from Part III - Analogue windows
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 March 2016
Enter the !Kung
As noted in Section 7.5, the behaviour of contemporary hunter-gatherer mothers has been used by Dean Falk (2004: 499) for modelling the way in which early hominin mothers may have cared for their infants. Facets of the linguistic behaviour of contemporary hunter-gatherers and other people living in non-industrialised societies have likewise been used as a window on the evolution of language. This has been done recently by Katharine MacDonald and Wil Roebroeks (2013) in their study of the linguistic capacities of Neanderthals and other earlier hominins. They proceed from the view that attempts to draw inferences about these capacities from data in the archaeological record have to date been unsuccessful. So, they consider it necessary to develop ‘other ways of entry into the topic’. And they (2013: 98), accordingly, propose to take Neanderthals’ accomplishments in hunting large animals as such a way of entry. Proceeding from the assumption that efficient forms of communication and of knowledge transfer by teaching lay at the root of Pleistocene hunting skills, MacDonald and Roebroeks address the question whether the same holds good for Neanderthals. In response to this question, they argue as follows:
MacDonald and Roebroeks's line of argument
We cannot, obviously, study whether that was the case for Neanderthals, but we can study the way and contexts in which communication enhances foraging efficiency among extant hunter-gatherers whose traditional subsistence practices must be in many ways comparable to those of the Neanderthals (MacDonald 2007). (MacDonald and Roebroeks 2013: 98)
Pursuing this line of argument, MacDonald and Roebroeks review a considerable body of ethnographic work on the role of verbal instruction in the acquisition of hunting skills by children of modern hunter-gatherers such as the !Kung. That role, they believe, is likely to be suggestive of the role played by linguistic communication in Neanderthal transfer of information about animal behaviour.
In a second recent study, Maggie Tallerman (2013a) adopts a similar approach to getting a handle on the use of (proto)language by early hominins such as Homo erectus. At issue in her study is the tenability of proposals to the effect that the evolution of (proto)language was driven by interactions between closely related individuals, mothers and infants among them. In appraising these proposals, she is confronted with the problem of the lack of direct evidence about early hominin lifestyles.
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.