Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T00:57:37.219Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Development of Communicative Competence through Private and Inner Speech

from Part II - Language, Communication, Social Cognition, and Awareness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 July 2009

Adam Winsler
Affiliation:
George Mason University, Virginia
Charles Fernyhough
Affiliation:
University of Durham
Ignacio Montero
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
Get access

Summary

One of the most challenging psycholinguistic problems children face when they are acquiring a first language is learning how to use it to communicate. Communicating with speech is complex because it is simultaneously a social, linguistic, and psychological activity. Although most research on children's acquisition of language has focused on the lexical, semantic, and syntactic structures, it is the discourse structures of language that are pivotal to communication (Moffett, 1968). Discourse occupies the level of organization of language just above the sentence or clause – the sociolinguistic level where a sentence initiated by one person is met with a responding sentence from another person (Stubbs, 1983). The initiation-response structure of discourse frames and regulates the interpersonal exchange of sentences, as well as the ideas conveyed by them. Conversational activity – the most basic type of discourse – has the capacity to structure communicative interactions because it is conducted according to social conventions, the most obvious of which is the alternation of turns at talking and listening.

Learning to converse involves understanding not only how to produce and comprehend words, phrases, and grammatically well-formed sentences, but also how to initiate and respond to a turn at talk, how to play complementary social roles, how to imagine another person's perspective, and how to formulate and interpret communicative intentions. Initiating and responding to a conversational turn at talk are deceptively complicated activities because they require knowing the more subtle social conventions that prescribe how participants are expected to think, such as the obligation speakers have to stick to the topic – that is, to adapt the information in their utterance so that it both responds to the previous turn and initiates the next one.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Astington, J. W., Harris, P. L., & Olson, D. R. (1988). Developing theories of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Austin, J. (1962). How to do things with words. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). Discourse in the novel. In Bakhtin, M. M., The dialogical imagination (Silverstein, M., Trans.) (pp. 259–422). Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Bakhtin, M. M. (1986). The problem of speech genres. In Bakhtin, M. M., Speech genres and other late essays (pp. 60–102). Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1975). Peer relations and the acquisition of language. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. A. (Eds.), Friendship and peer relations (pp. 259–292). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, J., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1977). From gesture to first word: On cognitive and social prerequisites. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (Eds.), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bem, S. L. (1967). Verbal self-control: The establishment of effective self-instruction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 484–491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berk, L. E. (1992). Children's private speech: An overview of theory and the status of research. In Díaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 17–53). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Boggs, S. T. (1990). The role of routines in the evolution of children's peer talk. In Dorval, B. (Ed.), Conversational organization and its development (pp. 101–130). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1968). Introduction. In Moffett, J., Teaching the universe of discourse. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1981). The pragmatics of acquisition. In Deutsch, W. (Ed.), The child's construction of language (pp. 39–55). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1983). Child's talk: Learning to use language. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Camaioni, L. (1979). Child-adult and child-child conversations: An interactional approach. In Ochs, E. & Shieffelin, B. B. (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics (pp. 325–337). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Newson, M. (1992). Assessing the importance of subvocalization during normal silent reading. Reading & Writing, 4, 55–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villiers, J. G., & Villiers, P. A. (2003). Language for thought: Coming to understand false beliefs. In Gentner, D. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Díaz, R. M. (1986). Issues in the empirical study of private speech: A response to Frawley and Lantolf's commentary. Developmental Psychology, 22, 709–711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Díaz, R. M. (1992). Methodological concerns in the study of private speech. In Díaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 55–81). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1975). Holophrases, speech acts and language universals. Journal of Child Language, 2, 21–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dore, J. (1977a). Children's illocutionary acts. In Freedle, R. (Ed.), Discourse production and comprehension (pp. 227–244). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1977b). “Oh them sheriff”: A pragmatic analysis of children's responses to questions. In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (Eds.), Child discourse (pp. 139–163). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dore, J. (1979). Conversational acts and the acquisition of language. In Ochs, E. & Shieffelin, B. B. (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics (pp. 339–361). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1982). Conversation as accountability practice: Ethno-pragmatic analyses of language meanings and functions. Cognition and Brain Research, 4, 201–215.