Skip to main content Accesibility Help
×
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 3
  • Cited by
    This chapter has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Lukasik, Karolina M. Lehtonen, Minna Soveri, Anna Waris, Otto Jylkkä, Jussi Laine, Matti and Dritschel, Barbara 2018. Bilingualism and working memory performance: Evidence from a large-scale online study. PLOS ONE, Vol. 13, Issue. 11, p. e0205916.

    Sharwood Smith, Michael 2017. Working with working memory and language. Second Language Research, Vol. 33, Issue. 3, p. 291.

    Heller, Benedikt Szmrecsanyi, Benedikt and Grafmiller, Jason 2017. Stability and Fluidity in Syntactic Variation World-Wide. Journal of English Linguistics, Vol. 45, Issue. 1, p. 3.

    ×
  • Print publication year: 2012
  • Online publication date: November 2012

3 - Working memory and (second) language processing

Summary
Abstract

This chapter discusses the interactions between two of the most important human cognitive functions: memory and language. First, the concept of working memory is introduced, along with a brief summary of the evolutions that working memory theory has undergone in the last decades. The second part of the chapter focuses on the role of (verbal) working memory in language acquisition and processing. It is argued that working memory, and especially the ability to temporarily represent serial-order information, is crucially involved in both native and foreign word learning, and perhaps also in sentence and text comprehension. The third and final part of the chapter explores the other direction of the interaction, by questioning whether language processing can influence working memory functioning. This question is addressed with recent behavioral and neurological evidence for a general executive control advantage in bilinguals, which makes a strong case for the trainability of some aspects of working memory.

Recommend this book

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this book to your organisation's collection.

Memory, Language, and Bilingualism
  • Online ISBN: 9781139035279
  • Book DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139035279
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to *
×

