27(3), 448455.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241273.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1992). Emotion, regulation, and the development of social competence. In Clark, M. S. (Ed.), Emotion and social behavior: Review of personality and social psychology (pp. 119150). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., (1994). Mothers’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s temperament and anger behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (1982–), 40(1), 138156.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., & Valiente, C. (2004). Elaborations on a theme: Beyond main effects in relations of parenting to children’s coping and regulation. Parenting: Science and Practice, 4(4), 319323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evers, C., Fischer, A. H., Rodriguez Mosquera, P. M., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2005). Anger and social appraisal: A ‘spicy’ sex difference? Emotion, 5(3), 258266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7(2), 117140.Google Scholar
Flack, W. (2006). Peripheral feedback effects of facial expressions, bodily postures, and vocal expressions on emotional feelings. Cognition & Emotion, 20(2), 177195.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. H. (1993). Sex differences in emotionality: Fact or stereotype? Feminism & Psychology, 3(3), 303318.Google Scholar
Fischer, P., Krueger, J. I., Greitemeyer, T., Vogrincic, C., Kastenmüller, A., Frey, D., … Kainbacher, M. (2011). The bystander-effect: A meta-analytic review on bystander intervention in dangerous and non-dangerous emergencies. Psychological Bulletin, 137(4), 517537.Google Scholar
Gerull, F. C., & Rapee, R. M. (2002). Mother knows best: Effects of maternal modelling on the acquisition of fear and avoidance behaviour in toddlers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(3), 279287.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348362.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., Richards, J. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Emotion regulation in everyday life. Regulation, 129, 134.Google Scholar
Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2012). The social signal value of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 26(3), 385389.Google Scholar
Harris, B. (1979). Whatever happened to little Albert? American Psychologist, 34(2), 151160.Google Scholar
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapson, R. (1994). Emotional contagion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2(3), 9699.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Blairy, S. (2001). Facial mimicry and emotional contagion to dynamic emotional facial expressions and their influence on decoding accuracy. International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 40(2), 129141.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17(2), 142157.Google Scholar
Hess, U., (2014). Emotional mimicry: Why and when we mimic emotions. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8, 4557.Google Scholar
Hofmann, W., de Houwer, J., Perugini, M., Baeyens, F., & Crombez, G. (2010). Evaluative conditioning in humans: a meta-analysis. Psychological bulletin, 136(3), 390.Google Scholar
Jones, S., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2002). Parents’ reactions to elementary school children’s negative emotions: Relations to social and emotional functioning at school. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48(2), 133159.Google Scholar
Klinnert, M., Campos, J. J., Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., & Svejda, M. (1983). Emotions as behavior regulators: Social referencing in infancy. In Plutchik, R. & Kellerman, H. (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience (pp. 5786). London, UK: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Klinnert, M. D., Emde, R. N., Butterfield, P., & Campos, J. J. (1986). Social Referencing: The infant’s use of emotional signals from a friendly adult with mother present. Developmental Psychology, 22(4), 427432.Google Scholar
Kret, M. E., Fischer, A. H., & de Dreu, C. K. W. (2015). Pupil mimicry correlates with trust in in-group partners with dilating pupils. Psychological Science, 26(9), 14011410.Google Scholar
Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1970). The unresponsive bystander: Why doesn’t he help? Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Mann, L., Feddes, A. R., Doosje, B., & Fischer, A. H. (2016). Withdraw or affiliate? The role of humiliation during initiation rituals. Cognition and Emotion, 30(1), 80100.Google Scholar
Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2001). Social appraisal: The social world as object of and influence on appraisal processes. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 221232). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Manstead, A. S. R., (2017). Social referencing and social appraisal: Commentary on the Clément and Dukes (2016) and Walle et al. (2016) articles. Emotion Review, 9(3), 262263.Google Scholar
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16, 261288.Google Scholar
Mumenthaler, C., & Sander, D. (2012). Social appraisal influences recognition of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 11181135.Google Scholar
Murray, L., de Rosnay, M., Pearson, J., Bergeron, C., Schofield, E., Royal-Lawson, M., & Cooper, P. J. (2008). Intergenerational transmission of social anxiety: The role of social referencing processes in infancy. Child Development, 79, 10491064.Google Scholar
Nicol-Harper, R., Harvey, A. G., & Stein, A. (2007). Interactions between mothers and infants: Impact of maternal anxiety. Infant Behavior and Development, 30(1), 161167.Google Scholar
Öhman, A., Fredrikson, M., Hugdahl, K., & Rimmo, P. A. (1976). The premise of equipotentiality in human classical conditioning: Conditioned electrodermal responses to potentially phobic stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105(4), 313337.Google Scholar
Olsson, A., Ebert, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). Psychology: The role of social groups in the persistence of learned fear. Psychological Science, 15(12), 822828.Google Scholar
Olsson, A., Nearing, K. I., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Learning fears by observing others: The neural systems of social fear transmission. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(1), 311.Google Scholar
Olsson, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2004). Learned fear of ‘unseen’ faces after pavlovian, observational, and instructed fear. Psychological Science, 15(12), 822828.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (1997). Untangling the appraisal-emotion connection. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1(1), 6279.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2011). Interpersonal emotion transfer: Contagion and social appraisal. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(7), 428439.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., Phiri, N., & Simons, G. (2012). Bursting with anxiety: Adult social referencing in an interpersonal Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Emotion, 12(4), 817826.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Simons, G. (2012). Worry spreads: Interpersonal transfer of problem-related anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 26(3), 462479.Google Scholar
Rimé, B. (2007). The social sharing of emotion as an interface between individual and collective processes in the construction of emotional climates. Journal of Social Issues, 63(2), 307322.Google Scholar
Rohan, M. J. (2000). A rose by any name? The values construct. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(3), 255277.Google Scholar
Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation: Experimental studies of the sources of gregariousness. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Shields, S. A. (2013). Gender and emotion. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37(4), 423435.Google Scholar
Suls, J., & Wheeler, L. (2012). Social comparison theory. In van Lange, P. A. M., Kruglanski, A. W. & Higgings, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology: Volume 1 (pp. 460483). London, UK: Sage.Google Scholar
van Harreveld, F., Nohlen, H. U., & Schneider, I. K. (2015). The ABC of ambivalence: Affective, behavioral, and cognitive consequences of attitudinal conflict. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 285324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Kleef, G. A. (2009). How emotions regulate social life: The emotions as social information (EASI) model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 184188.Google Scholar
Vaughan, K. B., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1980). Vicarious instigation and conditioning of facial expressive and autonomic responses to a model’s expressive display of pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(6), 909923.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B., & Morgan, J. J. B. (1917). Emotional reactions and psychological experimentation. American Journal of Psychology, 28(2), 163174.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3(1), 1.Google Scholar
Weinberg, M. K., & Tronick, E. Z. (1998). The impact of maternal psychiatric illness on infant development. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(2), 53061.Google Scholar
Witherington, D. C., Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Lejeune, L., & Seah, E. (2005). Avoidance of heights on the visual cliff in newly walking infants. Infancy, 7(3), 285298.Google Scholar
Zech, E., & Rimé, B. (2005). Is talking about an emotional experience helpful? Effects on emotional recovery and perceived benefits. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 12(4), 270287.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×