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1989). Monologue as reenvoicement of dialogue. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 231–260). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Eisler, F. G. (1968). Psycholinguistics: Experiments in spontaneous speech. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1979). Children's verbal turn-taking. In Ochs, E. & Shieffelin, B. B. (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics (pp. 391–414). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Feigenbaum, P. (1992). Development of the syntactic and discourse structures of private speech. In Díaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 181–198). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Feigenbaum, P. (2002). Private speech: Cornerstone of Vygotsky's theory of the development of higher psychological processes. In Robbins, D. & Stetsenko, A. (Eds.), Voices within Vygotsky's non-classical psychology: Past, present, future (pp. 162–174). New York: Nova Science.Google Scholar
Feigenbaum, P. (2004, November). Empirical evidence of the many uses of private and inner speech. Paper presented at the First International Symposium on the Self-Regulatory Functions of Language, Madrid, Spain.Google Scholar
Fernyhough, C. (1996). The dialogic mind: A dialogic approach to the higher mental functions. New Ideas in Psychology, 14, 47–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernyhough, C., & Fradley, E. (2005). Private speech on an executive task: Relations with task difficulty and task performance. Cognitive Development, 20, 103–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fijalkow, J. (1989). Auto-language and learning to read. Enfance, 42, 83–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Botkin, P. T., Fry, C. L., Wright, J. W., & Jarvis, P. E. (1968). The development of role-taking and communication skills in children. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Frake, C. O. (1977). How to ask for a drink in Subanun. In Giglioli, P. P. (Ed.), Language and social context: Selected readings. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Freedle, R., & Lewis, M. (1977). Prelinguistic conversations. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. A. (Eds.), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language (pp. 157–185). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Fuson, K. C. (1979). The development of self-regulating aspects of speech: A review. In Zivin, G. (Ed.), The development of self-regulation through private speech (pp. 135–217). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Galda, L. (1984). Narrative competence: Play, storytelling, and story comprehension. In Pellegrini, A. & Yaukey, T. (Eds.), The development of oral and written language in social contexts (pp. 195–117). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Garrity, L. I. (1977). Electromyography: A review of the current status of subvocal speech research. Memory & Cognition, 5, 615–622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1988). Mind matters: How mind and brain interact to create our conscious lives. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gillingham, K. M., & Berk, L. E. (1995, March). The role of private speech in the development of young children's focused attention during play. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Goldman-Eisler, F. (1951). The measurement of time sequences in conversational behaviour. British Journal of Psychology, 42, 355–362.Google Scholar
Goldman-Eisler, F. (1958). The predictability of words in context and the length of pauses in speech. Language and Speech, 1, 226–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goncu, A. (1993). Development of intersubjectivity in social pretend play. Human Development, 36, 185–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goudena, P. P. (1992). The problem of abbreviation and internalization of private speech. In Díaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 215–224). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gumperz, J. J. (1977). Sociocultural knowledge in conversational inference. In Saville-Troike, M. (Ed.), Linguistics and anthropology (pp. 191–212). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1984). Language as code and language as behavior: A systemic-functional interpretation of the nature and ontogenesis of dialogue. In Fawcett, R. P., Halliday, M. A. K., Lamb, S. M., & Makkai, A. (Eds.), The semiotics of culture and language: Vol. 1. Language as a social semiotic (pp. 3–35). London: Frances Pinter.Google Scholar
Hartup, W. W. (1983). Peer relations. In Mussen, P. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 103–196). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hazen, N. L., & Black, B. (1989). Preschool peer communication skills: The role of social status and interaction context. Child Development, 60, 867–876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hymes, D. H. (1962). The ethnography of speaking. In Gladwin, T. & Sturtevant, W. C. (Eds.), Anthropology and human behavior (pp. 15–53). Washington, DC: Anthropological Society of Washington.Google Scholar
Jorgensen, C., Lee, D. D., & Agabon, S. (2003). Sub auditory speech recognition based on EMG/EPG signals. Proceedings of the International Joint Conference on Neural Networks (Vol. 4, pp. 3128–3133), Portland, OR.Google Scholar
Karpov, Y. V. (2005). The neo-Vygotskian approach to child development. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, E. O. (1974). Conversational competence in children. Journal of Child Language, 1, 163–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, E. O. (1975). Again and again: The pragmatics of imitation in child language. Pragmatics Microfiche.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. O., & Klein, E. (1975). Coherency in children's discourse. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 365–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, E. O., & Shieffelin, B. (1976). Topic as a discourse notion: A study of topic in the conversations of children and adults. In Li, C. (Ed.), Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L., Yaeger, J., & Hjertholm, E. (1968). Private speech: Four studies and a review of theories. Child Development, 39, 691–736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krafft, K. C., & Berk, L. E. (1998). Private speech in two preschools: Significance of open-ended activities and make-believe play for verbal self-regulation. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13, 637–658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landy, R. J. (1993). Persona and performance: The meaning of role in drama, therapy, and everyday life. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior (Tizard, J., Trans.). New York: Liveright.Google Scholar
