Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T23:20:38.218Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Gestures, knowledge, and the world

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 January 2010

Curtis LeBaron
Affiliation:
Department of Communication, University of Colorado at Boulder
Jürgen Streeck
Affiliation:
Department of Communication Studies, University of Texas at Austin
David McNeill
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
Get access

Summary

The formation of gestures and the fabrication of knowledge

Among the essential (but almost forgotten) insights of the Age of Enlightenment was the recognition that “human understanding” (Locke 1959 [1690]) – the formation and accumulation of common knowledge – is dependent upon the formation and accumulation of material signs: material entities that reside, however fleetingly, in the public realm where they may be reused and thereby shared; representations that embody a world that may be jointly acted and reflected upon; artifacts that, while products of minds, are nevertheless external to them, providing tools not only for the mind, but also for labor and human self-creation; socially shared cognitive tools that evolve over time as humanity's mind evolves through relying on and refining them.

With this proposition, anti-Cartesian philosophers such as Condillac for the first time directed attention to the importance of symbol systems (or media) for human cognition, self-creation, and society. Condillac in particular recognized the inherently social character of the human mind, and he suggested that signs and insights originate in social practice. He wrote:

[The] history of communication will show the various circumstances under which signs have been formed. It will show us the true meanings of signs and … leave no doubt about the origin of our ideas.

(Condillac 1746: 61)

Condillac called signs “sensations transformées,” transformed sensations, by which he meant the entire complex of affect, desire, sensory perception, and motor action that makes up what nowadays we might call ‘embodied experience’.

Type
Chapter
Information
Language and Gesture , pp. 118 - 138
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2000

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×