Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 3
  • Cited by
    This chapter has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Chakraborty, Anya and Chakrabarti, Bhismadev 2018. Looking at My Own Face: Visual Processing Strategies in Self–Other Face Recognition. Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 9, Issue. ,

    Chakraborty, Anya and Chakrabarti, Bhismadev 2015. Is it me? Self-recognition bias across sensory modalities and its relationship to autistic traits. Molecular Autism, Vol. 6, Issue. 1,

    Meyer-Bahlburg, Heino F. L. 2010. Gender Outcome in 46,XY Complete Androgen Insensitivity Syndrome: Comment on T’Sjoen et al. (2010). Archives of Sexual Behavior, Vol. 39, Issue. 6, p. 1221.

    ×
  • Print publication year: 2003
  • Online publication date: December 2009

8 - The neural correlates of self-awareness and self-recognition

from Part II - Cognitive and neurosciences

Summary

Abstract

Attempts to elucidate the brain correlates of self-awareness have existed for centuries. However, only now is a clear picture emerging. Based on case studies and modern neuroimaging, it appears that the right hemisphere and the prefrontal cortex, probably the right prefrontal cortex, is dominant for self-related processing. Studies implicating the right hemisphere/prefrontal cortex have focused on self-recognition, self-face recognition, autobiographical episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness. We present this evidence and argue that the right hemisphere/prefrontal cortex may be dominant for the self.

Introduction

This chapter attempts to integrate a number of experimental findings pertaining to the neural correlates of the self and self-related processing. In this context, we have previously suggested that the brain be viewed as an Alexander Calder mobile, delicate and balanced, with a certain interdependence amongst the elements (Keenan, 2001). Calder's mobiles are fragile, with wire or string holding together elements typically made of metal in such a manner that perfect balance is achieved across the entire creation. These elements, which on some levels can be described as discrete units, are never wholly independent, and their actions shift as the environment changes. Destruction of a single unit often affects the entire brain, even if the change across distant regions is subtle. Further, different environmental conditions, analogous to different cognitive demands, often involve numerous elements, and the same element is often involved under numerous tasks.

Recommend this book

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this book to your organisation's collection.

