Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T04:06:16.306Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Reasoning with Quantifiers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Bart Geurts
Affiliation:
University of Nijmegen
Jonathan E. Adler
Affiliation:
Brooklyn College, City University of New York
Lance J. Rips
Affiliation:
Northwestern University, Illinois
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Reasoning
Studies of Human Inference and its Foundations
, pp. 249 - 268
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barwise,, J., & Cooper, R. (1981). Generalized quantifiers and natural language. Linguistics and Philosophy, 4, 159–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1998). Steps towards a mental-predicate logic. In Braine, M. D. S. & O'Brien, D. P. (Eds.), Mental logic (pp. 273–331). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S., Reiser, B. J., & Rumain, B. (1984). Some empirical justification for a theory of natural propositional logic. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bucciarelli, M., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1999). Strategies in syllogistic reasoning. Cognitive Science, 23, 247–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chater, N., & Oaksford, M. (1999). The probability heuristics model of syllogistic reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 191–258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (Eds.), The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 163–228). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dickstein, L. S. (1978). The effect of figure on syllogistic reasoning. Memory and Cognition, 6, 76–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickstein, L. S. (1981). Conversion and possibility in syllogistic reasoning. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 229–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dowty, D. (1994). The role of negative polarity and concord marking in natural language reasoning. In Harvey, M. & Santelmann, L. (Eds.), Proceedings from semantics and linguistic theory IV (pp. 114–144). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Drozd, K. F. (2001). Children's weak interpretations of universally quantified questions. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 340–376). Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., Newstead, S. E., & Byrne, R. M. J. (1993). Human reasoning: the psychology of deduction, Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ford, M. (1995). Two modes of mental representation and problem solution in syllogistic reasoning. Cognition, 54, 1–71.Google ScholarPubMed
Geach, P. T. (1962). Reference and generality. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Geurts, B. (1999). Presuppositions and pronouns. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Geurts, B. (2001). Quantifying kids. Unpublished manuscript.
Geurts, B. (2002). Donkey business. Linguistics and Philosophy, 25, 129–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., & Hug, K. (1992). Domain-specific reasoning: social contracts, cheating and perspective change. Cognition, 43, 127–171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, L. R. (1989). A natural history of negation. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Bara, B. G. (1984). Syllogistic inference. Cognition, 16, 1–61.Google ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Laird, P. N., Byrne, R. M. J., & Tabossi, P. (1989). Reasoning by model: the case of multiple quantification. Psychological Review, 96, 658–673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Steedman, M. (1978). The psychology of syllogisms. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 64–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanazawa, M. (1994). Weak vs. strong readings of donkey sentences and monotonicity inference in a dynamic setting. Linguistics and Philosophy, 17, 109–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladusaw, W. A. (1979). Polarity sensitivity as inherent scope relations. PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX.Google Scholar
Ladusaw, W. A. (1996). Negation and polarity items. In Lappin, S. (Ed.), The handbook of contemporary semantic theory (pp. 321–341). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Newstead, S. E., & Griggs, R. A. (1983). Drawing inferences from quantified statements: a study of the square of oppositions. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 535–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (2001). The probabilistic approach to human reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 349–357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rips, L. J. (1994). The psychology of proof: deductive reasoning in human thinking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Valencia, Sánchez V. M. (1991). Studies on natural logic and categorial grammar. Doctoral dissertation, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Smith, C. L. (1979). Children's understanding of natural language hierarchies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 437–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. L. (1980). Quantifiers and question answering in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 191–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sundholm, G. (1983). Systems of deduction. In Gabbay, D. & Guenthner, F. (Eds.), Handbook of philosophical logic (pp. 133–188). Vol. 1. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, Y. (1977). Symmetry and transitivity assumptions about a nonspecified logical relation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 677–684.Google Scholar
Wetherick, N. E., & Gilhooly, K. J. (1990). Syllogistic reasoning: effects of premiss order. In Gilhooly, K. J., Keane, M. T. G., Logie, R. H. & Erdos, G. (Eds.), Lines of thinking: reflections on the psychology of thought (pp. 99–108). Vol. 1. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wilkins, M. C. (1928). The effect of changed material on ability to do formal syllogistic reasoning. Archives of Psychology, 16, 1–83.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×