from Part III - METHODS: MEASUREMENT, STRATEGIES AND NEW APPROACHES
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 August 2016
Depending on the processes being assessed, the term ‘evaluative research’ is used for different approaches (Milne, 1987): ‘effort evaluation’ examines the relationship between the characteristics and activities of a programme or service (e.g. physician-patient ratio, utilisation of beds) and the money spent on it (e.g. for staff and equipment); ‘process evaluation’ is traditionally limited to a simple outline of services over time (number of patients, type and extent of services offered); ‘efficiency evaluation’ establishes the relationship between the cost of a service and the goal it aims to achieve; ‘client satisfaction evaluation’ examines the patients’ acceptance of treatment measures. ‘Outcome evaluation’ is the core of evaluative research; here, the result of a measure is the focus of the study. This approach is chosen when the effectiveness of interventions or services is to be examined. Outcome evaluation relates most directly to the primary goal of treatment, ‘which is to make the patient better’ (Schwartz et al.1973).
In a mental health care system many events occur at the same time. So at first sight it seems rather arbitrary to assume that of all things happening one event ‘treatment’ is causally related to another event ‘outcome’, for example absence or presence of schizophrenic symptoms at a given point in time. Many factors besides treatment may influence psychopathological status. It is well known that the mere incidence or correlation of two events does not tell anything about which is the cause and which is the effect, whether two events are mutual interdependent or whether they are both dependent upon an unknown third variable (Simon, 1976). Chains of cause and effect are not observable in principle; it is also impossible to verify a causal relationship empirically. The testing of a causal hypothesis rather results by ruling out rival explanations for an effect (Kraak, 1966; Cook & Campbell, 1979).
According to Kenny (1979) three commonly accepted conditions must hold for a scientist to claim that the event X causes the event Y:
Temporal precedence of the cause: for X to cause Y, X must precede Y in time; such temporal precedence means a causal relationship is asymmetric.
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.