Matuga, J. M. (2004). Situated creative activity: The drawings and private speech of young children. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 267–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meacham, J. A., Harris, A. E., & Blaschko, T. (1973, March). Integration of verbal and motor activities. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
Meichenbaum, D., & Goodman, J. (1969). The developmental control of operant motor responding by verbal operants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 7, 553–565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moffett, J. (1968). Teaching the universe of discourse. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1989). Monologues in the crib. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 1–23). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., & Gruendel, J. M. (1979). At morning it's lunchtime: A scriptal view of children's dialogues. Discourse Processes, 2, 73–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A., & Bruner, J. (1976). The achievement and antecedents of labeling. Unpublished manuscript, Hebrew University, Jerusalem.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1999). The Alex studies: Cognitive and communicative abilities of grey parrots. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (2005). Infants' insight into the mind: How deep?Science, 308, 214–216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philips, S. U. (1972). Participant structures and communicative competence: Warm Springs children in community and classroom. In Cazden, C. B., John, V. P., & Hymes, D. (Eds.), Functions of language in the classroom (pp. 370–394). New York: Teacher's College Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1955). The language and thought of the child (M. Gabain, Trans.). New York: Meridian. (Original work published 1923)Google Scholar
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1977). Intellectual operations and their development. In Gruber, H. E. & Vonèche, J. J. (Eds.), The essential Piaget: An interpretive reference and guide (pp. 342–358). New York: Basic Books. (Original work published 1963)Google Scholar
Ramirez, J. D. (1992). The functional differentiation of social and private speech: A dialogic approach. In Díaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 199–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Rowlandson, K., Rumsey, C., & Garnham, A. (2003). How language relates to theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 18, 139–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking in conversation. Language, 50, 696–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sawyer, R. K. (1997). Pretend play as improvisation: Conversation in the preschool classroom. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scarlett, W. G., & Wolf, D. (1979). When it's only make-believe: The construction of a boundary between fantasy and reality in storytelling. In Winner, E. & Gardner, H. (Eds.), Fact, fiction, and fantasy in childhood (pp. 29–40). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Scollon, R. (1976). Conversations with a one year old. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selman, R. L. (1971). Taking another's perspective: Role-taking development in early childhood. Child Development, 42, 1721–1734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selman, R. L. (1976). Toward a structural analysis of developing interpersonal relations concepts: Research with normal and disturbed preadolescent boys. Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, 10, 156–200.Google Scholar
Sinclair, J., & Coulthard, R. (1975). Towards an analysis of discourse. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smolucha, F. (1992). Social origins of private speech in pretend play. In Díaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 123–141). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stubbs, M. (1983). Discourse analysis: The sociolinguistic analysis of natural language. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P. (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Instincts for human understanding and for cultural cooperation: Their development in infancy. In Cranach, M., Foppa, K., Lepenies, W., & Ploog, D. (Eds.), Human ethology: Claims and limits of a new discipline. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language (Hanfmann, E. & Vakar, G., Eds. and Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published 1934)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher mental processes (Cole, M., John-Steiner, V., Scribner, S., & Souberman, E., Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1930, 1933)Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. 1. Problems of general psychology (Rieber, R. W. & Carton, A. S., Eds., Minick, N., Trans.). New York: Plenum Press. (Original work published 1934)Google Scholar
Wellman, H., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wertsch, J. V. (1979). The regulation of human action and the given-new organization of private speech. In Zivin, G. (Ed.), The development of self-regulation through private speech (pp. 79–98). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wertsch, J. V. (1980). The significance of dialogue in Vygotsky's account of social, egocentric, and inner speech. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 5, 150–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winsler, A., León, J. R., Wallace, B. A., Carlton, M. P., & Willson-Quayle, A. (2003). Private speech in preschool children: Developmental stability and change, across-task consistency, and relations with classroom behavior. Journal of Child Language, 30, 583–608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zivin, G. (1979). Removing common confusions about egocentric speech, private speech, and self-regulation. In Zivin, G. (Ed.), The development of self-regulation through private speech (pp. 13–49). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×