Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T19:00:51.841Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Talking and Thinking: The Role of Speech in Social Understanding

from Part II - Language, Communication, Social Cognition, and Awareness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 July 2009

Adam Winsler
Affiliation:
George Mason University, Virginia
Charles Fernyhough
Affiliation:
University of Durham
Ignacio Montero
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
Get access

Summary

How talking and thinking or language and thought relate to one another has been discussed through generations in philosophy, linguistics, and psychology. This is once again reemerging as an important issue in social development with new evidence that language acquisition tends to be correlated with social cognitive development. The specific part played by language and the function of social interaction more generally have emerged as factors of central importance in current research on social cognitive development, or children's so-called theory of mind (Astington & Baird, 2005; Carpendale & Lewis, 2004, 2006; Symons, 2004). Here we review approaches to the relations between language and thought, and discuss our position on the role of language in social cognitive development.

We should be clear, however, that the relations described between language and thought depend on one's view of these phenomena. Even to discuss such links appears to presuppose that the two are separable and monolithic entities that can interact. From our perspective, language and thought are overall umbrella terms that include different aspects of human activity. Asking questions, making requests, persuading, apologizing, and joking are diverse examples of what makes up language. Similarly, although thought may include imagining the smell of roses, remembering to buy a bouquet for one's spouse and then calculating the cost, or attempting to figure out why a particular rose bush is not doing well in one's garden, these diverse activities seem to have little in common.

