Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T01:23:49.518Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Musical Ensemble Performance

Representing Self, Other and Joint Action Outcomes

from Part III - Thinking, Perceiving and Acting with Others

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 October 2016

Sukhvinder S. Obhi
Affiliation:
McMaster University, Ontario
Emily S. Cross
Affiliation:
Bangor University
Get access

Summary

Abstract

Musical ensemble performance constitutes a refined form of joint action that involves the non-verbal communication of information about musical structure and expressive intentions via co-performers’ sounds and body movements. Successful musical communication requires co-performers to coordinate their actions across multiple musical dimensions (pitch and rhythm), timescales (expressive micro-timing versus large-scale tempo changes), sensory modalities (auditory and visual) and modes of interaction (unison versus complementary action). From a psychological perspective, ensemble performance necessitates precise yet flexible interpersonal coordination at the level of sensorimotor, cognitive, emotional and social processes. The current chapter addresses how such interpersonal coordination is facilitated by representations of shared performance goals, which are consolidated during preparation for joint musical performance. During actual performance, these shared goal representations interact with online sensorimotor and cognitive processes that allow co-performers to anticipate, attend and adapt to each other’s actions in real time. Studies employing behavioral and brain methods provide evidence for three functional characteristics of shared musical representations. First, shared representations involve the integration of information related to one’s own part, others’ parts and the joint action outcome, while maintaining a distinction between self and other. Second, self, other and joint action outcomes are represented in predictive internal models. Third, internal models recruit the motor system to simulate self- and other-produced actions at multiple hierarchical levels. Shared musical representations thus facilitate exquisite real-time interpersonal coordination by dynamically embodying intended action outcomes related to the self, others and the ensemble as a whole.

