Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-zzh7m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T05:10:51.198Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Behavioral testing in small-animal models: ischemic stroke

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 November 2009

Larry B. Goldstein
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology Duke Center for Cerebrovascular Disease Duke University Medical Center P.O. Box 3651 Durham, NC 27710 USA
Turgut Tatlisumak
Affiliation:
Helsinki University Central Hospital
Marc Fisher
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts Medical School
Get access

Summary

The ultimate validation of any animal model of human disease is the extent to which it simulates the parameter of interest. Depending on its purpose, the model may attempt to mimic the pathology and/or pathophysiology of a disease process, or predict the impact of putative therapeutic interventions. In the case of neurological disease, the ultimate outcome is behavioral. For example, in humans, the outcome of interest for patients with movement disorders is the preservation or return of normal motoric function, for persons afflicted with Alzheimer's disease it is the maintenance or restoration of normal cognition and for those with stroke or traumatic brain injury it is the return to their premorbid functional status. Although the fundamental features of simple stimulus–response relationships and even more complex environmental–behavioral interactions are remarkably preserved across species, the complete repertoire of human behaviors and their response to neurological disease are uniquely complex. Therefore, animal models will always be found to be wanting. Despite this fundamental limitation, animal behavior can provide critical insights into the functional consequences of human neurological disease and the potential benefit and toxicity of therapeutic interventions. They are particularly important as functional outcomes may be dissociated from the extent of neurological injury due to differences in the post-injury recovery process.

Limitations and principles

The limitations of small-animal behavioral models become apparent when considering the differences in even seemingly simple functional abilities as compared to humans.

