We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Peter returned to Greiz only once, in 1990 with his family, 44 years after he escaped from the Soviet zone with his stepmother and brothers. On this visit to the home of his childhood memories, the Berlin wall had just fallen but the German reunification was yet to come. East Germany – the German Democratic Republic – had existed as a nation since 1949, having transitioned from being a Soviet-occupied zone to statehood, but of course remaining under heavy Soviet influence. Peter’s visit was to an East Germany that was gradually being disbanded. It was still a communist nation, but citizens of the German Democratic Republic could now travel abroad. Through the centralized East German government, Peter had arranged modest accommodations in the old town area of Greiz, where no direct bookings or modern hotels existed at that time.
Peter arrived as a new assistant professor of pediatric neurology at Yale University in 1966. This was in the midst of the Reye’s syndrome peak of the late 1960s. Reye’s syndrome is a life-threatening pediatric disease characterized by progressive brain and liver damage in children, often after a viral illness. It became a rare condition after epidemiological studies suggested a link between aspirin use in children and the onset of Reye’s syndrome after a viral illness. The reduced rates of Reye’s syndrome are largely attributable to public health messaging to limit aspirin use in children, but until that change, it often fell to pediatric neurologists to keep Reye’s syndrome patients alive.
In the later stages of Parkinson’s disease most patients develop progressive cognitive impairment and dementia. During his final year, Peter often spoke in German. He alternated between disorientation and lucidity. During his disoriented periods, he wandered. At night he would scream out in terror. It became clear that he needed round-the-clock care and, after much hesitation, his family all decided that he needed to live in the care facility connected to Peter and Janellen’s retirement community. The iron grip of Parkinson’s disease is difficult for families. It is a confusing kind of dementia, the periods of lucidity raising hopes that are repeatedly dashed. Peter was locked in during those last months. It was not always clear he recognized his friends or family members, but sometimes he made so much sense! “Ich bin müde” (I am tired) or “genug” (enough), he said.
After completing his undergraduate training at the University of Buffalo in three years, Peter started at Harvard Medical School in 1953, four and a half years after arriving in the United States through Ellis Island. For the first time, Peter was really on his own. As a first-year student Peter moved into Vanderbilt Hall, a dormitory for male Harvard medical students. In the 1950s, the Harvard medical class was largely composed of elite highly educated men, many of whom had attended Harvard College or other Ivy League institutions. By living in Vanderbilt, Peter became fully immersed in an entirely new culture of the American elite.
My father, a practicing pediatric neurologist, was also a scientist. As a child, I remember my father peering at hundreds of micrographs of neuronal connections. Billions of these small connections form between neurons in the developing brain. Over the years he counted many, many synapses in post-mortem brain samples. A US immigrant who grew up in wartime Germany, he quietly shouldered his own childhood experiences. And he gained lasting fame – among neuroscientists and others – by discovering something fundamental. Brain synaptic connections increase dramatically during early human development, as anyone might expect. But Peter – Dad – Dr. Huttenlocher – discovered that, by the millions, these connections are also selectively removed as we learn and develop. This process, now referred to as synaptic pruning, is the process of refinement that mediates our skills, our abilities and our memories [1].
Peter’s father, Richard Huttenlocher, was born in 1900 in Alte Weinsteige, in the hilly vineyards near Stuttgart. Richard’s father, Johann Huttenlocher, was a policeman in Stuttgart. Richard’s mother, Elisabethe, “Oma” Huttenlocher (family name Gaupp), was a doting mother and ran a traditional home, with a knack for German baking that she shared with her grandson Peter. Both Richard and his older brother Friedrich (Uncle Fritz) studied the sciences in the Gymnasium (high school), but their studies were interrupted by the First World War. Fritz was stationed at the western front from 1914 to 1918, where he was wounded and was recognized with an Iron Cross from the Prussian government. Richard had only a brief stint as a young soldier, toward the end of the war. After the war, they both returned to live at the family home. Richard studied chemistry at the University of Stuttgart and Fritz studied geography/geology at the University of Tübingen. They both married young. Fritz married his wife Hannah, a traditional woman, in 1920, and continued to work at the University of Tübingen as a lecturer and then professor.
Peter Huttenlocher was unendingly interested. Interested in music. In baking. In medicine. In people and their ways of thinking. In his patients. In the science of the human brain. “Peter is a great dreamer,” said Hanne, Peter’s paternal grandmother, in 1948. Although he delved deeply into philosophy in college, Peter decided early after arriving in the United States that he wanted to become a physician. He wanted to take care of people. So, he immersed himself in human medicine. He memorized. He took part in the multi-year training process for doctors that “knocks every ounce of originality out of you” [1].