Type
Chapter

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amin, T. G., & Valsiner, J. (2004). Coordinating operative and figurative knowledge: Piaget, Vygotsky, and beyond. In Carpendale, J. I. M. & Müller, U. (Eds.), Social interaction and the development of knowledge (pp. 87–109). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Astington, J. W., & Baird, J. A. (Eds.). (2005). Why language matters for theory of mind. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relations between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1311–1320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Avis, J., & Harris, P. L. (1991). Belief-desire reasoning among Baka children: Evidence for a universal conception of mind. Child Development, 62, 460–467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bennett, M. R., & Hacker, P. M. S. (2003). Philosophical foundations of neuroscience. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Berk, L. E. (1992). Children's private speech: An overview of theory and the status of research. In Díaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 17–53). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berk, L. E., & Spuhl, S. T. (1995). Maternal interaction, private speech, and task performance in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, 145–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2001). Why children don't have to solve the frame problems: Cognitive representations are not encodings. Developmental Review, 21, 224–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2004). Why believe in beliefs?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 100–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billig, M. (1996). Arguing and thinking: A rhetorical approach to social psychology, New Edition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1999). A nonverbal false belief task: The performance of children and great apes. Child Development, 70, 381–395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canfield, J. V. (1993). The living language: Wittgenstein and the empirical study of communication. Language Sciences, 15, 165–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpendale, J. I. M. (1999). Constructivism, communication, and cooperation: Implications of Michael Chapman's “epistemic triangle.” In Sigel, I. (Ed.), The development of representational thought: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 129–146). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carpendale, J. I. M., & Lewis, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children's social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 79–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpendale, J. I. M., & Lewis, C. (2006). How children develop social understanding. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Carpendale, J. I. M., Lewis, C., Müller, U., & Racine, T. P. (2005). Constructing perspectives in the social making of minds. Interaction Studies, 6, 341–358.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2002). The cognitive functions of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 657–726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chandler, M. J., Fritz, A. S., & Hala, S. (1989). Small scale deceit: Deception as a marker of 2-, 3- and 4-year-olds' theories of mind. Child Development, 60, 1263–1277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandler, M. J., & Hala, S. (1994). The role of personal involvement in the assessment of early false belief skills. In Lewis, C. & Mitchell, P. (Eds.), Children's early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 403–425). Hove, UK: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chapman, M. (1991). The epistemic triangle: Operative and communicative components of cognitive development. In Chandler, M. & Chapman, M. (Eds.), Criteria for competence: Controversies in the conceptualization and assessment of children's abilities (pp. 209–228). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chapman, M. (1993). Everyday reasoning and the revision of belief. In Puckett, J. M. & Reese, H. W. (Eds.), Mechanisms of everyday cognition (pp. 95–113). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child Development, 70, 853–865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Villiers, J. G. (2005). Can language acquisition give children a point of view? In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. A. (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 186–219). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villiers, J. G., & Villiers, P. A. (2000). Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false beliefs. In Mitchell, P. & Riggs, K. J. (Eds.), Children's reasoning and the mind (pp. 191–228). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Villiers, J. G., & Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development, 17, 1037–1060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Díaz, R. M., & Berk, L. E. (Eds.). (1992). Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Duveen, G. (1997). Psychological development as a social process. In Smith, L., Dockrell, J., & Tomlinson, P. (Eds.), Piaget, Vygotsky and beyond: Future issues for developmental psychology and education (pp. 67–90). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Fernyhough, C. (1996). The dialogic mind: A dialogic approach to the higher mental functions. New Ideas in Psychology, 14, 47–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernyhough, C. (1997). Vygotsky's sociocultural approach: Theoretical issues and implications for current research. In Hala, S. (Ed.), The development of social cognition (pp. 65–93). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fernyhough, C. (2004). More than a context for learning? The epistemic triangle and the dialogic mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 104–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernyhough, C., & Fradley, E. (2005). Private speech on an executive task: Relations with task difficulty and task performance. Cognitive Development, 20, 103–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernyhough, C., & Meins, E. (2004, July). Self-regulatory speech as a mechanism for theory of mind acquisition. Paper presented at the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Ghent.Google Scholar
Freeman, N. H., Lewis, C., & Doherty, M. (1991). Preschoolers' grasp of a desire for knowledge in false-belief reasoning: Practical intelligence and verbal report. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 139–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacker, P. M. S. (1996). Analytical commentary on the philosophical investigations: Vol. 4. Wittgenstein: Mind and will. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hacker, P. M. S. (1997). Wittgenstein: On human nature. London: Phoenix.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (1996). Desires, beliefs, and language. In Carruthers, P. & Smith, P. K. (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 200–220). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P. L. (2005). Conversation, pretense, and theory of mind. In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. A. (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 70–83). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, H. A., Black, C., & Miller, S. A. (1996). A cross-task comparison of false belief understanding in a Head Start population. Journal of Experimental Child Development, 63, 263–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohlberg, L., Yaeger, J., & Hjertholm, E. (1968). Private speech: Four studies and a review of theories. Child Development, 39, 691–736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, C., Freeman, N., Hagestadt, C., & Douglas, H. (1994). Narrative access and production in preschoolers' false belief reasoning. Cognitive Development, 9, 397–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74, 1130–1144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lohmann, H., Tomasello, M., & Meyer, S. (2005). Linguistic communication and social understanding. In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. A. (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 245–265). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malcolm, N. (1991). Wittgenstein: The relation of language to instinctive behavior. In Hyman, J. (Ed.), Investigating psychology: Sciences of the mind after Wittgenstein (pp. 27–47). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1977). George Herbert Mead on social psychology (Strauss, A., Ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Das Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 1715–1726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, D. E. (2002). Mental verbs and semantic development. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3, 357–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., & Dunham, P. (Eds.). (1995). Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
O'Neill, D. K. (1996). Two-year-old children's sensitivity to a parent's knowledge state when making requests. Child Development, 67, 659–677CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., Zauner, P., & Sprung, M. (2005). What does “that” have to do with point of view? Conflicting desires and “want” in German. In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. A. (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 220–244). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1928). Judgment and reasoning in the child. London: Kegan. (Original work published 1924)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1959). The language and thought of the child. New York: Meridian Books. (Original work published 1923)Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: W. W. Norton & Co. (Original work published 1945)Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1995). Sociological studies. London: Routledge. (Original work published 1977)Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (2000). Commentary on Vygotsky's criticisms of Language and thought of the child and judgement and reasoning in the child (Smith, L., Trans.). New Ideas in Psychology, 18, 241–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plato, (1973). Theaetetus (McDowell, J., Trans.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1988). Representation and reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Racine, T. P., & Carpendale, J. I. M. (2007). The embodiment of mental states. In Overton, W. F., Müller, U., & Newman, J. (Eds.), Body in mind, mind in body: Developmental perspectives on embodiment and consciousness. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Reddy, M. J. (1979). The conduit metaphor – A case of frame conflict in our language about language. In Ortony, A. (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (pp. 284–324). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children's and mothers' mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 734–751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Rowlandson, K., Rumsey, C., & Garnham, A. (2003). How language relates to belief, desire, and emotion understanding. Cognitive Development, 18, 139–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid-Shönbein, C. (1990, June). Explicating some aspects of the process of reflective reconstruction. Paper presented at the meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1979). Psycholinguistics (2nd ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Scott, Foresman & Co.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (1996). With knowledge in mind: Novel transformation of the learner or transformation of novel knowledge. Human Development, 39, 257–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokolov, A. N. (1972). Inner speech and thought. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Symons, D. (2004). Mental state discourse, theory of mind, and the internalization of self-other understanding. Developmental Review, 24, 159–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1988a). Lev Vygotsky and Pierre Janet: On the origin of the concept of sociogenesis. Developmental Review, 8, 52–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1988b). On the social nature of human cognition: An analysis of the shared intellectual roots of George Herbert Mead and Lev Vygotsky. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 18, 117–136.Google Scholar
Vinden, P. G. (1996). Junín Quechua children's understanding of mind. Child Development, 67, 1701–1716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinden, P. G. (1999). Children's understanding of mind and emotion: A multi-cultural study. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 19–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published 1934)Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001) Meta-analysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittgenstein, L. (1968). Philosophical investigations. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×