References

Abutalebi, J., & Costa, A. (2008). Acquisition, processing and loss of L2: functional, cognitive and neural perspectives. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21, 473–476.
Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: the same or different constructs?Psychological Bulletin, 131, 30–60.
Aron, A. R., Watkins, L., Sahakian, B. J., Monsell, S., Barker, R. A., & Robbins, T. W. (2003). Task-set switching deficits in early-stage Huntington’s disease: implications for basal ganglia function. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 629–642.
Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Short-term phonological memory and long-term learning: a single-case study. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 5, 129–148.
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423.
Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 4, 829–839.
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation Vol. 8 (pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.
Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J. (2006). Executive control in a modified anti-saccade task: effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1341–1354.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 459–464.
Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1905). Application des méthodes nouvelles au diagnostic du niveau intellectual chez les enfants normaux et anormaux d’hospice et d’école primaire. L’Année Psychologique, 11, 245–336.
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.
Bowey, J. A. (2001). Nonword repetition and young children’s receptive vocabulary: a longitudinal study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 441–469.
Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 908–914.
Brass, M., (2004). Selection for cognitive control: an fMRI study on the selection of task relevant information. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 8847–8852.
Brysbaert, M., & Duyck, W. (2010). Is it time to leave behind the revised hierarchical model of bilingual language processing after 15 years of service?Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 359–371.
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: a network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106, 551–581.
Burgess, N., (2006). A revised model of short-term memory and long-term learning of verbal sequences. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 627–652.
Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S. (1999). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 77–94.
Cheung, H. (1996). Nonword span as a unique predictor of second-language vocabulary learning. Developmental Psychology, 32, 867–873.
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M. (2004). Lexical access in bilingual speech production: evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 491–511.
Costa, A., Santesteban, M., & Ivanova, I. (2006). How do highly proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? Inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both functional. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 32, 1057–1074.
Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information-processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 163–191.
Crinion, J., Turner, R., Grogan, A., Hanakawa, T., Noppeney, U., Devlin, J. T., et al. (2006). Language control in the bilingual brain. Science, 312, 1537–1540.
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.
Daneman, M., & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: a meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 422–433.
Duyck, W., Szmalec, A., Kemps, E., & Vandierendonck, A. (2003). Verbal working memory is involved in associative word learning unless visual codes are available. Journal of Memory & Language, 48, 527–541.
Emmorey, K., Luk, G., Pyers, J. E., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The source of enhanced cognitive control in bilinguals. Psychological Science, 19, 1201–1206.
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331.
French, L., & O’Brien (2008). Phonological memory and children’s second language grammar learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 463–487.
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Complexities and constraints in nonword repetition and word learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 599–613.
Gathercole, S. E., & Adams, A. M. (1993). Phonological working memory in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 29, 770–778.
Gathercole, S. E., (1994). Children’s phonological working memory: contributions of long-term knowledge and rehearsal. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 672–688.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: a study of young children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439–454.
Goethe, K., & Oberauer, K. (2008). The integration of familiarity and recollection information in short-term recognition: modeling speed–accuracy trade-off functions. Psychological Research, 72, 289–303.
Gollan, T. H., & Acenas, L. A. R. (2004). What is a TOT? Cognate and translation effects on tip-of-the-tongue states in Spanish–English and Tagalog–English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 246–269.
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81.
Gupta, P. (2005). Primacy and recency in nonword repetition. Memory, 13, 318–324.
Gupta, P. (2009). A computational model of nonword repetition, immediate serial recall, and nonword learning. In A. Thorn and M. Page (eds.), Interactions between short-term and long-term memory in the verbal domain (pp. 108–135). Hove: Psychology Press.
Gupta, P., & Tisdale, J. (2009). Does phonological short-term memory causally determine vocabulary learning? Toward a computational resolution of the debate. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 481–502.
Gupta, P., Lipinski, J., Abbs, B., & Lin, P.-H. (2005). Serial position effects in nonword repetition. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 141–162.
Hebb, D. O. (1961). Distinctive features of learning in the higher animal. In J. F. Delafresnaye (ed.), Brain mechanisms and learning (pp. 37–46). Oxford: Blackwell.
Hernandez, A. E., Martinez, A., & Kohnert, K. (2000). In search of the language switch: an fMRI study of picture naming in Spanish–English bilinguals. Brain and Language, 73, 421–431.
Hernandez, A. E., Dapretto, M., Mazziotta, J., & Bookheimer, S. (2001). Language switching and language representation in Spanish–English bilinguals: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 14, 510–520.
Hummel, K., & French, L. (2010). Phonological memory and implications for the second language classroom. Canadian Modern Language Review, 66, 371–391.
Ivanova, I., & Costa, A. (2008). Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production?Acta Psychologica, 127, 277–288.
Jacobs, J. (1887). Experiments on “prehension.”Mind, 12, 75–79.
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 6829–6833.
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Shah, P. (2011). Short and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 10081–10086.
Jarrold, C., & Towse, J. N. (2006). Individual differences in working memory. Neuroscience, 139, 39–50.
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 98, 122–149.
Kennedy, A., Brooks, R., Flynn, L.-A., & Prophet, C. (2003). The reader’s spatial code. In J. Hyönä, R. Radach, & H. Deubel (eds.), The mind’s eye: cognitive and applied aspects of eye movement research (pp. 193–212). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Lauro, L. J., Reis, J., Cohen, L. G., Cecchetto, C., & Papagno, C. (2010). A case for the involvement of phonological loop in sentence comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 48, 4003–4011.
Majerus, S., Lekeu, F., Van der Linden, M., & Salmon, E. (2001). Deep dysphasia: further evidence on the relationship between phonological short-term memory and language processing impairments. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 385–410.
Majerus, S., Van der Linden, M., Mulder, G., Meulemans, T., & Peters, F. (2004). Verbal short-term memory reflects the sublexical organization of the phonological language network: evidence from an incidental phonotactic learning paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 297–306.
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Elsen, B., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Exploring the relationship between new word learning and short-term memory for serial order recall, item recall, and item recognition. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18, 848–873.
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Greffe, C., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Relations between vocabulary development and verbal short-term memory: the relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and item information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 95–119.
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Van der Linden, M., & Weekes, B. (2008). Lexical learning in bilingual adults: the relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and phonological knowledge. Cognition, 107, 395–419.
Martin, N., & Saffran, E. M. (1992). A computational account of deep dysphasia: evidence from a single case study. Brain and Language, 43, 240–274.
Meuter, R. F. I., & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language switching in naming: asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 25–40.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.
Mosse, E. K., & Jarrold, C. (2008). Hebb learning, verbal short-term memory, and the acquisition of phonological forms in children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 505–514.
O’Brien, I., Segalowitz, N., Collentine, J., & Freed, B. (2006). Phonological memory and lexical, narrative, and grammatical skills in second-language oral production by adult learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 377–402.
Oberauer, K. (2009). Design for a working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 51, 45–100.
Oztekin, I., & McElree, B. (2007). Proactive interference slows recognition by eliminating fast assessments of familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 126–149.
Page, M. P. A., & Norris, D. (1998). The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. Psychological Review, 105, 761–781.
Page, M. P. A., (2009). A model linking immediate serial recall, the Hebb repetition effect and the learning of phonological word-forms. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 3737–3753.
Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1995). Short-term memory and vocabulary learning in polyglots. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 98–107.
Papagno, C., Valentine, T., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). Phonological short-term memory and foreign-language learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 331–347.
Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task switchers: evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 682–691.
Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443–447.
Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory, and foreign- language learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 21–50.
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2010). Does working memory training generalize?Psychologica Belgica, 50, 245–276.
Swets, B., Desmet, T., Hambrick, D. Z., & Ferreira, F. (2007). The role of working memory in syntactic ambiguity resolution: a psychometric approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 64–81.
Szmalec, A., Duyck, W., Vandierendonck, A., Barberá Mata, A., & Page, M. P. A. (2009). The Hebb repetition effect as a laboratory analogue of novel word learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 435–443.
Szmalec, A., Loncke, M., Page, M., & Duyck, W. (2011). Order or disorder? Impaired Hebb learning in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1270–1279.
Szmalec, A., Verbruggen, F., Vandierendonck, A., & Kemps, E. (2011). Control of interference during working memory updating. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 137–151.
Vallar, G., & Baddeley, A. D. (1984). Fractionation of working memory: neuropsychological evidence for a phonological short-term store. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 23, 151–161.
Van Assche, E., Duyck, W., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Diependaele, K. (2009). Does bilingualism change native-language reading? Cognate effects in sentence context. Psychological Science, 20, 923–927.
Vandierendonck, A., & Szmalec, A. (eds.) (2011). Spatial Working Memory. Hove: Psychology Press.
Vejnovic´, D., Milin, P., & Zdravkovic´, S. (2010). Effects of proficiency and age of language acquisition on working memory performance in bilinguals. Psihologija, 43, 219–232.
Wang, Y., Xue, G., Chen, C., Xue, F., & Dong, Q. (2007). Neural basis of asymmetric language switching in second-language learners: an ER-fMRI study. NeuroImage, 35, 862–870.
Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. (1996). The capacity theory of sentence comprehension: critique of Just and Carpenter (1992). Psychological Review, 103, 761–772.
Whitney, C., & Cornelissen, P. (2005). Letter-position encoding and dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 274–301.