Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T11:06:53.367Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Transparency versus Processing Efficiency: A Case Study on German Declension

from Part IV - Social and Language Evolution

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 November 2017

Remi Van Trijp
Affiliation:
Sony CSL Paris, France
Thierry Poibeau
Affiliation:
Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Paris
Aline Villavicencio
Affiliation:
Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil
Get access

Summary

Abstract

Ambiguity is one of the most fascinating mysteries of human language that divides the linguistic research field in roughly two camps: the mainstream view, which considers ambiguity to be undesirable for communication; and the cognitive-functional view, which argues that ambiguity allows for more efficient communication. This chapter subscribes to the cognitive-functional view and presents a case study on the German declension system, which is notorious for its ambiguity through the use of syncretic case forms. Through amethodology based on computational reconstruction, the paper suggests that the current declension system outperforms its historical predecessors in terms of efficient communication, while featuring a labor-saving distribution of morphological marking across articles, adjectives, and nouns.

Introduction

Intuitively speaking, successful communication requires languages to exhibit a transparent mapping between meaning and form. Cross-linguistic research, however, shows that such transparency is the exception rather than the rule (Leufkens, 2015). Indeed, computational linguists will sometimes jokingly admit that they only have three problems to solve: ambiguity, ambiguity, and ambiguity.

Ambiguity is a major puzzle that divides the study of natural language in roughly two views (Winkler, 2015). One view, most outspokenly assumed by Chomskyan linguistics, considers ambiguity to be harmful for communication – an assumption that is sometimes used for arguing that language is in fact “poorly designed” for communication (Chomsky, 2008, p. 136). The opposing view, defended in cognitive-functional approaches to language, argues that ambiguity makes inferential communication systems more efficient (Piantadosi et al., 2012).

This study subscribes to the cognitive-functional approach and argues that ambiguity may lead to greater efficiency in language processing. As a case study, the chapter focuses on why case syncretism has emerged in the German declension system. Syncretism occurs when the same form can be mapped onto different grammatical functions. One example is the English suffix -s, which can be used as a number marker in nouns (e.g. cats) or as a conjugation marker in verbs discovers. Often enough, these functions may be contradictory: as a nominal marker, -s expresses plurality, but when used as a verbal suffix, it expresses agreement with a singular subject. The German declension system in particular is notorious for this kind of syncretism, which has puzzled many linguists for decades (see a.o. Bierwisch, 1967; Blevins, 1995;Wiese, 1996;Wunderlich, 1997; Müller, 2001; Daniels, 2001; Müller, 2002; Crysmann, 2005; Dalrymple et al., 2009).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

