Published online by Cambridge University Press: 10 December 2009
Introduction
Learning by imitation is not a unitary task; thus understanding its various levels in order to potentiate imitative learning is daunting, whether subjects are human, animal or inanimate. For some syndromes arising in childhood (e.g. autism), lack of imitative ability (i.e. excluding rote, meaningless, often involuntary mimetic behavior, e.g. echolalia) is a defining characteristic (Hobson and Lee, 1999; Williams et al., 2001). What constitutes imitation in animals is still under study (Hurley and Chater, 2005; Nehaniv and Dautenhahn, this volume). And computers (and, by inference, robots using computer intelligence) may be “smart” in terms of brute processing power, but their learning is limited to what is easily programmed. Most computers and robots are presently analogous to living systems trained in relatively simple conditioned stimulus-response paradigms: given specific input parameters (that can, of course, be numerous and diverse), computers quickly and efficiently produce a predetermined, correct output (Pepperberg, 2001); computers can, however, generally solve only those new problems that are similar to ones they have already been programmed to solve. And although connectionist models are making significant advances, allowing generalization beyond training sets for a number of individual problems (e.g. Schlesinger and Parisi, 2004), at present most computational mechanisms – and autistic children – cannot learn in ways normal humans do. No one model, for example, does all the following: form new abstract representations, manipulate these representations to attain concrete goals, transfer information acquired in one domain to manage tasks in another, integrate new and existing disparate forms of knowledge (e.g. linguistic, contextual, emotional) to solve novel problems – nor can any one model achieve these behavior patterns through imitation in ways managed by every normal young child (see Lieberman, 2001).
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.