Type
Chapter
Information
Shared Representations
Sensorimotor Foundations of Social Life
, pp. 280 - 310
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bigand, E., McAdams, S., & Forêt, S. (2000). Divided attention in the listening of polyphonic music. International Journal of Psychology, 35, 270278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blank, M., & Davidson, J. W. (2007). An exploration of the effects of musical and social factors in piano duo collaborations. Psychology of Music, 35(2), 231248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bregman, A. S., & Campbell, J. (1971). Primary auditory stream segregation and perception of order in rapid sequences of tones. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 244249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaffin, R., Imreh, G., & Crawford, M. (2002). Practicing perfection: Memory and piano performance. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google ScholarPubMed
Chaffin, R., Lemieux, A. F., & Chen, C. (2007). ‘It is different each time I play’: Variability in highly prepared musical performance. Music Perception, 24(5), 455472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaffin, R., & Logan, T. (2006). Practicing perfection: How concert soloists prepare for performance. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2(2), 113130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and two ears. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 25, 975979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, E. S., Kraemer, D. J., Hamilton, A. F., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. T. (2009). Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. Cerebral Cortex, 19(2), 315326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Ausilio, A. (2007). The role of the mirror system in mapping complex sounds into actions. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(22), 58475848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, J. W. (2009). Movement and collaboration in musical performance. In Hallam, S., Cross, I., & Thaut, M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of music psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 364376.Google Scholar
Davidson, J. W., & Good, J. M. M. (2002). Social and musical co-ordination between members of a string quartet: An exploratory study. Psychology of Music, 30(2), 186201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, J. W., & King, E. C. (2004). Strategies for ensemble practice. In A. Williamon (Ed.), Enhancing musical performance. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 105–122.Google Scholar
De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. (2007). Participatory sense-making: An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6, 485507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairhurst, M. T., Janata, P., & Keller, P. E. (2014). Leading the follower: An fMRI investigation of dynamic cooperativity and leader–follower strategies in synchronization with an adaptive virtual partner. NeuroImage, 84, 688697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friberg, A., & Battel, G. U. (2002). Structural communication. In R. Parncutt & G. E. McPherson (Eds.), The science and psychology of music performance: Creative strategies for teaching and learning. New York: Oxford University Press, 116.Google Scholar
Gabrielsson, A. (1999). The performance of music. In Deutsch, D (Ed.), The psychology of music. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 501602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginsborg, J., Chaffin, R., & Nicholson, G. (2006). Shared performance cues in singing and conducting: A content analysis of talk during practice. Psychology of Music, 34, 167192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glowinski, D., et al. (2010). Multi-scale entropy analysis of dominance in social creative activities. Proceedings of the ACM Multimedia International Conference, Firenze, Italy, 10351038.Google Scholar
Glowinski, D., (2013). The movements made by performers in a skilled quartet a distinctive pattern, and then the function it serves. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goebl, W., & Palmer, C. (2009). Synchronization of timing and motion among performing musicians. Music Perception, 26(5), 427438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, E. (2002). Ensemble performance. In J. Rink (Ed.), Musical performance: A guide to understanding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 153167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haruno, M., Wolpert, D., & Kawato, M. (2001). Mosaic model for sensorimotor learning and control. Neural Computation, 13(10), 22012220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haueisen, J., & Knösche, T. R. (2001). Involuntary motor activation in pianists evoked by music perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 786792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herholz, S. C., & Zatorre, R. J. (2012). Musical training as a framework for brain plasticity: Behavior, function, and structure. Neuron, 76(3), 486502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurley, S. (2008). The shared circuits model (SCM): How control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation, deliberation, and mindreading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(1), 158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Idil, K., Engel, A., Kirschner, S., & Keysers, C. (2011). Synchronized drumming enhances activity in the caudate and facilitates prosocial commitment – if the rhythm comes easily. PloS One, 6(11), e27272.Google Scholar
Ito, M. (2008). Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 9(4), 304313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janata, P., Tillmann, B., & Bharucha, J. J. (2002). Listening to polyphonic music recruits domain-general attention and working memory circuits. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(2), 121140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14, S103S109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawato, M. (1999). Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9, 718727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, P. E. (1999). Attending in complex musical interactions: The adaptive dual role of meter. Australian Journal of Psychology, 51, 166175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, P. E. (2001). Attentional resource allocation in musical ensemble performance. Psychology of Music, 29, 2038.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, P. E. (2008). Joint action in music performance. In Morganti, F, Carassa, A, & Riva, G (Eds.), Enacting intersubjectivity: A cognitive and social perspective to the study of interactions. Amsterdam: IOS Press, 205221.Google Scholar
Keller, P. E. (2012). Mental imagery in music performance: Underlying mechanisms and potential benefits. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 206213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, P. E. (2014). Ensemble performance: Interpersonal alignment of musical expression. In Fabian, D, Timmers, R, & Schubert, E (Eds.), Expressiveness in music performance: Empirical approaches across styles and cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 260282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, P. E., & Appel, M. (2010). Individual differences, auditory imagery, and the coordination of body movements and sounds in musical ensembles. Music Perception, 28(1), 2746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, P. E., & Burnham, D. K. (2005). Musical meter in attention to multipart rhythm. Music Perception, 22, 629–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, P. E., Knoblich, G., & Repp, B. H. (2007). Pianists duet better when they play with themselves: On the possible role of action simulation in synchronization. Consciousness and cognition, 16(1), 102111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, P. E., & Repp, B. H. (2008). Multilevel coordination stability: Integrated goal representations in simultaneous intra-personal and inter-agent coordination. Acta Psychologica, 128(2), 378386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, P. E., Novembre, G., & Hove, M. J. (2014). Rhythm in joint action: Psychological and neurophysiological mechanisms for real-time interpersonal coordination. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 20130394.Google Scholar
Knoblich, G., Butterfill, S., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Psychological research on joint action. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), Advances in research and theory. London: Academic Press, 59101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Körding, K. P., & Wolpert, D. M. (2004). Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature, 427(6971), 244247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahav, A., Saltzman, E., & Schlaug, G. (2007). Action representation of sound: Audiomotor recognition network while listening to newly acquired actions. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(2), 308314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1998). Preparation of a public piano performance: The relation between practice and performance. Musicae Scientiae, 2, 6994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Loehr, J. D. (2013). Sensory attenuation for jointly produced action effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loehr, J. D., Kourtis, D., Vesper, C., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2013). Monitoring individual and joint action outcomes in duet music performance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(7), 10491061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loehr, J. D., & Palmer, C. (2011). Temporal coordination between performing musicians. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(11), 21532167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
