Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vfjqv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T05:20:15.354Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - How scientific usages reflect implicit theories: Adaptation, development, instinct, learning, cognition, and intelligence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 May 2010

Sue Taylor Parker
Affiliation:
Sonoma State University, California
Kathleen Rita Gibson
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Houston
Get access

Summary

As the anthropological linguist Edward Sapir noted, language is largely unconscious. In everyday life, we all use words without knowing exactly what we mean by them. Examination of scholarly works suggests that we extend this practice into our professional lives as well. The words we use and the way we use them reflect our implicit, unconscious knowledge, presuppositions, and theories (and, equally, our implicit, unconscious ignorance). The nature of one's implicit knowledge (and ignorance) is determined by one's disciplinary training and one's personal intellectual history.

Specialists in philosophy, zoology (especially ethology), cognitive sciences, and various branches of psychology, for example, often use the terms learning, intelligence, and cognition in subtly different ways. Scholars in different subdisciplines describe similar phenomena in different terms: Comparative psychologists, especially learning and conditioning theorists, for example, use the term learning to describe the acquisition of instrumental behaviors, whereas comparative developmental psychologists use intelligence or cognition to describe such acquisitions. Through the years, various investigators have used a variety of terms to characterize animal abilities. A brief survey of books on mental abilities of animals, for example, reveals varying usages of the related terms mind/mentality, learning, cognition, intelligence, thought, and consciousness (Table 2.1).

Instinct, learning, intelligence, and development are used in different ways by embryologists, learning and conditioning theorists, ethologists, and developmental psychologists. Although William James and John B.

Type
Chapter
Information
'Language' and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes
Comparative Developmental Perspectives
, pp. 65 - 96
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×