Type
Chapter
Information
Handbook of Experimental Neurology
Methods and Techniques in Animal Research
, pp. 154 - 172
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Irle, E. Lesion size and recovery of function: some new perspectives. Brain Res. Rev. 1987, 12: 307–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maguire, S, Strittmatter, R, Chandra, S, Barone, FC. Stroke-prone rats exhibit prolonged behavioral deficits without increased brain injury: an indication of disrupted post-stroke brain recovery of function. Neurosci. Lett. 2004, 354: 229–233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, TA, Bury, SD, Adkins-Muir, DL, et al. Importance of behavioral manipulations and measures in rat models of brain damage and brain repair. Inst. Lab. Anim. Res. J. 2003, 44: 144–152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brailowsky, S, Knight, RT. Recovery from GABA-mediated hemiplegia in young and aged rats: effects of catecholaminergic manipulations. Neurobiol. Aging 1987, 8: 441–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, JE, Krauter, E, Campbell, BA. Motor and reflexive behavior in the aging rat. J. Gerontol. 1980, 35: 364–370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickford, P. Motor learning deficits in aged rats are correlated with loss of cerebellar noradrenergic function. Brain Res. 1993, 620: 133–138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, LB, Bullman, S. Age but not sex affects motor recovery after unilateral sensorimotor cortex suction-ablation in the rat. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 1999, 15: 39–43.Google ScholarPubMed
Hoane, MR, Lasley, , Akstulewicz, SL. Middle age increases tissue vulnerability and impairs sensorimotor and cognitive recovery following traumatic brain injury in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 2004, 153: 189–197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roof, RL, Zhang, Q, Glasier, MM, Stein, DG. Gender-specific impairment on Morris water maze task after entorhinal cortex lesion. Behav. Brain Res. 1993, 57: 47–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roof, RL, Duvdevani, R, Stein, DG. Gender influences outcome of brain injury: progesterone plays a protective role. Brain Res. 1993, 607: 333–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, K, Futrell, N, Tovar, S, et al. Gender influences the magnitude of the inflammatory response within embolic cerebral infarcts in young rats. Stroke 1966, 27: 498–503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, FD, Al-Khamees, K, Davey, MJ, Attree, EA. Environmental enrichment following brain damage: an aid to recovery or compensation? Behav. Brain Res. 1993, 5: 93–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolb, B, Forgie, M, Gibb, R, Gorny, G, Rowntree, S. Age, experience and the changing brain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1998, 22: 143–159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaulieu, C, Colonnier, M. Richness of environment affects the numbers of contacts formed by boutons containing flat vesicles but does not alter the number of these boutons per neuron. J. Comp. Neurol. 1988, 274: 347–356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johansson, BB. Functional outcome in rats transferred to an enriched environment 15 days after focal brain ischemia. Stroke 1996, 27: 324–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Will, B, Kelche, C. Environmental approaches to recovery of function from brain damage: a review of animal studies (1981 to 1991). Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 1992, 325: 79–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamm, RJ, Temple, MD, O'Dell, DM, Pike, BR, Lyeth, BG. Exposure to environmental complexity promotes recovery of cognitive function after traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma 1996, 13: 41–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schallert, T, Woodlee, MT, Fleming, SM. Experimental focal ischemic injury: behavior–brain interactions and issues of animal handling and housing. Inst. Lab. Anim. Res. J. 2003, 44: 130–143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, FD, Davey, MJ, Attree, EA. How does environmental enrichment aid performance following cortical injury in the rat? NeuroReport 1993, 4: 163–166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cotman, CW, Berchtold, NC. Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 2002, 25: 295–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleim, JA, Jones, TA, Schallert, T. Motor enrichment and the induction of plasticity before or after brain injury. Neurochem. Res. 2003, 28: 1757–1769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, LB, Davis, JN. Beam-walking in rats: studies towards developing an animal model of functional recovery after brain injury. J. Neurosci. Methods 1990, 31: 101–107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, LB, Davis, JN. Post-lesion practice and amphetamine-facilitated recovery of beam-walking in the rat. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 1990, 1: 311–314.Google ScholarPubMed
Nudo, RJ, Wise, BM, SiFuentes, F, Milliken, GW. Neural substrates for the effects of rehabilitative training on motor recovery after ischemic infarct. Science 1996, 272: 1791–1794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biernaskie, J, Corbett, D. Enriched rehabilitative training promotes improved forelimb motor function and enhanced dendritic growth after focal ischemic injury. J. Neurosci. 2001, 21: 5272–5280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biernaskie, J, Chernenko, G, Corbett, D. Efficacy of rehabilitative experience declines with time after focal ischemic brain injury. J. Neurosci. 2004, 24: 1245–1254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delay, ER, Rudolph, TL. Crossmodal training reduces behavioral deficits in rats after either auditory or visual cortex lesions. Physiol. Behav. 1994, 55: 293–300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, TA, Chu, CJ, Grande, , Gregory, AD. Motor skills training enhances lesion-induced structural plasticity in the motor cortex of adult rats. J. Neurosci. 1999, 19: 10153–10163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friel, KM, Heddings, AA, Nudo, RJ. Effects of postlesion experience on behavioral recovery and neurophysiologic reorganization after cortical injury in primates. Neurorehab. Neur. Repair 2000, 14: 187–198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humm, JL, Kozlowski, DA, Bland, ST, James, DC, Schallert, T. Use-dependent exaggeration of brain injury: is glutamate involved? Exp. Neurol. 1999, 157: 349–358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bland, ST, Schallert, T, Strong, R, Aronowski, J, Grotta, JC. Early exclusive use of the affected forelimb after moderate transient focal ischemia in rats: functional and anatomic outcome. Stroke 2000, 31: 1144–1151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kozlowski, DA, James, DC, Schallert, T. Use-dependent exaggeration of neuronal injury after unilateral sensorimotor cortex lesions. J. Neurosci. 