London, J. (2012). Hearing in time: Psychological aspects of musical meter. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacRitchie, J., Buck, B., & Bailey, N. J. (2013). Inferring musical structure through bodily gestures. Musicae Scientiae, 17(1), 86108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchini, M., Papiotis, P., & Maestre, E. (2012). Timing synchronization in string quartet performance: A preliminary study. In Barthet, M. & Dixon, S. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 9th International Symposium on Computer Music Modeling and Retrieval (CMMR). London: Queen Mary University of London, 177185.Google Scholar
Müller, D., Widmann, A., & Schröger, E. (2005). Auditory streaming affects the processing of successive deviant and standard sounds. Psychophysiology, 42(6), 668676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieuwenhuis, S., Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). Decision-making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 510532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noorden, L. van. (1975). Temporal coherence in the perception of tone sequences. Eindhoven: Institute for Perception Research.Google Scholar
Novembre, G., Ticini, L. C. F., Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Keller, P. E. (2012). Distinguishing self and other in joint action: Evidence from a musical paradigm. Cerebral Cortex, 22(12), 28942903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Novembre, G., Ticini, L. C. F., Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Keller, P. E. (2014). Motor simulation and the coordination of self and other in real-time joint action. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 10621068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowicki, L., Prinz, W., Grosjean, M., Repp, B. H., & Keller, P. E. (2013). Mutual adaptive timing in interpersonal action coordination. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 23(1), 620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nusseck, M., & Wanderley, M. M. (2009). Music and motion: How music-related ancillary body movements contribute to the experience of music. Music Perception, 26(4), 335353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliveira, F. T. P., MacDonald, J. J., & Goodman, D. (2007). Performance monitoring in the anterior cingulate cortex is not all error related: Expectancy deviation and the representation of action–outcome associations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 19942004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pacherie, E. (2008). The phenomenology of action: A conceptual framework. Cognition, 107(1), 179217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pacherie, E. (2012). The phenomenology of joint action: Self-agency vs. joint-agency. In Seemann, A (Ed.), Joint attention: New developments, Vol. 93. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 343389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, C. (1997). Music performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 115138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, C., & Loehr, J. D. (2013). Meeting of two minds in duet piano performance. In Bernstein, L. F. & Rozin, A. (Eds.), Musical implications: Essays in honor of Eugene Narmour. Hillsdale, NY: Pendragon Press, 323338.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G., Candidi, M., Dindo, H., & Barca, L. (2013). Action simulation in the human brain: Twelve questions. New Ideas in Psychology, 31(3), 270290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips-Silver, J., & Keller, P. E. (2012). Searching for roots of entrainment and joint action in early musical interactions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, H. E., & Byo, J. L. (2002). Rehearsing and conducting. In Parncutt, R. & McPherson, G. E. (Eds.), The science and psychology of musical performance: Creative strategies for teaching and learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 335351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ragert, M., Fairhurst, M. T., & Keller, P. E. (2014). Segregation and integration of auditory streams when listening to multi-part music. PloS One, 9(1), e84085.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ragert, M., Schroeder, T., & Keller, P. E. (2013). Knowing too little or too much: The effects of familiarity with a co-performer’s part on interpersonal coordination in musical ensembles. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repp, B. H. (1995). Expressive timing in Schumann’s ‘Träumerei:’ An analysis of performances by graduate student pianists. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 98(5), 24132427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repp, B. H. (1997). The aesthetic quality of a quantitatively average music performance: Two preliminary experiments. Music Perception, 14(4), 419444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Repp, B. H. (1998). A microcosm of musical expression. I. Quantitative analysis of pianists’ timing in the initial measures of Chopin’s Etude in E Major. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 104(2), 10851100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repp, B. H. (2002). The embodiment of musical structure: Effects of musical context on sensorimotor synchronization with complex timing patterns. In Prinz, W & Hommel, B (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and Performance XIX. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 245265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Repp, B. H., & Keller, P. E. (2010). Self versus other in piano performance: Detectability of timing perturbations depends on personal playing style. Experimental Brain Research, 202(1), 101110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sänger, J., Lindenberger, U., & Müller, V. (2011). Interactive brains, social minds. Communicative and Integrative Biology, 4, 655663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satoh, M., Takeda, K. Nagata, K. Hatazawa, J., & Kuzuhara, S. (2001). Activated brain regions in musicians during an ensemble: A PET study. Cognitive Brain Research, 12, 101108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L., Gilbert, D. T., & Wegner, D. M. (2011). Psychology, 2nd edition. New York: Worth.Google Scholar
Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Prediction of external events with our motor system: Towards a new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 211218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schütz-Bosbach, S., Avenanti, A., Aglioti, S. M. & Haggard, P. (2009). Don’t do it! Cortical inhibition and self-attribution during action observation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(6), 12151227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schutz-Bosbach, S., Mancini, B., Aglioti, S. M., & Haggard, P. (2006). Self and other in the human motor system. Current Biology, 16(18), 18301834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 7076.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2005). How two share a task: Corepresenting stimulus–response mappings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(6), 12341246.Google Scholar
Snyder, J. S., & Alain, C. (2007). Toward a neurophysiological theory of auditory stream segregation. Psychological Bulletin, 133(5), 780799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steen, M.C. van der, & Keller, P. (2013). The ADaptation and Anticipation Model (ADAM) of sensorimotor synchronization. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 253.Google ScholarPubMed
Trusheim, W. H. (1991). Audiation and mental imagery: Implications for artistic performance. Quarterly Journal of Music Teaching and Learning, 2, 138147.Google Scholar
Uhlig, M., Fairhurst, M. T., & Keller, P. E. (2013). The importance of integration and top-down salience when listening to complex multi-part musical stimuli. NeuroImage, 77, 5261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vesper, C., Butterfill, S. Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2010). A minimal architecture for joint action. Neural Networks, 23(8–9), 9981003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, C., Tsakiris, M., Haggard, P., & Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2014). Agency in the sensorimotor system and its relation to explicit action awareness. Neuropsychologia, 52, 8292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williamon, A., & Davidson, J. W. (2002). Exploring co-performer communication. Musicae Scientiae, 6, 5372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131(3), 460473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 593602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolpert, D. M., Miall, R. C., & Kawato, M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 338347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., & Penhune, V. B. (2007). When the brain plays music: Auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience 8(7), 547558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×