Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T22:39:06.716Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Recognizing, Defining, and Representing Problems

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Jean E. Pretz
Affiliation:
Yale University
Adam J. Naples
Affiliation:
Yale University
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University
Janet E. Davidson
Affiliation:
Lewis and Clark College, Portland
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

What are the problems that you are currently trying to solve in your life? Most of us have problems that have been posed to us (e.g., assignments from our supervisors). But we also recognize problems on our own (e.g., you might have noticed the need for additional parking space in the city where you work). After identifying the existence of a problem, we must define its scope and goals. The problem of parking space is often seen as a need for more parking lots or parking garages. However, in order to solve this problem creatively, it may be useful to turn it around and redefine it as a problem of too many vehicles requiring a space in which to sit during the workday. In that case, you may be prompted to redefine the problem: You decide to organize a carpool among people who use downtown parking lots and institute a daytime local taxi service using these privately owned vehicles. Thus, you solve the problem not as you originally posed it but as you later reconceived it.

Problem solving does not usually begin with a clear statement of the problem; rather, most problems must be identified in the environment; then they must be defined and represented mentally. The focus of this chapter is on these early stages of problem solving: problem recognition, problem definition, and problem representation.

THE PROBLEM-SOLVING CYCLE

Psychologists have described the problem-solving process in terms of a cycle (Bransford & Stein, 1993; Hayes, 1989; Sternberg, 1986).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to “The social psychology of creativity.” Boulder, CO: Westview Press
Barron, F. (1969). Creative person and creative process. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660Google ScholarPubMed
Bastick, T. (1982). Intuition: How we think and act. New York: Wiley
Beale, R. J., & Jackson, T. (1990). Neural computing: An introduction. Bristol: Institute of Physics Publishing
Boden, M. (1999). Computer models of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of Creativity (pp. 351–372). New York: Cambridge University Press
Bransford, J. D., & Stein, B. S. (1993). The ideal problem solver: A guide for improving thinking, learning, and creativity (2nd ed.). New York: W. H. Freeman
Brophy, D. R. (1998). Understanding, measuring and enhancing individual creative problem solving efforts. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 123–150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chi, M., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (Eds.). (1988). The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Clement, E. & Richard, J. (1997). Knowledge of domain effects in problem representation: The case of Tower of Hanoi isomorphs. Thinking and Reasoning, 3, 133–157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, M. A., & Amabile, T. M. (1999). Motivation and creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 297–312). New York: Cambridge University Press
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity. New York: Harper Collins
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Sawyer, K. (1995). Creative insight: The social dimension of a solitary moment. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 329–363). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2001, April). An fMRI study on the conscious and unconscious evaluations of social groups. Paper presented at the UCLA Conference on Social Cognitive Neuroscience, Los Angeles, CA
Davidson, J. E. (1995). The suddenness of insight. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 125–155). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Ellis, S., & Siegler, R. S. (1994). Development of problem solving. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Thinking and problem solving. Handbook of perception and cognition (2nd ed.) (pp. 333–367). San Diego, CA: Academic PressCrossRef
Ennis, R. H. (1987). A taxonomy of critical thinking dispositions and abilities. In J. B. Baron & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Teaching thinking skills: Theory and practice. Series of books in psychology (pp. 9–26). New York: Freeman
Eysenck, H. J. (1997). Creativity and personality. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), The creativity research handbook (pp. 41–66). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton
Eysenck, H. J. & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck personality questionnaire. London: Hodder & Stoughton
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290–309CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frensch, P. A., & Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Expertise and intelligent thinking: When is it worse to know better? In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 5, pp. 157–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Getzels, J. W. (1982). The problem of the problem. In R. Hogarth (Ed.), New directions for methodology of social and behavioral science: Question framing and response consistency (No. 11). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass
Getzels, J. W., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The creative vision: A longitudinal study of problem finding in art. New York: Wiley
Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 306–355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstone, R. L., & Barsalou, L. (1998). Reuniting perception and conception. In S. A. Sloman and L. J. Rips (Eds.), Similarity and symbols in human thinking (pp. 145–176). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressCrossRef
Hayes, J. R. (1989). The complete problem solver (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Jay, E. S. (1996). The nature of problem finding in students' scientific inquiry. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Graduate School of Education, Cambridge, MA
Jay, E. S., & Perkins, D. N. (1997). Problem finding: The search for mechanism. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), The creativity research handbook (pp. 257–293). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton
Kaufman, J. C. (2001). The Sylvia Plath effect: Mental illness in eminent creative writers. Journal of Creative Behavior, 35(1), 37–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kotovsky, K., Hayes, J. R., & Simon, H. A. (1985). Why are some problems hard? Evidence from the Tower of Hanoi. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 248–294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lesgold, A. M. (1988). Problem solving. In R. J. Sternberg & E. E. Smith (Eds.), The psychology of human thought (pp. 188–213). New York: Cambridge University Press
MacKinnon, D. W. (1978). In search of human effectiveness: Identifying and developing creativity. Buffalo, NY: Creative Education Foundation
MacLeod, C. M., Hunt, E. B., & Mathews, N. N. (1978). Individual differences in the verification of sentence-picture relationships. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 493–507CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metcalfe, J. A., & Wiebe, D., (1987). Intuition in insight and noninsight problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 15, 238–246CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minsky, M., & Papert, S. (1969). Perceptrons: An introduction to computational geometry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Mumford, M. D., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Redmond, M. R. (1994). Problem construction and cognition: Applying problem representations in ill-defined domains. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 1–39). Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Newell, A., & Simon, H. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., & Lieberman, M. D. (2001). The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 56, 717–734CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reed, S. K. (1987). A structure-mapping model for word problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 13, 125–139Google Scholar
Rosenblatt, F. (1958). The perceptron: A probabilistic model for information storage and organization in the brain. Psychological Review, 65, 386–408CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & University of California San Diego, PDP Research Group. (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Seifert, C. M., Meyer, D. E., Davidson, N., Patalano, A. L., & Yaniv, I. (1995). Demystification of cognitive insight: Opportunistic assimilation and the prepared-mind perspective. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 125–155). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Siegler, R. S. (1978). The origins of scientific reasoning. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children's thinking: What develops? (pp. 109–49). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Simon, H. A., & Newell, A. (1971). Human problem solving: The state of the theory in 1970. American Psychologist, 26, 145–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. (1999). Creativity from a historiometric perspective. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 116–136). New York: Cambridge University Press
Smith, S. M. (1995). Fixation, incubation, and insight in memory and creative thinking. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 135–156). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1989). Incubation effects. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 311–314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press
Sternberg, R. J. (1986). Intelligence applied? Understanding and increasing your intellectual skills. San Diego, CA: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich
Sternberg, R. J. (1999). Cognitive psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Harcourt Brace
Sternberg, R. J., & Davidson, J. E. (Eds.) (1995). The nature of insight. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Sternberg, R. J., & Weil, E. M. (1980). An aptitude-strategy interaction in linear syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 226–234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Pretz, J. E. (2001). The propulsion model of creative contributions applied to the arts and letters. Journal of Creative Behavior, 35, 75–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Pretz, J. E. (2002). The creativity conundrum: A propulsion model of kinds of creative contributions. Philadelphia: Psychology Press
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the crowd: Cultivating creativity in a culture of conformity. New York: Free Press
Terman, L. M. (1950). Concept Mastery Test. New York: Psychological Corporation
Torrance, E. P., & Ball, O. E. (1984). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: Revised manual. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Test Service
Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York: Harcourt, Brace
Weisberg, R. W., & Alba, J. W. (1981). An examination of the alleged role of “fixation” in the solution of several “insight” problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 169–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wertheimer, M. (1945). Productive thinking. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×