1996, 16: 4776–4786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humm, JL, Kozlowski, DA, James, DC, Gotts, JE, Schallert, T. Use-dependent exacerbation of brain damage occurs during an early post-lesion vulnerable period. Brain Res. 1998, 783: 286–292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Risedal, A, Zeng, J, Johansson, BB. Early training may exacerbate brain damage after focal brain ischemia in the rat. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 1999, 19: 997–1003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leasure, JL, Schallert, T. Consequences of forced disuse of the impaired forelimb after unilateral cortical injury. Behav. Brain Res. 2004, 150: 83–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farrell, R, Evans, S, Corbett, D. Environmental enrichment enhances recovery of function but exacerbates ischemic cell death. Neuroscience 2001, 107: 585–592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bederson, JB, Pitts, LH, Tsuji, M, et al. Rat middle cerebral artery occlusion: evaluation of the model and development of a neurological examination. Stroke 1986, 17: 472–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donatelle, JM. Growth of the corticospinal tract and the development of placing reactions in the postnatal rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 1977, 175: 207–232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schallert, T, Fleming, SM, Leasure, JL, Tillerson, JL, Bland, ST. CNS plasticity and assessment of forelimb sensorimotor outcome in unilateral rat models of stroke, cortical ablation, parkinsonism and spinal cord injury. Neuropharmacology 2000, 39: 777–787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, JF. Sensorimotor disturbances in the aging rodent. J. Gerontol. 1982, 37: 548–554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyden, PD, Lonzo, LM, Nunez, SY, et al. Effect of ischemic cerebral volume changes on behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 1997, 87: 59–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markgraf, CG, Green, EJ, Hurwitz, BE, et al. Sensorimotor and cognitive consequences of middle cerebral artery occlusion in rats. Brain Res. 1992, 575: 238–246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernandez, TD, Schallert, T. Seizures and recovery from experimental brain damage. Exp. Neurol. 1988, 102: 318–324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barth, TM, Jones, TA, Schallert, T. Functional subdivisions of the rat somatic sensorimotor cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 1990, 39: 73–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aronowski, J, Samways, E, Strong, R, Rhoades, HM, Grotta, JC. An alternative method for the quantitation of neuronal damage after experimental middle cerebral artery occlusion in rats: analysis of behavioral deficit. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 1996, 16: 705–713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feeney, DM, Gonzalez, A, Law, WA. Amphetamine, haloperidol, and experience interact to affect the rate of recovery after motor cortex injury. Science 1982, 217: 855–857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, LB. Beam-walking in rats: the measurement of motor recovery after injury to the cerebral cortex. Neurosci. Protocols 1993, 10: 1–13.Google Scholar
Brailowsky, S, Knight, RT, Blood, K. G-aminobutyric acid-induced potentiation of cortical hemiplegia. Brain Res. 1986, 362: 322–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, LB, Vitek, MP, Dawson, H, Bullman, S. Expression of the apolipoproteine gene does not affect motor recovery after sensorimotor cortex injury in the mouse. Neuroscience 2000, 99: 705–710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, CA, Clifton, GL, Lighthall, JW, Yaghami, AA, Hayes, RL. A controlled cortical impact model of traumatic brain injury in the rat. J. Neurosci. Methods 1991, 39: 253–262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamm, RJ, Pike, BR, O'Dell, DM, Lyeth, BG, Jenkins, LW. The rotarod test: an evaluation of its effectiveness in assessing motor deficits following traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma 1994, 11: 187–196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, L, Schallert, T, Zhang, ZG, et al. A test for detecting long-term sensorimotor dysfunction in the mouse after focal cerebral ischemia. J. Neurosci. Methods 2002, 117: 207–214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montoya, CP, Campbell-Hope, LJ, Pemberton, KD, Dunnett, SB. The “staircase test”: a measure of independent forelimb reaching and grasping abilities in rats. J. Neurosci. Methods 1991, 36: 219–228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, RG, Garrud, P, Rawlins, JN, O'Keefe, J. Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature 1982, 297: 681–683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Staay, FJ, Augstein, K-H, Horváth, E. Sensorimotor impairments in rats with cerebral infarction, induced by unilateral occlusion of the left middle cerebral artery: strain differences and effects of the occlusion site. Brain Res. 1996, 735: 271–284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Modo, M, Stroemer, RP, Tang, E, et al. Neurological sequelae and long-term behavioural assessment of rats with transient middle cerebral artery occlusion. J. Neurosci. Methods 2000, 104: 99–109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Staay, FJ, Augstein, K-H, Horváth, E. Sensorimotor impairments in Wistar Kyoto rats with cerebral infarction, induced by unilateral occlusion of the middle cerebral artery: recovery of function. Brain Res. 1996, 715: 180–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamamoto, M, Tamura, A, Kirino, T, Shimizu, M, Sano, K. Behavioral changes after focal cerebral ischemia by left middle cerebral artery occlusion in rats. Brain Res. 1988, 452: 323–328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, CS, Andersen, AB, Finger, S. Neurological correlates of unilateral and bilateral “strokes” of the middle cerebral artery in the rat. Physiol. Behavi. 1991, 50: 263–269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nunn, J, Hodges, H. Cognitive deficits induced by global cerebral ischaemia: relationship to brain damage and reversal by transplants. Behav. Brain Res. 1994, 65: 1–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J, Sanberg, PR, Li, Y, et al. Intravenous administration of human umbilical cord blood reduces behavioral deficits after stroke in rats. Stroke 2001, 32: 2682–2688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, XL, Blizzard, KK, Zeng, ZY, et al. Chronic behavioral testing after focal ischemia in the mouse: functional recovery and the effects of gender. Exp. Neurol. 2004, 187: 94–104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Longa, EZ, Weinstein, PR, Carlson, S, Cummins, R. Reversible middle cerebral artery occlusion without craniectomy in rats. Stroke 1989, 20: 84–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunter, AJ, Hatcher, J, Virley, D, et al. Functional assessments in mice and rats after focal stroke. Neuropharmacology 2000, 39: 806–816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×