As his former National Institutes of Health (NIH) colleague Irwin Feinberg put it, Peter’s research “took a 180-degree turn” after his move to the University of Chicago in 1974. Instead of focusing on the brains of patients with neurological deficits, he started to study the “gridwork” of healthy brains. As Peter said, “the findings in the normal population were more interesting than the abnormal population.” His landmark 1979 study published in Brain Research was unexpected [1]. The accepted thinking at that time was that brains actually get more connections as we learn and develop, but he found the opposite to be true. After a burst of new synapses form in the first year of life, unneeded connections are removed, or pruned. All scientific discoveries are incremental and build on the work of other scientists. But rarely, a scientific discovery can also present an entirely new way of thinking about a problem – and is truly a breakthrough. In a conversation in May 2022, Feinberg said about Peter’s work: “the idea of brain connections was not in the thinking in the 1980s. The skeptics were many.”
In 1937, the Spanish neuroscientist and artist Ramón y Cajal commented on an apparent trial and error period of chaotic dendritic and axonic growth, with most of the resulting connections destined to disappear. But over subsequent decades the prevailing concept among neurobiologists and behavioral biologists was that synaptic connections increase with learning. Peter then rigorously showed in 1979 that after birth and an initial burst of growth in synapse numbers in human infants, there is a regression. He and his collaborators went on to show that these connections disappear at different times, with different kinetics, in distinct regions of the brain. The physical housekeeping task of this process is not small – selecting and eliminating billions of inactive synapses in the developing brain. In some regions of the brain, such as the visual system, the clearance mechanism seems to be relatively rapid over weeks or months. In other regions of the brain, for example in areas of higher cognitive function like the frontal cortex, this process of elimination is apparently more prolonged. What process or system has the intrinsic ability to identify and clear unused synapses? What has the ability to do this in a refined way, by targeting the correct contact sites, and without leaving dead cells and debris behind?
Peering at thousands of neurons under an electron microscope in the 1970s, Peter sought to understand how the structures through which neurons communicate, known as “synapses,” change during development of the human brain. How are these billions of synapses formed and refined? How do these circuits change, and allow us to remember, and to learn? What happens when these connections go awry and what is their relationship to human disease?
In the summer 2012, Peter said to me: “I have become an expert on Parkinson’s dementia.” He paused with a smile, his quiet humor intact after more than two decades of Parkinson’s disease. His next sentence came out after a bit of effort. “It is all about the synapses and abnormal pruning.”
The concept of synaptic pruning had an impact on mainstream thinking about early education soon after its discovery. The fact that millions of synapses are eliminated between early childhood and adulthood was mind boggling. But conceptually, it made sense that the brain circuitry starts with more connections than are needed, and subsequently can be sculpted based on input. Although it may seem inefficient, the early overabundance of connections enables a process for selective elimination and refinement that occurs over years, as children learn. But how does synaptic pruning relate to childhood learning? Education scholars and advocates had for centuries been alert to the positive impacts of education on child development. In 1979, education initiatives such as the Head Start program in the United States. were already in place. The discovery of synaptic pruning then supplied a new and specific biological basis supporting the benefits of early childhood education.
Peter and Janellen had shared ideas throughout their careers, molding each other’s perspectives on the fields of brain plasticity and development. Their shared interest and distinct approaches were accurately highlighted when they were introduced to their new retirement community in the early 2000s, through the Montgomery Place newsletter.
The Cognitive Revolution was an intellectual movement in the 1950s and 1960s focused on using the scientific method to understand human cognition, otherwise known as the process of learning and memory. This revolution in the field of psychology was happening at the same time that neuroscientists like Hubel and Wiesel were exploring how neurons in the brain are organized to enable behaviors such as visual perception. It was in this environment that both Peter and Janellen developed further as scientists. The connections between their respective fields provided fodder for discussion and debate throughout their careers. Peter focused on cellular events and biological processes in developmental neuroscience, while Janellen probed the mechanisms of human verbal and mathematical learning and memory.
Peter and Dieter traveled to “America” on the Marine Shark, a US cargo ship (Figure 7.1). The trip, as Peter understood it, was to “visit their mother.” Peter had not seen his mother since he was six years old. Intermittent letters and the packages of food and gifts were the only contacts. Peter refused to make the trip without Dieter, whose approval by the US government was delayed because he had helped in the German Airforce. Richard and Charlotte prepared Dieter and Peter for their transit, putting together a photo album for the boys with childhood photos. Dieter and Peter arrived in Ellis Island at the end of 1948, shortly before Peter’s 18th birthday (Figure 7.2).