M., Bierwisch. Syntactic features in morphology: General problems of so-called pronominal inflection in German. In To Honour Roman Jakobson, pages 239–270. Mouton De Gruyter, Berlin, 1967.
Mouton De Gruyter, Berlin, 1967. James Blevins. Syncretism and paradigmatic opposition. Linguistics and Philosophy, 18:113–152, 1995.Google Scholar
David, Carter. Efficient disjunctive unification for bottom-up parsing. In Proceedings of the 13th Conference on Computational Linguistics, pages 70–75. ACL, 1990.
Noam, Chomsky. On phases. In Robert, Freidin, Carlos P., Otero, and Maria Luisa, Zubizarreta, editors, Foundational Issues in Linguistic Theory: Essays in Honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud, pages 133–166. MIT Press, Cambridge MA, 2008.Google Scholar
H., Clahsen, M., Hadler, S., Eisenbeiss, and I., Sonnenstuhl-Henning. Morphological paradigms in language processing and language disorders. Transactions of the Philological Society, 99(2):247–277, 2001.Google Scholar
Ann, Copestake. Implementing Typed Feature Structure Grammars. CSLI Publications, Stanford, 2002.Google Scholar
B., Crysmann. Syncretism in german: A unified approach to underspecification, indeterminacy, and likeness of case. In S. Müller, editor, Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar, pages 91–107. Stanford, 2005. CSLI.
Walter, Daelemans and Antal, Van den Bosch. Memory-Based Language Processing. Studies in Natural Language Processing. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 2005.Google Scholar
Mary, Dalrymple, Tracy, Holloway King, and Louisa, Sadler. Indeterminacy by underspecification. Journal of Linguistics, 45:31–68, 2009.Google Scholar
M., Daniels. On a type-based analysis of feature neutrality and the coordination of unlikes. In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on HPSG, pages 137– 147, Stanford, 2001. CSLI.
Sonja, Eisenbeiss, Suzanne, Bartke, and Harald, Clahsen. Structural and lexical case in child German: Evidence from language language-impaired and typicallydeveloping children. Language Acquisition, 13:3–32, 2005/2006.Google Scholar
D.P., Flickinger. On building a more efficient grammar by exploiting types. Natural Language Engineering, 6(1): 15–28, 2000.Google Scholar
J.T., Hale. The information conveyed by words in sentences. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(2):101–123, 2003.Google Scholar
W., Heinz and J., Matiasek. Argument structure and case assignment in German. In J., Nerbonne, K., Netter, and C., Pollard, editors, German in Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar, pages 199–236. CSLI, Stanford, 1994.Google Scholar
R.J.P., Ingria. The limits of unification. In Proceedings of the 28th Annual Meeting of the ACL, pages 194–204. 1990.
L., Karttunen. Features and values. In Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Computational Linguistics, Stanford, 1984.
Sterre, Leufkens. Transparency in Language: A Typological Study. LOT, Utrecht, 2015.Google Scholar
Robert, Levine, Thomas, Hukari, and Michael, Calcagno. Parasitic gaps in english: Some overlooked cases and their theoretical consequences. In Peter W. Culicover and Paul, M. Postal, editors, Parasitic Gaps. MIT Press, Cambridge MA, 2001.Google Scholar
Roger, Levy. Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106(3): 1126–1177, 2008.Google Scholar
Christopher, D. Mannning and Hinrich, Schütze. Foundations of Statistical Natural Language Processing. MIT Press, Cambridge MA, 1999.Google Scholar
Gereon, Müller. Remarks on nominal inflection in German. In Ingrid Kaufmann and Barbara, Stiebels, editors, More than Words: A Festschrift for Dieter Wunderlich, pages 113–145. Akademie Verlag, Berlin, 2002.Google Scholar
Stefan, Müller. An HPSG-analysis for free relative clauses in german. Grammars, 2(1): 53–105, 1999.Google Scholar
Stefan, Müller. Case in German – towards and HPSG analysis. In Tibor, Kiss and Detmar, Meurers, editors, Constraint-Based Approaches to Germanic Syntax. CSLI, Stanford, 2001.Google Scholar
J., Perkell, M., Zandipour, M., Matthies, and H., Lane. Economy of effort in different speaking conditions. i. a preliminary study of intersubject differences and modeling issues. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 112: 1627–1641, 2002.Google Scholar
Steven T., Piantadosi, Harry, Tily, and Edward, Gibson. The communicative function of ambiguity in language. Cognition, 122: 280–291, 2012.Google Scholar
Geoffrey, Pullum and Arnold, Zwicky. Phonological resolution of syntactic feature conflict. Language, 62(4): 751–773, 1986.Google Scholar
Allan, Ramsay. Disjunction without tears. Computational Linguistics, 16(3): 171–174, 1990.Google Scholar
C., Russ. The German Language Today. A Linguistic Introduction. Routledge, London, 1994.Google Scholar
I.A., Sag. Coordination and underspecification. In J., Kom and S., Wechsler, editors, Proceedings of the Ninth International Conference on HPSG, Stanford, 2003. CSLI.Google Scholar
Luc, Steels. Constructivist development of grounded construction grammars. In Walter Daelemans, editor, Proceedings 42nd Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, pages 9–19. Barcelona, 2004.
K.N., Stevens. Toward a model for lexical access based on acoustic landmarks and distinctive features. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 111: 1872–1891, 2002.Google Scholar
Remi, van Trijp. A design pattern for argument structure constructions. In Luc, Steels, editor, Design Patterns in Fluid Construction Grammar. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, 2011a.Google Scholar
Remi, van Trijp. A design pattern for argument structure constructions. In Luc, Steels, editor, Design Patterns in Fluid Construction Grammar, pages 205–235. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, 2011b.Google Scholar
Remi, van Trijp. Linguistic selection criteria for explaining language change: A case study on syncretism in German definite articles. Language Dynamics and Change, 3: 105–132, 2013.Google Scholar
B., Wiese. Iconicity and syncretism. on pronominal inflection in Modern German. In R., Sckmann, editor, Theoretical Linguistics and Grammatical Description, pages 323–344. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, 1996.Google Scholar
Susanne, Winkler, editor. Ambiguity: Language and Communication. Walter De Gruyter, Berlin, 2015.
Joseph, Wright. An Old High German Primer. Clarendon Press, Oxford, 2nd edition, 1906.Google Scholar
Dieter, Wunderlich. Der unterspezifizierte Artikel. In Karl, Heinz Ramers, Dürscheid and Monika, Schwarz, editors, Sprache im Fokus, pages 47–55. Niemeyer, Tübingen, 1997.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×