The Self in Neuroscience and Psychiatry
  • Online ISBN: 9780511543708
  • Book DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511543708
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to *
×
REFERENCES
Ackerley, S. S., &Benton, A. L., (1947). Report of a case of bilateral frontal lobe defect. Research Publications: Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 27, 479–504
Breen, N., Caine, D., &Coltheart, M., (2001). Mirrored-self misidentification: two cases of focal onset dementia. Neurocase, 7, 239–54
Calabrese, P., Markowitsch, H. J., Durwen, H. F., et al. (1996). Right temporofrontal cortex as critical locus for the ecphory of old episodic memories. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 61, 304–10
Feinberg, T., &Shapiro, R., (1989). Misidentification-reduplication and the right hemisphere. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 2, 39–48
Feinberg, T. E., Haber, L. D., &Leeds, N. E., (1990). Verbal asomatognosia. Neurology, 40, 1391–4
Fink, G. R., Markowitsch, H. J., Reinkemeier, M. et al. (1996). Cerebral representation of one's own past: neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 4275–82
Freeman, W. & Watts, J. W. (1950). Psychosurgery, 2nd edn. Springfield, IL: Charles S. Thomas
Gallup, G. G. (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–7
Gallup, G. G. Jr (1977). Absence of self-recognition in a monkey (Macaca fascicularis) following prolonged exposure to a mirror. Developmental Psychobiology, 10, 281–4
Gardiner, J. M. &Java, R. I. (1990). Recollective experience in word and nonword recognition. Memory and Cognition, 18, 23–30
Howe, M. L. &Courage, M. L. (1997). The emergence and early development of autobiographical memory. Psychological Review, 104, 499–523
Ingvar, D. H. (1985). ‘Memory of the future’: an essay on the temporal organization of conscious awareness. Human Neurobiology, 4, 127–56
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J. &Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302–11
Kanwisher, N., Tong, F. &Nakayama, K. (1998). The effect of face inversion on the human fusiform face area. Cognition, 68, B1–11
Kanwisher, N., Stanley, D. &Harris, A. (1999). The fusiform face area is selective for faces not animals. Neuroreport, 10, 183–7
Keenan, J. P. (2001). A thing done well: a reply to Dr Antti Revonsuo's ‘Can functional brain imaging discover consciousness in the brain?’ Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 31–3
Keenan, J. P., McCutcheon, N. B. &Pascual-Leone, A. (2001a). Functional magnetic resonance imaging and event related potentials suggest right prefrontal activation for self-related processing. Brain and Cognition, 47, 87–91
Keenan, J. P., Nelson, A., O'Connor, M. &Pascual-Leone, A. (2001b). Self-recognition and the right hemisphere. Nature, 409, 305
Kircher, T. T., Senior, C., Phillips, M. L. et al. (2001). Recognizing one's own face. Cognition, 78, B1–15
Kitchen, A., Denton, D. &Brent, L. (1996). Self-recognition and abstraction abilities in the common chimpanzee studied with distorting mirrors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 93, 7405–8
Levine, B., Black, S. E., Cabeza, R., et al. (1998). Episodic memory and the self in a case of isolated retrograde amnesia. Brain 121 1951–73
Lewis, M. & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social Cognition and Acquisition of Self. New York, NY: Plenum
Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., Stanger, C. &Weiss, M. (1989). Self development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 60, 146–56
Luria, A. R., Pribram, K. H. &Homskaya, E. D. (1964). An experimental analysis of the behavioral disturbance produced by a left frontal arachnoidal endothelioma. Neuropsychologia, 2, 257–80
Markowitsch, H. J. (1995). Which brain regions are critically involved in the retrieval of old episodic memory? Brain Research and Brain Research Reviews, 21, 117–27
Markowitsch, H. J., Calabrese, P., Haupts, M. et al. (1993). Searching for the anatomical basis of retrograde amnesia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 947–67
Markowitsch, H. J., Calabrese, P., Neufeld, H., Gehlen, W. &Durwen, H. F. (1999). Retrograde amnesia for world knowledge and preserved memory for autobiographic events. A case report. Cortex, 35, 243–52
Meador, K. J., Loring, D. W., Feinberg, T. E., Lee, G. P. &Nichols, M. E. (2000). Anosognosia and asomatognosia during intracarotid amobarbital inactivation. Neurology, 55, 816–20
Miller, P. J., Potts, R., Fung, H. &Hoogstra, L. (1990). Narrative practices and the social construction of self in childhood. American Ethnologist, 17, 292–311
Moniz, E. (1936). Tentatives Operatoires dans le Traitement de Certaines Psychoses. Paris: Mason
Patterson, F. (1984). Self-recognition by gorilla Gorilla gorilla. Gorilla, 7, 2–3
Povinelli, D. J., Gallup, G. G. Jr, Eddy, T. J. &Bierschwale, D. T. (1997). Chimpanzees recognize themselves in mirrors. Animal Behaviour, 53, 1083–8
Preilowski, B. (1977). Self-recognition as a test of consciousness in left and right hemisphere of ‘split-brain’ patients. Acta Nervous Super (Praha), 19(suppl. 2), 343–4
Sergent, J. &Gross, C. (1992). Face recognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 2, 156–61
Sergent, J. &Poncet, M. (1990). From covert to overt recognition of faces in a prosopagnosic patient. Brain, 113, 989–1004
Shillito, D. J., Gallup, G. G. Jr, & Beck, B. B. (1999). Factors affecting mirror behaviour in western lowland gorillas, Gorilla gorilla. Animal Behaviour, 57, 999–1004
Spangenberg, K., Wagner, M. &Bachman, D. (1998). Neuropsychological analysis of a case of abrupt onset following a hypotensive crisis in a patient with vascular dementia. Neurocase, 4, 149–54
Sperry, R., Zaidel, E. &Zaidel, D. (1979). Self recognition and social awareness in the deconnected minor hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 17, 153–66
Stuss, D. & Benson, F. (1986). The frontal lobes and control of cognition and memory. In The Frontal Lobes Revisited, ed. E. Perceman, pp. 141–54. Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Suarez, S. &Gallup, G. (1981). Self-recognition in chimpanzees and orangutans, but not gorillas. Journal of Human Evolution, 10, 175–88
Suarez, S. D. &Gallup, G. G. (1986). Social responding to mirrors in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): effects of changing mirror location. American Journal of Primatology, 11, 239–44
Tulving, E. (1987). Multiple memory systems and consciousness. Human Neurobiology, 6, 67–80
Wheeleri, M. A., Stuss, D. T., &Tulving, E., (1997). Toward a theory of episodic memory: the frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 331–54