from Part II - Computational neuroscience models
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 November 2011
A central and largely unsolved problem in the brain sciences is to understand the functional architecture of the vertebrate nervous system. Many questions about this architecture revolve around the issue of action selection. Because it is a fundamental property of neurons to be selective with regard to the patterns of input activity to which they respond, claims that particular brain subsystems are specifically or preferentially involved in the selection of action, as distinct to other aspects of control, must meet more stringent requirements (see below). It is also by no means inevitable that the functional decomposition of the brain will contain specialist action-selection mechanisms (see Seth, this volume). Appropriate behavioural switching could be a global property of nervous system function, and of its embedding in a body and environment, that cannot be attributed to specific subcomponents of brain architecture. In other words, it is plausible that an animal may ‘flip’ from one integrated pattern of behavioural output to another without some identifiable internal ‘switch’ being thrown. On the other hand, theoretical arguments can be presented, based for instance on the benefits that accrue from modularity (Bryson, 2005; Prescott et al., 1999; Wagner and Altenberg, 1996), to suggest that biological control systems may include specialised action-selection components. Hence, one important debate in this field is whether there are specialised mechanisms for action selection in animal nervous systems, and, if so, where these might be found (see also Prescott, 2007, for an evolutionary perspective on this question).
Neural substrates for action selection in cortico-basal ganglia loops
Redgrave et al. (1999) have proposed that, to be considered as a candidate action-selection mechanism, a neural subsystem should exhibit the following properties. First, it should have inputs that carry information about both internal and external cues relevant to decision making. Second, there should be some internal mechanism that allows calculation of the urgency or ‘salience’ that should be attached to each available action. Third, there should be mechanisms that allow for the resolution of conflicts between competing actions based on their relative salience. Finally, the outputs of the system should be configured so as to allow the expression of winning actions whilst disallowing losers. There is now a growing consensus in the neuroscience literature that the basal ganglia – a group of functionally related structures found in the mid- and forebrain of all vertebrates – meet these criteria and therefore may represent an important neural action-selection substrate.
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.